Word to picture matching task

10000+ results for ‘picture and word match’

Word and Picture Match

Word and Picture Match
Match up

by Anniefair

 igh word and picture match

igh word and picture match
Find the match

by Heidipaquettecom

Word and Picture Match

Word and Picture Match
Group sort

by Alisonpangan

cool sufixes

cool sufixes
Flip tiles

by Edarst6257

G7
word match

CVC picture/word match

CVC picture/word match
Match up

by Lhoward1

English

CVC Word-Picture Match

CVC Word-Picture Match
Match up

by Sarobinson

 blends and digraphs word picture match

blends and digraphs word picture match
Find the match

by Cdavis3

K
G1
G2
G3
phonics
Reading

Match the word and picture

Match the word and picture
Match up

by Msknutson

Match the word and picture

Match the word and picture
Open the box

by Kfilipe

Digraph Word and Picture Match

Digraph Word and Picture Match
Group sort

by Ssays

SH and CH Word and Picture Match

SH and CH Word and Picture Match
Matching pairs

by Mitchellm

CVC digraph picture and word match

CVC digraph picture and word match
Find the match

by Ctoti

G1
Reading

Word & picture match

Word & picture match
Labelled diagram

by Rvangund

Writing

Wilson's Welded Sounds (Word and Picture Match)

Wilson’s Welded Sounds (Word and Picture Match)
Matching pairs

by Duleyj

Word and Picture Match - sion and -tion

Word and Picture Match — sion and -tion
Match up

by Teach2020

Picture and Word Match ou and ow

Picture and Word Match ou and ow
Find the match

by Rhondablake

CVCe picture word match

CVCe picture word match
Match up

by Huddle

 ur- match the word and picture

ur- match the word and picture
Match up

by Lluckmann

Soft c and g - Word / Picture Match

Soft c and g — Word / Picture Match
Match up

by Teach2020

7.9 AR and ER Word/Picture match

7.9 AR and ER Word/Picture match
Match up

by Amybaylosis1

Short i picture and word match

Short i picture and word match
Find the match

by Mrsvasquez09

aw and au Word to Picture Match

aw and au Word to Picture Match
Match up

by Kalcor

Reading

Match the picture and the word

Match the picture and the word
Match up

by Squimi

ai and ay picture/word match

ai and ay picture/word match
Match up

by Shrawdeh

Blends and digraphs word picture match

Blends and digraphs word picture match
Find the match

by Johnnancym

 Tu B'ishvat match the word and picture

Tu B’ishvat match the word and picture
Find the match

by Igottlieb

CVC word/picture match

CVC word/picture match
Quiz

by Jotomberg

G1
Reading

 Wilson's Welded Sounds (Word and Picture Match)

Wilson’s Welded Sounds (Word and Picture Match)
Matching pairs

by Nadia407

Reading

Short /u/ Picture and Word Match

Short /u/ Picture and Word Match
Matching pairs

by Hbentz99

-sk and -sp word picture match

-sk and -sp word picture match
Matching pairs

by Lkwdenison

OG

Match the word and the picture

Match the word and the picture
Quiz

by Jotomberg

G2
Reading

OG Lesson 17 - Match CVC and CCVC Word and Picture

OG Lesson 17 — Match CVC and CCVC Word and Picture
Match up

by Mknoerlein

Letterland Unit 17: Picture and Word Match

Letterland Unit 17: Picture and Word Match
Maze chase

by Lswain

Match the oi oy word to picture

Match the oi oy word to picture
Matching pairs

by Cwalsh2

G1
G2
Phonemic Awareness
Phonics
Reading
trick words
Word Families
compound words

Letter Match w/ Picture and Word

Letter Match w/ Picture and Word
Categorize

by Cerobinson

Short /a/ Picture and Word Card Match

Short /a/ Picture and Word Card Match
Matching pairs

by Cindiloh7

Digraph Picture Word Match

Digraph Picture Word Match
Match up

by Huddle

Reading Comprehension - Match word to picture

Reading Comprehension — Match word to picture
Quiz

by Eac51038

English
Reading

 6.5 Word - Picture Match #1

6.5 Word — Picture Match #1
Quiz

by Carollykins

FLOSS Word Picture Match

FLOSS Word Picture Match
Match up

by Bluecottagetuto

K
G1
English

6.5 Word-Picture Match

6.5 Word-Picture Match
Find the match

by Carollykins

6.5 Word-Picture Match #2

6.5 Word-Picture Match #2
Quiz

by Carollykins

Soft C Sounds

Soft C Sounds
True or false

by Maureen6

Phonics and Word Work

match the word with the correct picture -at -an -and

match the word with the correct picture -at -an -and
Match up

by Rheavner

Match Picture to Word

Match Picture to Word
Matching pairs

by Juliebates

K

Picture-Word Match

Picture-Word Match
Quiz

by Amyhowell17

G2

Fundations Word-Picture Match U2W1

Fundations Word-Picture Match U2W1
Match up

by Paronek

Hard c and soft c word/picture match

Hard c and soft c word/picture match
Matching pairs

by Jotomberg

G2
Orton Gillingham
Reading
Recipe for Reading

Sight Words (always, were, once, have)

Sight Words (always, were, once, have)
Missing word

by Maureen6

Phonics and Word Work

CVC word/picture match

CVC word/picture match
Match up

by Jmitchell14

Dylan's word/picture match

Dylan’s word/picture match
Match up

by Tedeco

word picture match

word picture match
Match up

by Charlenem

K
G1
Reading

Match Word to Picture

Match Word to Picture
Match up

by Esti

Word to Picture Match

Word to Picture Match
Match up

by Elhammon

-ung picture word match

-ung picture word match
Match up

by Msarroyopc

-tion word / picture match

-tion word / picture match
Match up

by Teach2020

match word to picture

match word to picture
Match up

by Rachelmk35

VCe picture/word match

VCe picture/word match
Match up

by Cscurachio87

2.2 Word-Picture Match

2.2 Word-Picture Match
Match up

by Meredith20

Wilson

Digraph picture / word match

Digraph picture / word match
Match up

by Cdavis3

K
G1
G2
G3
phonics
Reading

I have a measurement task box to share with you for FREE today!

If you don’t have an independent work station… you need to start one right now! Here’s where to start. Building my independent work library was life changing for me! I was understaffed and had a classroom full of students who needed 1:1. Teaching my students how to work independently not only freed up some staff…. but taught them life long skills (independence, schedule following, clean up, attention) that they will carry with them throughout life.

A measurement task box
A measurement task box

This month I added a measurement matching task box to the Simply Free library. I didn’t have many math or science tasks so I am focusing on adding lots in those categories in the near future!

Match the word to picture task box for measurement

Match the word to the picture card

This task box comes with 2 levels. Matching picture to picture and matching picture to word!

A completed measurement matching activity

A completed measurement matching task box.

The exposure to these measurement words makes this the perfect independent work task!

If you haven’t started building your library, I create a few free downloads a month and add them to my Simply Free Library to help you get started. To join the Simply Free Library and get your password, sign up for me email list here.

To join the Simply Free Library, you just need to sign up for my email list! You will get the password and have instant access to over 70 free resources, 50+ are independent work tasks!

If you are already on the email list, you can log in here, the password is at the bottom of every email I send.

JOIN THE SIMPLY FREE LIBRARY TODAY!

Worksheets

Powerpoints

Video Lessons

Search

Filters

SORT BY

Most popular

TIME PERIOD

All-time

JanayeRose

JanayeRose

Picture to Word

732 uses

Auphelia

Auphelia

Veterinarian word find & picture matching

512 uses

sumeyyeogultekin

sumeyyeogultekin

Special Days

479 uses

yamatki039

yamatki039

Coronavirus Prevention ESL Printable Picture Dictionary Worksheet For Kids

375 uses

Lollybags

Lollybags

Match the word with the picture

355 uses

LoubeeSav

LoubeeSav

Word and picture matching - 'at' words

332 uses

maktub

maktub

Mix Memory Picture Cards

309 uses

Meryevergreen

Meryevergreen

MATCH WORD/PICTURE SCHOOL MATERIAL

262 uses

AStringer01

AStringer01

'Can' for beginners

202 uses

LoubeeSav

LoubeeSav

Word and picture matching - 'an' words

192 uses

LoubeeSav

LoubeeSav

St. Patrick's Day - Word and Picture Matching

166 uses

jodielj33

jodielj33

Match the word with the picture

144 uses

Next

3

Blog

FAQ

About us

Terms of use

Your Copyright

Summer Word to Picture Match is an activity that develops vocabulary, reading and spelling skills. It is good for early age, kindergarten, preschool, ESL students and kids with autism and special needs.

The following words are included: sun, pool, beach, sand, bucket, sandcastle, hat, sunglasses, flip flops, ball, lake, tent, picnic, swimsuit, jellyfish, ice cream, flowers, campfire, bike, lemonade, watermelon, butterfly, sunflower, suitcase, shovel, barbecue, seashells, toys, crab.

The pages must be printed and laminated. Use some velcro in the middle of the blanc squares on the first 10 pages. Cut the cards with words out from the last 5 pages and use velcro on the back side.

Given a picture related to a topic or theme, STUDENT will choose the correct response card by reading the word with 80% accuracy, in 4 out of 5 opportunities, by MONTH, YEAR.

This item is recommended for the following grade levels:

PreK, Kindergarten, 1st Grade

Write a review

Your Name:

Your Review:

Note: HTML is not translated!

Rating: Bad 

 

 

 

 

 Good

Captcha

I’m starting to publish my collection of ESL teaching materials related to clothes and accessories. These are printable worksheets, flashcards and games that English teachers can use for their classes like I do.

The kit includes flashcards, 4 types of worksheets, dominoes and bingo game

I had already published a song about clothes and decided I should expand my offering for ESL teachers with more printables on the topic.

Watch the Clothing Song video on my Youtube channel

My ESL clothes kit includes materials that help English teachers introduce the vocabulary related to clothing & accessories and make it easier for students to memorize the words. The featured vocabulary comprises the following words: apron, backpack, bathrobe, belt, bikini, blouse, boot, bow tie, bra, bracelet, button, cap, cardigan, coat, dress, earring, flip-flops, glasses, gloves, handbag, handkerchief, hat, high heels, hoodie, jacket, jeans, jersey, mittens, necklace, night gown, overalls, pajamas (UK: pyjamas), panties, pants (UK: trousers), polo shirt, purse, raincoat (UK: Mackintosh), ring, sandals, scarf, shirt, shoes, shorts, skirt, slippers, sneakers (UK: trainers), socks, stockings, suit, sunglasses, sweater (UK: jumper), swimsuit, tank top, tie, T-shirt, umbrella, underpants, underwear, vest, wallet, wristwatch, zipper (UK: zip).

Clothes and accessories vocabulary for ESL students in pictures
Clothes and accessories vocabulary featured in my ESL kit

Traditionally, I designed four types of printable worksheets: word to picture matching, picture to word matching, crossword puzzles and word search puzzles. I decided the first blog post dedicated to the clothes and accessories topic should feature the word to picture matching worksheets. So here they are:

Printable clothes worksheets for English classes



Clothes and accessories worksheet

Clothes and accessories matching worksheet

ESL clothes worksheet

Printable clothes and accessories worksheet

Free clothes and accessories worksheet for ESL students

Clothes and accessories vocabulary matching worksheet

Clothes and accessories ESL printable exercise worksheet

Introduction

Accessing words (lexical representations) and meanings (semantic representations) from the same vs. different categories can interfere with future access from the category. For example, patients with aphasia due to stroke tend to make semantic errors when naming pictures and/or matching words to pictures in the context of semantically related words (e.g., Schnur et al., 2006; Biegler et al., 2008; Harvey and Schnur, 2015). Moreover, naming pictures (e.g., Kroll and Stewart, 1994) or matching words to pictures (Campanella and Shallice, 2011) belonging to the same semantic category has a detrimental effect on healthy participants’ performance, known as semantic interference. That both picture naming and word-picture matching performance is sensitive to semantic contexts demonstrates that both tasks are semantically mediated (see Belke, 2013). However, because semantic interference in picture naming and word-picture matching tasks is usually investigated separately, this has led to different conclusions about the locus of interference in each task. In picture naming, evidence suggests that interference arises when mapping from semantic to lexical representations (hereafter, lexical locus; Levelt et al., 1999; Howard et al., 2006; Oppenheim et al., 2010), whereas in word-picture matching tasks evidence suggests that interference arises within the semantic system itself (Warrington and McCarthy, 1983, 1987; Warrington and Cipolotti, 1996; Forde and Humphreys, 1997, 2007; Gotts and Plaut, 2002; Campanella and Shallice, 2011). While semantic interference in picture naming tasks has been largely explored in healthy subjects, semantic interference in word-picture matching tasks is less often reported in the healthy population (cf. Biegler et al., 2008; Campanella and Shallice, 2011; Wei and Schnur, 2016). Here, we investigated the locus of semantic interference in naming and word-picture matching by testing in healthy participants whether interference was sensitive to semantic and lexical factors and transferred between the two tasks. Finding that interference is affected by the same factors and/or transfers across the two tasks can elucidate the extent to which processes governing access to semantic and lexical representations operate similarly across the two tasks. In turn, this work informs theories of lexical-semantic access, providing clues about the organization of the language system as a whole.

In both picture naming and word-picture matching tasks, repeatedly accessing semantically related stimuli has a negative effect on performance. For example, participants are slower to name pictures or match words to pictures when trials depict items belonging to the same categories (related context: e.g., CAT, DOG, BEAR, and COW) vs. different categories (unrelated context: e.g., CAT, TRAIN, SHIRT, and DESK)1 (i.e., blocked naming and word-picture matching tasks; e.g., Damian et al., 2001; Damian and Als, 2005; Campanella and Shallice, 2011). Interference is thought to occur because activating the semantic system to produce a target word (i.e., “dog”) or access a word’s meaning (i.e., DOG) results in the co-activation of related words and meanings (e.g., “cat” and CAT) due to the high degree of semantic feature overlap amongst members of the same category (e.g., Collins and Loftus, 1975; see also Vigliocco et al., 2002; Forde and Humphreys, 2007). This is evidenced by the findings of graded semantic interference effects in both tasks (i.e., larger interference for semantically close vs. distant category members; naming: Vigliocco et al., 2002; Navarrete et al., 2012; word-picture matching: Crutch and Warrington, 2005, Experiment 1; Warrington and Cipolotti, 1996, Experiment 5). That naming and word-picture matching are sensitive to semantic contexts demonstrates that interference in both tasks originates at the semantic level.

However, the locus of semantic interference in each task is thought to differ. By most accounts, semantic interference in naming exerts its effects at the lexical level (e.g., Howard et al., 2006; Oppenheim et al., 2010; see also Damian and Als, 2005; cf. Levelt et al., 1999; Damian et al., 2001), whereas semantic interference in word-picture matching exerts its effects at the semantic level (e.g., Forde and Humphreys, 1997, 2007; Campanella and Shallice, 2011). Computational models of semantic interference in naming (Howard et al., 2006; Oppenheim et al., 2010; see also Roelofs, 1992) assume that naming a picture (i.e., DOG) activates its lexical representation (i.e., “dog”) and those sharing semantic features with the target (e.g., “cat”) to a greater extent than those that do not share semantic features with the target (e.g., “shoe”). Producing the word “dog” increases its lexical representation’s activation level, which negatively affects the subsequent selection of same-category lexical representations (e.g., “cat”). Accordingly, theories of semantic interference in naming (e.g., Howard et al., 2006; Oppenheim et al., 2010) assume that shared activation at the semantic level causes interference that exerts its effects at a lexical level. Theories of semantic interference in word-picture matching assume that activating the meaning of a given word (i.e., “dog”) also activates related word meanings (e.g., CAT), which interfere with the ability to distinguish between same-category meanings on subsequent trials (Forde and Humphreys, 1997, 2007; see also Warrington and Cipolotti, 1996; Gotts and Plaut, 2002). Thus, semantic interference in naming originates at the semantic level, but has a lexical-level locus (see also Belke, 2013), whereas semantic interference in word-picture matching both originates and has its locus at the semantic level.

That semantic contexts are thought to interfere with word-picture matching performance at a semantic level seemingly contradicts the generally accepted view that semantic contexts facilitate performance on tasks requiring semantic but not lexical access for spoken output (Bajo, 1988; Belke, 2013). For example, semantic relationships facilitate the recognition of words preceded by a semantically related prime word (i.e., lexical decision task; e.g., McRae and Boisvert, 1998) and the categorization of pictured objects based on the direction (i.e., left or right) they face (i.e., orientation judgment task; Damian et al., 2001)2 or based on their superordinate category (i.e., man-made or natural) membership (i.e., semantic classification task; Belke, 2013; see also Vitkovitch and Humphreys, 1991). However, tasks argued to tap semantic level facilitatory processes differ in a number of respects with those eliciting semantic interference. In the lexical decision task, “…co-activation of other words would not be costly because the task only requires participants to decide whether the presented string is a word or not…” (Vigliocco et al., 2004, p. 468) but not whether the word refers to a specific meaning. Further, judging the orientation of a pictured object (i.e., tip of a shoe) in terms of which direction it faces relies more on decoding the visual properties of the object (e.g., Humphreys et al., 1988, 1995) and not necessarily the semantic features corresponding to the object (see Belke, 2013). Lastly, the semantic classification task while likely requiring access to semantic information, does not necessitate accessing fine-grained semantic level distinguishing information, as all members of a semantic category are consistent with the classification of man-made or natural. By contrast, matching a word to its corresponding picture necessitates making fine-grained semantic decisions about the set of semantic features associated with that particular word, which in some ways is like naming a picture, as picture naming necessitates the selection of a word based on the set of semantic features that distinguish the target lexical representation from co-activated, semantically related, lexical representations (see Wei and Schnur, 2016 for a similar discussion). Thus, the assumption that semantic contexts facilitate processing at the semantic level may be an artifact of the types of tasks used to tap semantic-level processes (see Chen and Mirman, 2012 for a similar argument).

Does semantic interference occur in the healthy semantic system when discriminating a target from related meanings? Evidence of semantic interference in word-picture matching almost exclusively comes from neuropsychological studies of patients with aphasia secondary to stroke (cf. Biegler et al., 2008; Campanella and Shallice, 2011; Wei and Schnur, 2016). Consequently, the extent to which the healthy semantic system operates similarly when accessing words and meanings is not well understood. To our knowledge, only a few studies have investigated semantic interference in healthy younger adults’ word-picture matching performance, demonstrating that semantic interference occurs in tasks tapping semantic-level processes (Campanella and Shallice, 2011; Mirman and Graziano, 2012; Wei and Schnur, 2016; see Biegler et al., 2008 for evidence of semantic interference in healthy older adults’ word-picture matching performance). What remains unclear is whether the semantic context effects observed in word-picture matching occur due to the same processes that create interference in naming.

The Current Research

The main goal of this research was to investigate whether semantic interference in naming and word-picture matching originate and/or exert their effects at a shared processing level(s). Accordingly, we explored how factors that tap semantic and lexical processing affect semantic interference in each task, and whether semantic interference at a shared processing level(s) allows for the effect to transfer across tasks.

Here, we used cyclical variants of the blocked naming and word-picture matching tasks, where subjects name pictures or match words to pictures in related vs. unrelated contexts, and target items repeat multiple times (cycles) in different orders (e.g., Kroll and Stewart, 1994; Damian, et al., 2001; Campanella and Shallice, 2011; see also Wei and Schnur, 2016). Whether assuming a lexical- or semantic-level locus, interference is thought to emerge with repetition because competition increases with repeated access to same- vs. different-category items (e.g., Forde and Humphreys, 1995, 2007; Belke et al., 2005b; cf. Navarrete et al., 2014 for an alternative account in naming). We hypothesize that because both picture naming and word-picture matching tasks require mapping between shared lexical and semantic representational levels (see Figure 1; reviewed in Howard, 1995; Levelt, 1999; cf. Caramazza, 1997), it suggests a shared origin and/or locus of interference in the two tasks. Specifically, in both tasks it is necessary to access the semantic features corresponding to the target representation (picture or word form) – a process that results in the co-activation of related representations. However, because the order with which lexical and semantic representations are activated occurs in reverse in the two tasks (semantic-to-lexical in naming and vice versa in word-picture matching), the level at which co-activated representations interfere with performance is thought to differ. Consequently, it remains an open question as to whether semantic interference in the two tasks is a reflection of the same underlying phenomena occurring at shared semantic and/or lexical representational levels.

www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. The basic structure of the language system. It is hypothesized that naming and matching words to pictures require access to shared semantic and lexical level representations, but the output vs. input levels of representation differ across the tasks (cf. Caramazza, 1997; Levelt et al., 1999).

Origin of Interference

Because interference is assumed to originate at the semantic level in both picture naming and word-picture matching, it is predicted to generalize to novel category members (Forde and Humphreys, 1995; Belke et al., 2005b) and transfer across tasks (Belke, 2013). In naming, shared activation at the semantic level gives rise to the co-activation of related lexical representations (Howard et al., 2006; Oppenheim et al., 2010), resulting in the accumulation of semantic interference for both previously named and novel category members (e.g., Belke et al., 2005b). Similarly, in word-picture matching, accessing semantically related word meanings in succession renders disambiguating both previously accessed and novel word meanings belonging to the same category more difficult (e.g., Forde and Humphreys, 1995, 2007). Moreover, a shared semantic-level origin of interference in naming and word-picture matching predicts that interference will transfer across tasks. For example, if accessing the semantic system in word-picture matching results in the co-activation of both related semantic and lexical representations, then this should interfere with subsequent naming of novel same-category pictures. Thus, interference originating at the semantic level predicts both generalization of interference within each task, and transfer of interference across tasks.

To our knowledge, there are only two studies that have demonstrated interference generalization using the blocked-cyclic paradigms: one in healthy subjects’ picture naming performance (Belke et al., 2005b, Experiment 3) and the other in an aphasic patient’s comprehension performance (patient J.M.; Forde and Humphreys, 1995, Experiment 12). Belke et al. (2005b) examined whether interference generalized to cycles of naming novel items semantically related to those named previously. They found that semantic interference emerged after the first cycle and remained unchanged across subsequent cycles of both previously named and novel pictures. Forde and Humphreys (1995) found that in comprehension (i.e., auditory-to-written word matching), the interference effect was larger for the first cycle of novel words relative to the first cycle of semantically related “old” words. Thus, generalization of semantic interference has been quantified in two different ways. Belke et al. (2005b) defined generalization as an unchanging effect (once it emerges after cycle 1) across old and novel items (cycles 2–8), whereas Forde and Humphreys defined generalization as larger interference for the first cycle of novel compared to the first cycle of old items. In the experiments reported here, we assessed generalization as an increase in interference for novel compared to old items collapsed across cycles because both characterizations (i.e., no interference at cycle 1 followed by unchanging interference for cycles 2–8, Belke et al., 2005b and larger interference for the first cycle of novel vs. the first cycle of old items, Forde and Humphreys, 1995) should when averaged across cycles yield larger interference for novel vs. old items. The first goal of this study was to replicate and extend the findings of interference generalization obtained in previous studies using blocked-cyclic naming and word-picture matching tasks to demonstrate that interference in both tasks originates at the semantic level.

Locus of Interference

Because theories of semantic interference in naming and word-picture matching tasks assume different semantic interference loci (e.g., Forde and Humphreys, 2007; Oppenheim et al., 2010), this generates the prediction that lexical frequency, a factor thought to exert its effects primarily at the name retrieval stage (e.g., Vitkovitch and Humphreys, 1991),3 should affect semantic interference in naming but not word-picture matching as word-picture matching does not require access to lexical representations for spoken output (see Campanella and Shallice, 2011 for discussion). That subjects name pictures depicting high-frequency words faster than those depicting low-frequency words (e.g., Oldfield and Wingfield, 1965) and recognize high- vs. low-frequency words faster in word recognition tasks such as the lexical decision task (e.g., Scarborough et al., 1977; Balota and Chumbley, 1985) indicates that high- vs. low-frequency lexical representations have increased activation levels (Morton, 1969; McClelland and Rumelhart, 1981; Dell, 1986; Seidenberg and McClelland, 1989; Plaut et al., 1996; Caramazza, 1997; Barry et al., 2001; Kittredge et al., 2008), rendering them more available for selection in naming and identification in word recognition. Thus, a lexical, but not semantic, locus of interference predicts that interference will be affected by the lexical frequency of semantically related words when naming pictures, but not when matching words to pictures.

Although lexical frequency is predicted to interact with semantic interference in naming but not word-picture matching, previous studies investigating these factors provide equivocal results. To our knowledge, there is only one study that examined the effect of lexical frequency on response times (RTs) in blocked-cyclic naming and although there was an overall effect of lexical frequency on naming, it did not interact with semantic interference (Santesteban et al., 2006). Campanella and Shallice (2011) manipulated semantic context (close vs. distant) and lexical frequency (high- vs. low-frequency) in a non-cyclical word-picture matching task, and found that healthy subjects were slower and less accurate in the semantically close, low-frequency condition compared to all other conditions (i.e., semantically close, high-frequency, semantically distant high-frequency, and semantically distant, low-frequency; Experiment 1). Experiment 2 used a cyclical variant of the task, testing only those items that gave rise to the largest interference effects in Experiment 1 (i.e., semantically close, low-frequency words), and found that interference increased across cycles of repeated word-picture matching. That semantic interference was numerically larger for low- compared to high-frequency words (Experiment 1) contradicts a semantic locus of interference, suggesting instead that interference in word-picture matching has a lexical locus. Thus, the second aim of this study was to test whether lexical frequency interacts with semantic interference in naming and/or word-picture matching to determine whether or not the locus of interference is shared across the two tasks.

Lastly, it remains an open question whether or not semantic interference observed in picture naming and word-picture matching arises due to the same or partially overlapping processing stages. To our knowledge, no one has tested whether semantic interference transfers across the two tasks. However, previous work has examined interference transfer to and from different levels of the language system, but the evidence here is mixed. While Navarrete et al. (2010) found that interference transferred from a task tapping semantically mediated lexical retrieval (i.e., picture + determiner naming) to one requiring lexical retrieval without semantic mediation (i.e., word + determiner naming) but not vice versa (Experiment 3), Belke (2013) did not replicate this finding (Experiment 4). Moreover, Belke (2013) demonstrated that picture naming affected subsequent semantic classification (i.e., man-made or natural) of categorically related objects but not vice versa (Experiment 5), which conflicts with previous evidence that semantic classification affects the subsequent naming of categorically related pictures (Vitkovitch and Humphreys, 1991, Experiment 2). Consequently, the extent to which interference transfers across tasks tapping shared representational levels remains unclear. Thus, the third goal of this study was to investigate whether a shared origin and/or locus of interference exists, as evidenced by increased semantic interference (and thus transfer) when performing the naming (or word-picture matching) task on novel items categorically related to those which appeared previously in the word-picture matching (or naming) task. If the origin and locus of interference is shared in naming and word-picture matching, then interference will be sensitive to both semantic and lexical factors within each task (Experiments 1 and 2) and transfer across tasks (Experiment 3). However, if interference has a shared origin but different loci, then interference will generalize within tasks and transfer across tasks, but only inference in naming will interact with lexical frequency.

Materials and Methods

Participants

There were 94 participants total. Thirty-one participated in Experiment 1 [15 female, 16 male; mean (and range) age: 19 years (18–21)], 20 participated in Experiment 2 [12 female, 8 male; mean (and range) age: 19 years (18–22)], and 43 in Experiment 3 [25 female, 18 male; mean (and range) age: 19 years (18–22)]. Data from four participants who took part in Experiment 1 were excluded: two due to experimenter error and two due to equipment error. All were native English speakers with normal or corrected to normal vision attending Rice University, and received course credit for their participation. Informed consent in accordance with the IRB at Rice University was obtained from each participant.

Materials and Design

Stimuli were 64 colored pictures of familiar objects belonging to eight semantic categories. Pictures were taken from the Bank of Standardized Stimuli (BOSS; Brodeur et al., 2010) and another image database (Viggiano et al., 2004), and scaled to 400 pixels × 400 pixels. Within each category, pictures consisted of all high- or low-frequency names, and were selected to minimize differences in other factors known to correlate with lexical frequency, such as familiarity and imageability (e.g., Morrison et al., 1997). Measures of lexical frequency, familiarity, and imageability of target stimuli were obtained from an online database4 (see also Wilson, 1988). Half of the categories depicted objects with high-frequency names (mean 59.72; range 41–86), whereas the other half depicted objects with low-frequency names (mean 8.75; range 5–15; see Appendix A). Lexical frequency differed significantly for high- and low-frequency categories [t(62) = 5.02, p < 0.00001], even after controlling for indices of imageability and familiarity [F(1, 60) = 20.44, p < 0.001]. Picture names were either mono- or disyllabic, and the number of syllables did not differ between high- and low-frequency categories [t(62) = 1.59, p = 0.12]. In Experiments 2 and 3, stimuli also included visually presented written word forms of the 64 target picture names.

Items in each of the eight semantic categories appeared together to form four high- and four low-frequency related blocks of trials consisting of eight items each. One item from each of the high- or low-frequency related categories appeared together in a set to form four high- and low-frequency unrelated blocks of trials, resulting in a total of 16 blocked sets (see Appendix B). Each block consisted of a set of four pictures that repeated for four cycles in different orders (i.e., Old) followed by four cycles repeating the remaining four pictures in the set (i.e., Novel). For example, in the Related Condition, the Old set contained four same-category pictures (e.g., animal: BEAR, CAT, LION, and SHEEP), and the Novel set contained four novel pictures drawn from the same semantic category (e.g., animal: DOG, COW, RABBIT, and HORSE). The 8-item unrelated sets contained two exemplars from each of the four high- or low-frequency semantic categories, where one appeared in the Old Block Half (e.g., BEAR, CAR, SHOE, and CHAIR) and another appeared in the Novel Block Half (e.g., DOG, VAN, SHIRT, and RUG; see Figure 2). Stimuli appeared an equal number of times in each condition. Blocked sets appeared in pseudorandom order, such that no more than three blocks of the same Condition (Related and Unrelated) or Frequency (High and Low) appeared consecutively. Following these constraints, we created five stimulus presentation lists. Items appearing in the Old vs. Novel sets were counterbalanced across participants to ensure that any differences between semantic interference effects in the Old and Novel Block Halves were not due to the specific items used. This resulted in a total of 10 lists of test materials. Together, there were 16 blocks with 32 trials each for a total of 512 trials per subject.

www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Illustration of the blocked-cyclic naming (top panel) and word-picture matching (bottom panel) tasks. Pictures appeared in sets of semantically related and unrelated contexts. Four items repeated (i.e., Old; left panel) for four cycles (i.e., Cycles 1–4) followed by the repetition of four novel items (i.e., Novel; right panel). Experiment 3 followed a similar design except that participants first performed the blocked-cyclic naming (or word-picture matching) for the Old Block Half and then performed the blocked-cyclic word-picture matching (or naming) for the Novel Block Half. Subjects in Experiment 3 performed the naming task (i.e., Cycles 1–4) followed by the word-picture matching task (i.e., Cycles 5–8) for half of the blocks, and performed the tasks in the reverse order for the other half of the blocks.

Apparatus

Target stimuli were presented using DMDX software (Forster and Forster, 2003). To record naming performance (i.e., Experiments 1 and 3), a microphone headset triggered a voice key to collect RTs to the nearest millisecond (ms) and record verbal responses. An experimenter coded naming errors. To record word-picture matching performance (i.e., Experiments 2 and 3), participants made their response using a touch screen monitor, and DMDX software recorded RTs to the nearest ms and error data (i.e., tapping the wrong picture).

Experiment 1: Semantic Interference in Picture Naming

To establish that semantic interference in blocked-cyclic naming originates at the semantic level but has it locus at the lexical level (e.g., Belke, 2013), we tested whether (1) repeatedly naming a set of semantically related pictures renders subsequent naming of novel pictures drawn from the same semantic category more difficult (i.e., generalization of semantic interference), and (2) whether the lexical frequency of targets affects semantic interference magnitudes. To test the prediction that interference generalizes to novel category members (Howard et al., 2006; Oppenheim et al., 2010; see also Belke et al., 2005b), in Experiment 1 we examined whether semantic interference increased when naming novel items (collapsed across cycles) in comparison to having previously named different items from the same category. Subjects named sets of semantically related and unrelated pictures across four cycles (i.e., Old) immediately followed by naming novel semantically related or unrelated pictures for an additional four cycles (i.e., Novel; following Belke et al., 2005b). While Belke et al. (2005b) found that interference emerged on the second cycle and remained stable thereafter, a computational simulation of this experiment predicts that semantic interference increases incrementally (linearly) across cycles of previously named and novel category members (Oppenheim et al., 2010, Simulation 3). In either case, larger semantic interference for novel compared to old items (collapsed across cycles) is predicted to occur because either interference is absent on the first cycle of the block and present on later cycles (Belke et al., 2005b) or because interference continues to increase linearly across cycles of both old and novel items (Oppenheim et al., 2010). Thus, although we predict increased interference for novel items categorically related to those named previously (collapsed across cycles), it remains unclear how semantic interference in naming generalizes to novel category members in the blocked-cyclic naming task (i.e., stable vs. linear increase), as there is limited evidence to support either account of interference generalization in this task.

To investigate the contribution of lexical-level processing on semantic interference in naming and word-picture matching, we compared semantic interference for high- vs. low-frequency picture names. The semantically related and unrelated sets consisted of objects depicting words with similar frequency counts. This was done to replicate the word-picture matching findings of Campanella and Shallice (2011), and directly compare semantic interference in word-picture matching with that of naming. If semantic interference in naming has a lexical-level locus, we predict greater interference for low- vs. high-frequency picture names because their inherently lower activation levels render them more susceptible to interference from co-activated, same-category high-frequency words with inherently higher activation levels.

Procedure

Prior to the experiment, subjects were familiarized with the picture stimuli and their corresponding names. In the learning phase, each picture appeared centrally on the computer screen with its written name displayed underneath the picture. The picture and name stayed on the screen until the subject pressed a key indicating that they understood the correct response for the stimulus. To keep the learning phase consistent across experiments, subjects were instructed to not name the pictures, as naming the pictures could contaminate semantic interference effects observed in the word-picture matching variants of the task (Experiments 2 and 3).

Immediately after the learning phase, the experimental phase began. A single picture appeared in the center of the screen, and subjects were instructed to name the picture as quickly and accurately as possible into the microphone headset. If the microphone failed to trigger the voice key, then the subject would see the words “Speak up” before the next picture appeared which indicated that they should speak more loudly on the next trial. The picture remained on the screen for 1600 ms or until the subject made a response (similar to previous studies using the blocked-cyclic tasks; e.g., Damian and Als, 2005; Campanella and Shallice, 2011). Once a response was made, the next trial began immediately [i.e., 0 ms response stimulus interval (RSI), following Campanella and Shallice, 2011]. Subsequent trials either depicted same category (Related Condition) or different category items (Unrelated Condition). See Figure 2. The experiment lasted approximately 20 min.

Statistical Analyses

We excluded from the analyses RTs for trials classified as an error (i.e., incorrect naming response or no response and voice-key malfunction) and responses faster than 250 ms or slower than 1550 ms (following Damian and Als, 2005). Valid RTs were analyzed using a repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with participants and items as random factors, yielding F1 and F2 statistics, respectively. Fixed factors included Condition (Related and Unrelated), Block Half (Old, Novel), Cycles (1–4), and Frequency (High-frequency and Low-frequency). All fixed factors were considered within-subject, within-item variables except for Frequency, which was a within-subject variable in the F1 analysis and a between-item variable in the F2 analysis.

Results and Discussion

Response errors occurred on 4.4% of experimental trials. Tables 1 and 2 summarize RT F statistics and mean RTs, respectively.

www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Experiment 1 ANOVA results.

www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Experiment 1 naming latencies.

There were significant main effects of Condition, Block Half, Cycle, and Frequency. Participants responded more slowly in the Related (685 ms) compared to the Unrelated Condition [670 ms; Condition effect 15 ms, 95% confidence interval (CI) 7–23 ms]. RTs were faster in the Old (669 ms) vs. Novel Block Half (687 ms; Block Half effect 18 ms, 95% CI 12–24 ms). Participants also became faster across naming cycles (755, 667, 654, and 636 ms), replicating previous findings of repetition priming in studies using the blocked-cyclic naming task (e.g., Schnur et al., 2006; Navarrete et al., 2012, 2014). Lastly, naming latencies were faster for high- (666 ms) compared to low-frequency words (690 ms; Frequency effect 24 ms, 95% CI 15–33 ms), which replicates the lexical frequency effect found elsewhere (e.g., Oldfield and Wingfield, 1965; Jescheniak and Levelt, 1994; Griffin and Bock, 1998) and demonstrates the items were sensitive to this variable.5

Two-way interactions were significant between Condition and Block Half and Cycle. The Condition × Cycle interaction revealed that the semantic interference effect (Related – Unrelated) increased with repetition across cycles (collapsed across Block Half; -5, 9, 27, and 27 ms). The Condition × Block Half interaction revealed that semantic interference increased when naming novel (21 ms) vs. old pictures (8 ms; Condition × Block Half effect 13 ms, 95% CI 1–25 ms), indicating that semantic interference in the blocked-cyclic naming task generalizes to novel category items not previously named (Belke et al., 2005b; see Figure 3B). We assessed whether generalization of semantic interference manifested as a linear increase across cycles of old and novel items (Oppenheim et al., 2010, Simulation 3) vs. emerging on the second cycle and remaining stable thereafter (Belke et al., 2005b, Experiment 3). While interference increased linearly when all eight cycles are included in the analyses [F1(1,30) = 21.18, p < 0.001; F2(1,63) = 23.64, p < 0.001], the linear contrast is not significant when the first cycle is excluded from the analyses (p’s > 0.11), suggesting that interference emerges after Cycle 1 and remains stable thereafter (see Belke et al., 2005b). We also conducted analyses including Cycles 2–5 following the prediction put forth in Belke et al. (2005b, p. 683): “If the semantic blocking effect generalizes to new items, the difference between homogeneous and heterogeneous sets that we expected to observe in cycles 2–4 should prevail on the 5th cycle.” Consistent with Belke et al. (2005b) we find a significant main effect of Condition [F1(1,30) = 12.15, p = 0.002; F2(1,63) = 11.43, p = 0.001] and Cycle [F1(3,90) = 222.35, p < 0.001; F2(3,189) = 199.10, p < 0.001], but no interaction between the two variables (F’s < 1.73, p’s > 0.16). Consistent with generalization of interference as defined in Forde and Humphreys (1995, Experiment 12), we also find larger semantic interference on the first cycle of novel items (i.e., Cycle 5) vs. the first cycle of old items (i.e., Cycle 1) [Related – Unrelated 12 ms vs. -22 ms, respectively; F1(1,30) = 16.01, p = 0.002; F2(1,63) = 12.74, p = 0.001].

www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Experiment 1: Blocked-cyclic naming. (A) Mean response times (RTs) and associated within-subjects 95% confidence intervals (CIs) across cycles of naming semantically related (blue) and unrelated (red) pictures. Cycles 1–4 correspond to items named in the Old Block Half, whereas Cycles 5–8 correspond to items named in the Novel Block Half. (B) The magnitude of semantic interference (Related – Unrelated) and associated within-subjects 95% CIs collapsed across cycles separated by Old and Novel Block Halves. (C) The magnitude of semantic interference (Related – Unrelated) and associated within-subjects 95% CIs collapsed across cycles and separated by Old and Novel Block Halves for High- and Low-frequency categories. An * indicates a significant effect at p < 0.05.

Analyses examining Frequency revealed marginally significant interactions between this variable and Cycle and Condition – but a significant three-way interaction between Frequency, Condition, and Block Half. The Frequency × Cycle marginal interaction was due to a reduction in the lexical frequency effect (High- < Low-frequency) with repetition (Low-frequency – High-frequency difference: 39, 12, 23, and 20 ms), which is consistent with previous studies (e.g., Scarborough et al., 1977; Jescheniak and Levelt, 1994; Griffin and Bock, 1998). The marginal interaction between Frequency and Condition indicated that low- compared to high-frequency items exhibited greater semantic interference (Related – Unrelated; 23 vs. 6 ms, respectively; Condition × Frequency effect 17 ms, 95% CI 1–32 ms). The Frequency × Cycle and Frequency × Condition interactions were significant by subject and marginally significant by item (Frequency × Cycle: p = 0.07; Frequency × Condition: p = 0.09), which may be because Frequency is a between-item variable in the F2 analyses, and the significant three-way interaction between Frequency, Condition, and Block Half. That is, semantic interference for low-frequency words exceeded that of high-frequency words in the Old Block Half [22 ms vs. -6 ms, respectively; F1(1,30) = 7.92, p < 0.001; F2(1,62) = 7.53, p < 0.001], but did not differ from high-frequency words in the Novel Block Half [24 ms vs. 19 ms, respectively; F1(1,30) = 0.35, p = 0.56; F2(1,62) = 0.23, p = 0.63; see Figure 3C).6

To summarize, the results from Experiment 1 are consistent with the assumption that semantic interference in naming originates at the semantic level but has its locus at the lexical level (e.g., Belke, 2013). That semantic interference increased when naming novel items categorically related to those named previously (collapsed across cycles) demonstrates that interference originates at the semantic level as a result of shared activation. However, inconsistent with computational models of semantic interference in naming (e.g., Oppenheim et al., 2010; see also Howard et al., 2006) is the finding that the effect does not increase linearly across cycles of old and novel items. Instead, we replicate evidence elsewhere demonstrating that interference emerges on the second cycle and remains stable thereafter (Belke et al., 2005b), resulting in larger interference for the first cycle of novel vs. the first cycle of old items (Forde and Humphreys, 1995). This suggests that the larger interference effect observed for Novel relative to Old Block Halves (collapsed across cycles) reflects in part the presence of facilitation on the first cycle of old items vs. its absence on the first cycle of the novel items.

In addition, lexical frequency modulated semantic interference in naming, providing evidence of a lexical-level locus. However, the three-way interaction between lexical frequency, semantic interference, and Block Half (Old vs. Novel) was surprising, as it might be expected that lexical frequency would have a consistent impact on interference across both old and novel items. We discuss this unexpected finding in the General Discussion, as it has implications for different mechanistic accounts of how semantic interference in naming occurs, i.e., due to competitive lexical selection (e.g., Roelofs, 1992; Levelt et al., 1999; Howard et al., 2006) vs. competitive learning (Oppenheim et al., 2010). Nonetheless, the results from Experiment 1 support a semantic origin and lexical locus of the interference effect in naming.

Experiment 2: Semantic Interference in Word-Picture Matching

Although the locus of interference is thought to differ in the two tasks – lexical in naming (e.g., Howard et al., 2006; Oppenheim et al., 2010) and semantic in word-picture matching (e.g., Forde and Humphreys, 1997; Campanella and Shallice, 2011) – interference is assumed to originate at a shared semantic level. Thus, interference in word-picture matching is predicted to generalize to novel category items. Moreover, qualitative RT patterns suggest that semantic interference increases for low- relative to high-frequency words in word-picture matching (Campanella and Shallice, 2011), raising the possibility that interference in naming and word-picture matching have a shared origin and locus. We test this hypothesis in Experiment 2 using a word-picture matching variant of the blocked-cyclic naming task used in Experiment 1. In this task, subjects matched a visually presented word to its corresponding picture which appeared embedded in an array of three distractor pictures either semantically related or unrelated to the target picture (e.g., Biegler et al., 2008, Experiment 2B; Harvey and Schnur, 2015). If the locus of interference is shared across the two tasks, then semantic interference in word-picture matching will exhibit the same characteristics as those observed in picture naming (Experiment 1): increased semantic interference for novel compared to old categorically related words (i.e., generalization of semantic interference) and greater interference for low- compared to high-frequency words for old vs. novel pictures (i.e., an interaction between Condition, Frequency, and Block Half). Alternatively, if semantic interference in word-picture matching has its origin and locus within the semantic system (e.g., Forde and Humphreys, 1997, 2007; Campanella and Shallice, 2011), then it is predicted to generalize to novel category members, but not interact with lexical frequency.

Procedure

Subjects first completed the learning phase identical to that of Experiment 1. Immediately thereafter, subjects completed a practice phase, which followed the same parameters as the actual experiment but used items not depicted in the experiment (i.e., Old: BEE, ORANGE, SCISSORS, and DOLFIN; Novel: PLANT, GRAPES, PEN, and SHARK). This was done to demonstrate the speed with which the target words appear in the blocked-cyclic word-picture matching task. The procedure was as follows. A visually presented word appeared in the center of the screen for 300 ms followed by an array of four pictures: one corresponding to the previous target word and three distractor pictures. Subjects were instructed to select the picture that matches the previously presented word by tapping the picture on the touch screen monitor. Distractor pictures depicted words appearing as other targets in the cycle, and therefore either belonged to the same semantic category as the target (Related Condition) or belonged to different semantic categories as the target (Unrelated Condition). See Figure 2. All other experiment parameters were identical to Experiment 1.

Statistical Analyses

Response times analyses did not include trials classified as an error (i.e., selecting an incorrect picture) or responses faster than 250 ms or slower than 1550 ms. Valid RTs were analyzed using the same repeated measures ANOVAs as those used in Experiment 1: Random factors included participants and items, yielding F1 and F2 statistics, respectively. Fixed factors included Condition (Related and Unrelated), Block Half (Old and Novel), Cycles (1–4), and Frequency (High-frequency and Low-frequency). All fixed factors were considered within-subject, within-item variables except for Frequency, which was a within-subject variable in the F1 analysis and a between-item variable in the F2 analysis.

Results and Discussion

Response errors occurred on 1.1% of experimental trials. Tables 3 and 4 summarize RT F statistics and mean RTs, respectively. See Figure 3A for the full pattern of results.

www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 3. Experiment 2 ANOVA results.

www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 4. Experiment 2 word-picture matching response latencies.

There was a main effect of Condition, due to slower RTs in the Related (676 ms) compared to the Unrelated Condition (598 ms; Condition effect 79 ms, 95% CI 63–94 ms). However, in contrast to the naming results obtained in Experiment 1, main effects of Block Half, Frequency, and Cycle were not significant. Lastly, two- and three-way interactions between Condition, Block Half, and Frequency were not significant (see Figure 4). As in Experiment 1, we examined interference generalization in word-picture matching following Belke et al. (2005b; i.e., stable interference across Cycles 2–5) and Forde and Humphreys (1995; i.e., larger Cycle 5 vs. Cycle 1 interference). We found that although interference remained stable across Cycles 2–5 (i.e., main effect of Condition [F1(1,19) = 74.62, p < 0.001; F2(1,63) = 33.17, p < 0.001], but no interaction with Cycle (F’s < 1.19, p’s > 0.31)), interference did not differ on the first cycle of old vs. the first cycle of novel items (Related – Unrelated 86 ms vs. 83 ms, respectively; F’s < 0.07, p’s > 0.80). Together, these findings demonstrate that although semantic interference occurs in healthy participants’ word-picture matching performance, it does not manifest in the same manner as that which occurs in picture naming (i.e., Experiment 1).

www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 4. Experiment 2: Blocked-cyclic word-picture matching. (A) Mean RTs and associated within-subjects 95% CIs across cycles of matching words to pictures in the related (blue) vs. unrelated (red) condition. Cycles 1–4 correspond to items that appeared in the Old Block Half, whereas Cycles 5–8 correspond to items that appeared in the Novel Block Half. (B) The magnitude of semantic interference (Related – Unrelated) and associated within-subjects 95% CIs collapsed across cycles separated by Old and Novel Block Halves. (C) The magnitude of semantic interference (Related – Unrelated) and associated within-subjects 95% CIs collapsed across cycles and separated by High- and Low-frequency categories.

The findings from Experiment 2 are only partially consistent with the assumption that the origin and locus of semantic interference in word-picture matching is at the semantic level (e.g., Warrington and Cipolotti, 1996; Forde and Humphreys, 2007) for two reasons. First, interference did not increase for novel relative to old category items (i.e., no generalization of interference) – a result at odds with a previous neuropsychological finding suggesting a semantic-level locus (Forde and Humphreys, 1995, Experiment 12). It is possible that the visual similarity between target and distractor pictures in related vs. unrelated picture arrays contaminates the interference effect, masking potential changes in interference across cycles. Belke et al. (2005b) suggested that in naming, the absence of interference on the first cycle argues against a visual (similarity) locus of the effect, as interference due to visual similarity should be largest at the first presentation of items (i.e., Cycle 1) and reduced on subsequent presentations. We had participants from Experiment 2 rate the visual similarity of target pictures appearing together in an array after completing the word-picture matching task to assess the contribution of visual similarity on word-picture matching RTs. Two pictures appeared side-by-side, and participants rated the visually similarity of the two pictures on a 5-point scale (1 = not at all visually similar, 5 = very highly visually similar). Items appearing in the related vs. unrelated picture arrays were rated as more visually similar [t1(19) = 9.46, p < 0.001; t2(63) = 14.41, p < 0.001]. An analysis of covariance revealed that the condition effect remained significant after controlling for visual similarity in the analysis by subject [F1(1,18) = 15.35, p = 0.001], but not by item [F2(1,62) = 2.20, p = 0.14], suggesting that for some of the picture arrays visual similarity contributed to the interference effect.

Second, consistent with a non-lexical locus of interference (e.g., Forde and Humphreys, 1997, 2007), interference was not sensitive to lexical frequency, a hypothesized lexical, not semantic effect. This is in contrast to Campanella and Shallice (2011) who found that lexical frequency had some effect on semantic interference in word-picture matching. While it is not entirely clear why we did not replicate Campanella and Shallice, we hypothesize it may be due to a difference between experiment designs. Campanella and Shallice (Experiment 1) included a baseline (unrelated) condition but the items in this baseline condition differed from those tested in the experimental (related) condition (and cycle was not manipulated as a factor). In contrast, in the present study, items served as their own controls by appearing in both the related and unrelated conditions (allowing us to draw stronger conclusions concerning the effects of relatedness across items; see also Biegler et al., 2008; Harvey and Schnur, 2015; Wei and Schnur, 2016). Thus, it may be the case that their findings were driven by item-specific differences between related and unrelated conditions.

Experiment 3: Transfer of Semantic Interference

Experiments 1 and 2 demonstrated that the locus of interference in naming and word-picture matching tasks differs, but leave open the possibility that semantic interference originates at a shared processing level. Experiment 3 tested this hypothesis by investigating whether accessing the semantic system when performing the word-picture matching task with semantically related vs. unrelated words subsequently impacts naming novel pictures drawn from the same vs. different semantic categories.7 If the origin of semantic interference in the two tasks is not shared, then semantic interference when naming before (i.e., Old) and after word-picture matching (i.e., Novel) will be of comparable magnitudes, i.e., unaffected by interference in word-picture matching. However, if interference in word-picture matching and naming originate at a shared level, then semantic interference should transfer across tasks, whereby naming novel pictures semantically related to those accessed previously in word-picture matching should result in greater semantic interference than when naming precedes word-picture matching (i.e., novel > old same-category naming).

Procedure

The procedure followed that of the previous experiments except in Experiment 3 subjects either named pictures or performed word-picture matching in the first Block Half (i.e., Old) and switched tasks for the second Block Half (i.e., Novel) within both related and unrelated blocks of trials (see Figure 2). For a given subject, half of the blocks began with word-picture matching (i.e., Cycles 1–4) followed by naming (i.e., Cycles 5–8), whereas the other half began with naming (i.e., Cycles 1–4) followed by word-picture matching (i.e., Cycles 5–8). While we did not expect to find changes when switching from naming to word-picture matching (see footnote 6), we included this manipulation to determine if the semantic interference magnitude for naming increased in the Novel Block Half (i.e., after word-picture matching) relative to naming in the Old Block Half (i.e., before word-picture matching), where a larger magnitude when switching to naming would indicate interference transferred from word-picture matching to picture naming. Subjects always switched tasks halfway through the block (i.e., when presented with cycles of novel stimuli) an equal number of times throughout the experiment. The order of the task switch (from naming to word-picture matching and vice versa) was constrained so that no more than three consecutive blocks occurred with the same task switching direction. Further, there were an equal number of Related and Unrelated as well as High- and Low-frequency blocks for each task switching direction. We created an additional 10 lists of test materials by counterbalancing across subjects items appearing in each Block Half for a given task (i.e., blocked-cyclic naming or word-picture matching), resulting in a total of 20 lists.

As in Experiment 2, participants completed a practice phase with the same stimuli used in Experiment 2 to familiarize them with not only the fast presentation rate, but also the task-switching procedure. Because the task-switching was somewhat unpredictable, subjects were given a cue (i.e., #####) when the task switched from word-picture matching to naming, and the visually presented target word served as a cue when switching from naming to word-picture matching. Thus, participants always practiced the word-picture matching task before the picture naming task in order to familiarize them with the switch cue. When a single picture appeared on the screen, subjects were instructed to name the picture into the microphone headset as quickly and accurately as possible. When a word appeared followed by an array of pictures, subjects were instructed to select the picture corresponding to the previously presented word by tapping the picture on the touch screen monitor. All other experiment parameters were identical to Experiments 1 and 2.

Statistical Analyses

Response times for trials were excluded in the same manner as Experiments 1 and 2. Valid RTs were analyzed using repeated measures ANOVAs with participants and items as random factors, yielding F1 and F2 statistics, respectively. Fixed factors included: Task (Blocked-cyclic naming and Blocked-cyclic word-picture matching), Condition (Related and Unrelated), Block Half (Old vs. Novel), and Cycles (1–4). All fixed factors were considered within-subject, within-item variables.

Results and Discussion

Response errors occurred on 3.9% of experimental trials. Tables 5 and 6 summarize RT F statistics and mean RTs, respectively.

www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 5. Experiment 3 ANOVA results.

www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 6. Experiment 3 response latencies.

There were main effects of Task, Condition, and Cycle. The effect of Task revealed that RTs were faster for blocked-cyclic naming (666 ms) compared to word-picture matching (707 ms; Task effect 41 ms, 95% CI 15–67 ms). As expected, the main effect of Condition was due to slower RTs in the Related (713 ms) vs. Unrelated Condition (660 ms; Condition effect 53 ms, 95% CI 47–60 ms). The Cycle effect was due to decreasing response latencies from the first (751 ms) to the remaining three cycles (669, 662, and 665 ms; see Figures 5A,B).

www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 5. Experimen 3: Blocked-cyclic naming and word-picture matching. Mean RTs and associated within-subjects 95% CIs across cycles of naming pictures and matching words to pictures in the semantically related (blue) and unrelated (red) conditions. (A) Cycles 1–4 correspond to items named in the Old Block Half prior to performing the word-picture matching task, whereas Cycles 5–8 correspond to items named in the Novel Block Half after having performed the word-picture matching task with semantically related words in the Old Block Half. (B) Cycles 1–4 correspond to items that appeared in the word-picture matching task in the Old Block Half prior to naming pictures, whereas Cycles 5–8 correspond to items that appeared in the word-picture matching task in the Novel Block Half after having named semantically related pictures in the Old Block Half. (C) The magnitude of semantic interference (Related – Unrelated) and associated within-subjects 95% CIs collapsed across cycles and separated by Old and Novel Block Halves for blocked-cyclic naming and word-picture matching. Here, “Old” bars illustrate the semantic interference effect observed before switching tasks, whereas “Novel” refers to the semantic interference effect observed after switching tasks. An * indicates a significant effect at p < 0.05.

The interaction between Task and Cycle revealed that RTs decreased with repetition to a greater extent in blocked-cyclic naming (749, 645, 638, and 629 ms) than word-picture matching (753, 691, 684, and 700 ms), and Block Half also interacted with Cycle, but not in a meaningful way. The interaction between Task and Condition was due to smaller semantic interference (Related – Unrelated) in naming (9 ms) than word-picture matching (96 ms; Task × Condition difference 87 ms, 95% CI 73–101 ms). The significant three-way interaction between Task, Condition, and Block Half was due to larger semantic interference when naming in the Novel Block Half (i.e., after word-picture matching) compared to naming in the Old Block Half (20 ms vs. -2 ms, respectively). The increase in naming-induced semantic interference for Novel vs. Old Block Halves was confirmed with a simple effects comparison [Condition × Block Half effect 22 ms, 95% CI 7–35 ms; t1(42) = -3.01, p < 0.01; t2(63) = -2.69, p < 0.01]. That interference in naming was greater following word-picture matching suggests that semantic interference transferred from word-picture matching to picture naming (see Figure 5C).

We were surprised, however, to find that interference in the Old Block Half of naming was numerically smaller in Experiment 3 (-2 ms) compared to Experiment 1 (8 ms). We assessed post hoc whether interference in naming across the two experiments was similar in magnitude (thus similar in terms of “generalization”) with repeated measures ANOVAs that included Experiment (Experiments 1 and 3) as a between-subjects, within-item variable. The results mirrored the main findings of interference in naming reported above (i.e., main effects of Condition, Related > Unrelated), Cycle (RTs decreased across cycles), and interactions between Condition and Block Half (Novel > Old) and Condition and Cycle (semantic interference increased across cycles; F’s > 10.35, p’s < 0.002). Critically, the factor Experiment did not modulate any interactions with Condition (F’s < 1.45, p’s > 0.23). The increase in semantic interference from old to novel same-category naming was the same regardless of the task performed in the Old Block Half (i.e., 21 ms after naming in Experiment 1 and 20 ms after word-picture matching in Experiment 3). Likewise, analyses of interference across Cycles 2–5 (i.e., Belke et al., 2005b) and comparisons between interference on Cycle 1 vs. 5 (Forde and Humphreys, 1995) were consistent with the findings of Experiment 1 [i.e., main effects of Condition and Cycle (F’s > 9.75, p’s < 0.005), but no Condition × Cycle interaction (F’s < 2.17, p’s > 0.09), and greater Cycle 5 vs. Cycle 1 interference (Related – Unrelated = 12 ms vs. -21 ms, respectively; F’s > 12.95, p’s < 0.001)], where here too Experiment did not modulate interactions with Condition (F’s < 1.90, p’s > 0.17). This suggests that interference in naming and word-picture matching originate at a shared (semantic) level of the language system (see Belke, 2013 for a similar rationale).

General Discussion

To bridge the gap between theories of lexical-semantic access in naming vs. word-picture matching tasks (e.g., Forde and Humphreys, 1997, 2007; Levelt et al., 1999), we examined whether the origin and/or locus of semantic interference is shared across the two tasks. Accordingly, we tested the extent to which interference generalized to novel category items and interacted with lexical frequency in picture naming and word-picture matching variants of the blocked-cyclic paradigm, while also assessing whether interference transferred across the two tasks. In line with a semantically mediated lexical locus of interference in naming (cf. Belke, 2013), Experiment 1 demonstrated that semantic interference increased when naming novel pictures drawn from the same category as those named previously where the effect differed based on the lexical frequency of target items. Experiment 2 demonstrated that although interference occurs in word-picture matching, it did not change in magnitude for old vs. novel items or for high- vs. low-frequency words. Lastly, Experiment 3 revealed that semantic interference increased when naming novel items categorically related to those accessed previously in word-picture matching (as compared to when naming preceded word-picture matching). Together, these experiments suggest that the locus of semantic interference in picture naming and word-picture matching differs (lexical vs. semantic), but that both interference effects originate at a shared (semantic) level. In the following, we discuss how these findings inform existing theories of lexical-semantic access and semantic interference phenomena in the blocked-cyclic paradigms.

Semantic Interference in Naming

That semantic interference in naming generalized to novel category pictures and differed based on lexical frequency is consistent with a semantically mediated lexical locus of the effect. However, in order to account for the full pattern of results, additional assumptions must be adopted. For example, Oppenheim et al. (2010) predicts linearly increasing semantic interference across cycles of old and novel items (Simulation 3). However, this prediction was not confirmed, as semantic interference remains stable from when it emerged on the second cycle to the introduction of novel category items (seen here in Experiment 1 and in Belke et al., 2005b, Experiment 3). Although there may be increasing semantic interference across cycles in the blocked-cyclic naming task, according to Belke (2008) and Belke and Stielow (2013), this task promotes the use of top-down cognitive control processes which masks the accumulation of semantic interference across cycles by biasing activation toward within-set category representations and away from set-external category members (i.e., biased selection account; see also Thompson-Schill and Botvinick, 2006; Belke, 2013; Crowther and Martin, 2014). Thus, top-down control may reduce, if not eliminate, semantic interference on the first cycle of novel items (i.e., Cycle 5), providing an explanation for why the Oppenheim et al. (2010) Simulation 3 does not fully capture the lack of interference change across cycles.

Although we find that interference in naming interacted with lexical frequency – consistent with a lexical-level locus (Roelofs, 1992; Levelt et al., 1999; Howard et al., 2006; Oppenheim et al., 2010), the finding that lexical frequency differentially impacted interference for old vs. novel items was not expected. Models that assume interference occurs due to competitive lexical selection (e.g., Roelofs, 1992; Levelt et al., 1999; Howard et al., 2006) predict greater interference for both old and novel low-frequency words due to their inherently lower activation levels, which makes them more susceptible to competition from related high-frequency words. However, a recent account proposes that interference arises due to a learning mechanism that strengthens target lexical-semantic connections while weakening those of related representations (i.e., competitive learning; Oppenheim et al., 2010). Lexical-semantic connections change in magnitude (strengthen or weaken) in proportion to their activation levels, or error in becoming active on a given trial (i.e., delta rule learning; e.g., Chang et al., 2000; Gupta and Cohen, 2002). Naming the same low- vs. high-frequency words (i.e., old items) results in greater semantic interference because low-frequency words have a greater learning potential due to their inherently lower activation levels (e.g., Oldfield and Wingfield, 1965; Morton, 1969). However, novel high-frequency words will be more active than novel low-frequency words due to their inherently higher activation levels, and thus greater “unlearning” potential. In turn, novel high- compared to low-frequency related words should undergo greater lexical-semantic connection weight weakening, rendering them functionally similar to low-frequency words. Thus, the competitive learning account provides a potential explanation as to why the interaction between frequency and semantic interference differed for old vs. novel items. Although this account can only be verified by computational modeling, on the face of it, the competitive learning account explains both the finding of increased interference for low-frequency words and the interaction between this characteristic and naming old vs. novel items. However, this extension of Oppenheim et al.’s (2010) account would also predict repetition (across cycles) modulates the observed interactions between lexical frequency and semantic interference – a prediction that was not borne out by the results. That both generalization of semantic interference and lexical frequency were not sensitive to repetition (i.e., cycle) suggests that there may be other factors at play in the blocked-cyclic naming task. Future work is needed to clarify the different mechanisms underlying semantic interference when repeatedly naming the same and novel categorically related high- vs. low-frequency words in the blocked-cyclic naming task.

Semantic Interference in Word-Picture Matching

Semantic interference in the word-picture matching task differed from that observed in picture naming, suggesting that the locus of interference differs from naming. Although we observed semantic interference in word-picture matching (replicating previous results, e.g., Biegler et al., 2008; Campanella and Shallice, 2011), interference did not increase from old to novel categorically related words, which is inconsistent with a semantic-level origin and/or locus of the effect. However, there are several other explanations to consider. First, as discussed in Experiment 2, because interference did not change across cycles but was of the same magnitude from the first cycle onward, this makes it impossible to detect any generalization (or change) from old to novel items. Other variants of the word-picture matching task may be more sensitive to change of interference and thus may provide better tools to investigate generalization (cf. Wei and Schnur, 2016). Our findings suggest that the high degree of visual similarity among related vs. unrelated picture arrays had some effect on the magnitude of interference in word-picture matching (see Results and Discussion in Experiment 2). However, consistent with a semantic-, and not lexical-level locus of interference (e.g., Warrington and Cipolotti, 1996; Gotts and Plaut, 2002; Forde and Humphreys, 2007), lexical frequency did not affect semantic interference magnitudes in word-picture matching. On the assumption that lexical frequency reflects word-level processing, this factor should only affect the time it takes to recognize the target word (e.g., Scarborough et al., 1977), and should not affect the time it takes to select the word’s depicted referent. This is because the word is presented first for a fixed amount of time, and differences in RTs for each condition reflect differences in the time it takes to select the picture rather than recognizing the word itself. Nonetheless, the failure to detect lexical frequency semantic interference effects in the word-picture matching task is consistent with a semantic-level locus of interference, but future work is needed to determine if a semantic locus of interference allows for generalization to novel word meanings using a task that minimizes potential confounds such as visual similarity among distractors pictures in the related vs. unrelated arrays.

Transfer of Semantic Interference across Tasks

Lastly, the transfer of semantic interference from word-picture matching to naming suggests overlap in where interference originates in the two tasks. Because the naming results suggest a semantic origin and lexical locus of interference, whereas the word-picture matching results suggest a non-lexical locus of interference, together this suggests that the common origin of interference across the tasks is a semantic one. However, what is the evidence to rule out a lexical locus? First, lexical frequency did not interact with semantic interference in word-picture matching suggesting a non-lexical locus (Experiment 2). Second, although participants may have tacitly named the targets and pictures in the array (cf. Biegler et al., 2008), allowing semantic interference to seemingly transfer across tasks, there is evidence which argues against this possibility. For example, it is unlikely that subjects covertly named the four pictures in the array before responding because in Experiment 3, subjects named one picture on average within 667 ms, whereas average RTs for the word-picture matching task were 708 ms. Additionally, if subjects named the targets during the word-picture matching task, then semantic interference should have manifested as it did for naming (i.e., interacted with block half and lexical frequency). Previous work also demonstrates that semantic interference occurs in word-picture matching tasks that do not promote a silent naming strategy (by requiring subjects to select the picture most associated with the word), regardless of whether the distractor pictures in the array are related (Biegler et al., 2008, Experiment 3) or unrelated to the target picture (Wei and Schnur, 2016). That semantic interference occurs when matching associatively related words and pictures but does not occur when naming associatively related pictures (unless participants are primed with the scene/event name characterizing the associative relationship; Abdel Rahman and Melinger, 2011; cf. de Zubicaray et al., 2014), suggests that interference in word-picture matching is not due to silent naming. Third, models of semantic interference in naming (Howard et al., 2006; Oppenheim et al., 2010) assume that interference occurs only after having previously named from the category, and therefore do not predict that accessing the semantic system in word-picture matching leads to interference when subsequently naming novel category pictures. Thus, the transfer of semantic interference from word-picture matching to naming indicates a shared semantic-level origin of the effect. However, because interference did not change in magnitude when word-picture matching was tested alone (Experiment 2), we were unable to examine interference transfer from naming to word-picture matching. Consequently, future work is needed to better understand the mechanisms by which semantic interference arises in word-picture matching and those that allow for interference to transfer across tasks.

Conclusion

We examined whether the origin and/or locus of semantic interference in picture naming and word-picture matching is shared. We found that interference in naming generalized to novel category members and interacted with lexical frequency, a pattern that supports a semantic origin and lexical locus of interference in naming. In word-picture matching, however, the evidence for a semantic-level origin and locus of interference was mixed. We observed semantic interference which did not interact with lexical frequency, suggesting a non-lexical locus of the effect. Yet interference did not generalize to novel category members, arguing against a semantic locus of the effect. Because semantic interference in word-picture matching transferred to picture naming, this suggests a common origin of interference in the two tasks. We propose the shared origin is at the semantic level, whereby accessing semantic representations contributes to interference effects in both tasks. However, future research is needed to provide more conclusive evidence regarding the shared origin of semantic interference and the mechanism by which interference transfers across word-picture matching and naming tasks.

Author Contributions

All authors listed, have made substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

These experiments were part of the doctoral dissertation thesis of DH. A subset of these results was presented at the International Workshop on Language Production (2014; Geneva, Switzerland). We thank Eman Bahrani and Kelsey Tomlinson for their help with data collection. This research was supported by a fellowship through the William Orr Dingwall Foundation and the Rice University School of Social Sciences Dissertation Research Improvement Grant awarded to DH.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00710

Footnotes

  1. ^We use quotations to denote lexical representations and picture name responses (e.g., “dog”), whereas capitalization denotes the semantic representation corresponding to a word (e.g., DOG).
  2. ^Damian et al. (2001) do not report if the facilitation observed in the orientation task was statistically significant. However, the RTs reported for 10 subjects are in the predicted direction: orientation judgments were faster for semantically related compared to unrelated objects (i.e., 388 ms vs. 396 ms, respectively).
  3. ^Most agree that lexical frequency does not reflect semantic-level processes (e.g., Wingfield, 1968; Bartram, 1976; Meyer et al., 1998), but whether frequency has a lexical or phonological locus is debated (e.g., Jescheniak and Levelt, 1994; Finocchiaro and Caramazza, 2006; Navarrete et al., 2006). Because practice via frequently producing a word most likely strengthens connections between semantic-to-lexical and lexical-to-phonological representational levels, it is likely that lexical frequency effects are represented at both levels (if two levels are assumed: lexical vs. phonological; e.g., cf. Caramazza, 1997; Levelt et al., 1999).
  4. ^http://websites.psychology.uwa.edu.au/school/MRCDatabase/uwa_mrc.htmabase/uwa_mrc.htm
  5. ^Although the effects of lexical frequency described here could be due to either frequency or age of acquisition (they are highly correlated due to the fact that frequently produced words tend to be those acquired early in life e.g., Brysbaert and Ghyselinck, 2006, also see Barry et al., 2001), both are thought to reflect processes occurring when mapping meanings to words for naming (e.g., Belke et al., 2005a; Anderson, 2008).
  6. ^Interference for low- but not high-frequency words decreased numerically from the last cycle of old to the first cycle of novel items (low frequency Cycle 4 vs. Cycle 5 interference effect = 43 ms vs. 9 ms; high-frequency Cycle 4 vs. Cycle 5 interference effect = 11 ms vs. 14 ms; see Table 2), suggesting that lexical frequency differentially affects interference magnitudes across cycles, and thus interference generalization. However, post hoc analyses examining whether the interference effect across Cycles 1–8 and 2–5 differed for high- vs. low-frequency words were not significant (F’s < 1.30, p’s > 0.25 and F’s < 1.68, p’s > 0.17, respectively).
  7. ^Because we found that semantic interference remained stable across old and novel items in word-picture matching, it would not be expected to increase for novel items semantically related to those tested in a previous block or task. Thus, the failure to demonstrate interference generalization in word-picture matching precludes inferences concerning interference transfer from naming to word-picture matching.

References

Abdel Rahman, R. A., and Melinger, A. (2011). The dynamic microstructure of speech production: semantic interference built on the fly. J. Exp. Psychol. 37, 149–161. doi: 10.1037/a0021208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Anderson, J. D. (2008). Age of acquisition and repetition priming effects on picture naming of children who do and do not stutter. J. Fluency Disord. 33, 135–155. doi: 10.1016/j.jfludis.2008.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bajo, M. T. (1988). Semantic facilitation with pictures and words. J. Exp. Psychol. 14, 579–589.

Google Scholar

Balota, D. A., and Chumbley, J. I. (1985). The locus of word-frequency effects in the pronunciation task: lexical access and/or production? J. Mem. Lang. 24, 89–106. doi: 10.1016/0749-596X(85)90017-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barry, C., Hirsh, K. W., Johnston, R. A., and Williams, C. L. (2001). Age of acquisition, word frequency, and the locus of repetition priming of picture naming. J. Mem. Lang. 44, 350–375. doi: 10.1006/jmla.2000.2743

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belke, E. (2013). Long-lasting inhibitory semantic context effects on object naming are necessarily conceptually mediated: implications for models of lexical-semantic encoding. J. Mem. Lang. 69, 228–256. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2013.05.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belke, E., Brysbaert, M., Meyer, A. S., and Ghyselinck, M. (2005a). Age of acquisition effects in picture naming: evidence for a lexical-semantic competition hypothesis. Cognition 96, B45–B54. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2004.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belke, E., Meyer, A. S., and Damian, M. F. (2005b). Refractory effects in picture naming as assessed in a semantic blocking paradigm. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 58, 667–692. doi: 10.1080/02724980443000142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belke, E., and Stielow, A. (2013). Cumulative and non-cumulative semantic interference in object naming: evidence from blocked and continuous manipulations of semantic context. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 66, 2135–2160. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2013.775318

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biegler, K. A., Crowther, J. E., and Martin, R. C. (2008). Consequences of an inhibition deficit for word production and comprehension: evidence from the semantic blocking paradigm. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 25, 493–527. doi: 10.1080/02643290701862316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brodeur, M. B., Dionne-Dostie, E., Montreuil, T., and Lepage, M. (2010). The Bank of Standardized Stimuli (BOSS), a new set of 480 normative photos of objects to be used as visual stimuli in cognitive research. PLoS ONE 5:e10773. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0010773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brysbaert, M., and Ghyselinck, M. (2006). The effect of age of acquisition: partly frequency related, partly frequency independent. Vis. Cogn. 13, 992–1011. doi: 10.1080/13506280544000165

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Caramazza, A. (1997). How many levels of processing are there in lexical access? Cogn. Neuropsychol. 14, 177–208. doi: 10.1080/026432997381664

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, F., Dell, G. S., Bock, K., and Griffin, Z. M. (2000). Structural priming as implicit learning: a comparison of models of sentence production. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 29, 217–230. doi: 10.1023/A:1005101313330

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chen, Q., and Mirman, D. (2012). Competition and cooperation among similar representations: toward a unified account of facilitative and inhibitory effects of lexical neighbors. Psychol. Rev. 119, 417–430. doi: 10.1037/a0027175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, A. M., and Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychol. Rev. 82, 407–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crowther, J. E., and Martin, R. C. (2014). Lexical selection in the semantically blocked cyclic naming task: the role of cognitive control and learning. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:9. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crutch, S. J., and Warrington, E. K. (2005). Gradients of semantic relatedness and their contrasting explanations in refractory access and storage semantic impairments. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 22, 851–876. doi: 10.1080/02643290442000374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Damian, M. F., and Als, L. C. (2005). Long-lasting semantic context effects in the spoken production of object names. J. Exp. Psychol. 31, 1372–1384.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Damian, M. F., Vigliocco, G., and Levelt, W. J. M. (2001). Effects of semantic context in the naming of pictures and words. Cognition 81, B77–B86. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(01)00135-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Zubicaray, G., Johnson, K., Howard, D., and McMahon, K. (2014). A perfusion fMRI investigation of thematic and categorical context effects in the spoken production of object names. Cortex 54, 135–149. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dell, G. S. (1986). A spreading-activation theory of retrieval in sentence production. Psychol. Rev. 93, 283–321. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.93.3.283

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Finocchiaro, C., and Caramazza, A. (2006). The production of pronominal clitics: implications for theories of lexical access. Lang. Cogn. Process. 21, 141–180. doi: 10.1080/01690960400001887

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forde, E., and Humphreys, G. W. (1995). Refractory semantics in global aphasia: on semantic organisation and the access–storage distinction in neuropsychology. Memory 3, 265–307. doi: 10.1080/09658219508253154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forde, E. M., and Humphreys, G. W. (1997). A semantic locus for refractory behaviour: implications for access storage distinctions and the nature of semantic memory. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 14, 367–402. doi: 10.1080/026432997381529

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forde, E. M., and Humphreys, G. W. (2007). Contrasting effects of repetition across tasks: implications for understanding the nature of refractory behavior and models of semantic memory. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 7, 198–211. doi: 10.3758/CABN.7.3.198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forster, K. I., and Forster, J. C. (2003). DMDX: a windows display program with millisecond accuracy. Behav. Res. Methods 35, 116–124. doi: 10.3758/BF03195503

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gotts, S. J., and Plaut, D. C. (2002). The impact of synaptic depression following brain damage: a connectionist account of “access/refractory” and “degraded-store” semantic impairments. Cogn. Affect. Behav. Neurosci. 2, 187–213. doi: 10.3758/CABN.2.3.187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffin, Z. M., and Bock, K. (1998). Constraint, word frequency, and the relationship between lexical processing levels in spoken word production. J. Mem. Lang. 38, 313–338. doi: 10.1006/jmla.1997.2547

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gupta, P., and Cohen, N. J. (2002). Theoretical and computational analysis of skill learning, repetition priming, and procedural memory. Psychol. Rev. 109, 401–448. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.109.2.401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harvey, D. Y., and Schnur, T. T. (2015). Distinct loci of lexical and semantic access deficits in aphasia: evidence from voxel-based lesion-symptom mapping and diffusion tensor imaging. Cortex 67, 37–58. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, D. (1995). Lexical anomia: or the case of the missing lexical entries. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 48, 999–1023. doi: 10.1080/14640749508401426

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Howard, D., Nickels, L., Coltheart, M., and Cole-Virtue, J. (2006). Cumulative semantic inhibition in picture naming: experimental and computational studies. Cognition 100, 464–482. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2005.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Humphreys, G. W., Lamote, C., and Lloyd-Jones, T. J. (1995). An interactive activation approach to object processing: effects of structural similarity, name frequency, and task in normality and pathology. Memory 3, 535–586. doi: 10.1080/09658219508253164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Humphreys, G. W., Riddoch, M. J., and Quinlan, P. T. (1988). Cascade processes in picture identification. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 5, 67–104. doi: 10.1080/02643298808252927

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jescheniak, J. D., and Levelt, W. J. M. (1994). Word frequency effects in speech production: retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form. J. Exp. Psychol. 20, 824–843.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Kittredge, A. K., Dell, G. S., Verkuilen, J., and Schwartz, M. F. (2008). Where is the effect of frequency in word production? Insights from aphasic picture-naming errors. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 25, 463–492. doi: 10.1080/02643290701674851

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kroll, J. F., and Stewart, E. (1994). Category interference in translation and picture naming: evidence for asymmetric connections between bilingual memory representations. J. Mem. Lang. 33, 149–174. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levelt, W. J. M., Roelofs, A., and Meyer, A. S. (1999). A theory of lexical access in speech production. Behav. Brain Sci. 22, 1–75. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X99001776

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClelland, J. L., and Rumelhart, D. E. (1981). An interactive activation model of context effects in letter perception: I. an account of basic findings. Psychol. Rev. 88:375. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.88.5.375

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McRae, K., and Boisvert, S. (1998). Automatic semantic similarity priming. J. Exp. Psychol. 24, 558–572.

Google Scholar

Meyer, A. S., Sleiderink, A. M., and Levelt, W. J. M. (1998). Viewing and naming objects: eye movements during noun phrase production. Cognition 66, B25–B33. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00009-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morrison, C. M., Chappell, T. D., and Ellis, A. W. (1997). Age of acquisition norms for a large set of object names and their relation to adult estimates and other variables. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 50, 528–559. doi: 10.1080/027249897392017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarrete, E., Basagni, B., Alario, F. X., and Costa, A. (2006). Does word frequency affect lexical selection in speech production? Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 59, 1681–1690. doi: 10.1080/17470210600750558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarrete, E., Del Prato, P., and Mahon, B. Z. (2012). Factors determining semantic facilitation and interference in the cyclic naming paradigm. Front. Psychol. 3:38. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarrete, E., Del Prato, P., Peressotti, F., and Mahon, B. Z. (2014). Lexical selection is not by competition: evidence from the blocked naming paradigm. J. Mem. Lang. 76, 253–272. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2014.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Navarrete, E., Mahon, B. Z., and Caramazza, A. (2010). The cumulative semantic cost does not reflect lexical selection by competition. Acta Psychol. 134, 279–289. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2010.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oldfield, R. C., and Wingfield, A. (1965). Response latencies in naming objects. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 17, 273–281. doi: 10.1080/17470216508416445

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Oppenheim, G. M., Dell, G. S., and Schwartz, M. F. (2010). The dark side of incremental learning: a model of cumulative semantic interference during lexical access in speech production. Cognition 114, 227–252. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2009.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plaut, D. C., McClelland, J. L., Seidenberg, M. S., and Patterson, K. (1996). Understanding normal and impaired word reading: computational principles in quasi-regular domains. Psychol. Rev. 103, 56–115. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.103.1.56

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roelofs, A. (1992). A spreading-activation theory of lemma retrieval in speaking. Cognition 42, 107–142. doi: 10.1016/0010-0277(92)90041-F

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Santesteban, M., Costa, A., Pontin, S., and Navarrete, E. (2006). The effect of word-frequency on lexical selection in speech production: evidence from semantic homogeneous naming contexts. Cognitiva 18, 75–84. doi: 10.1174/021435506775462436

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scarborough, D. L., Cortese, C., and Scarborough, H. S. (1977). Frequency and repetition effects in lexical memory. J. Exp. Psychol. 3, 1–17.

Google Scholar

Schnur, T. T., Schwartz, M. F., Brecher, A., and Hodgson, C. (2006). Semantic interference during blocked-cyclic naming: evidence from aphasia. J. Mem. Lang. 54, 199–227. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2008.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Viggiano, M. P., Vannucci, M., and Righi, S. (2004). A new standardized set of ecological pictures for experimental and clinical research on visual object processing. Cortex 40, 491–509. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70142-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Damian, M. F., and Levelt, W. J. M. (2002). Semantic distance effects on object and action naming. Cognition 85, B61–B69. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(02)00107-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Lewis, W., and Garrett, M. F. (2004). Representing the meanings of object and action words: the featural and unitary semantic space hypothesis. Cogn. Psychol. 48, 422–488. doi: 10.1016/j.cogpsych.2003.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vitkovitch, M., and Humphreys, G. W. (1991). Perseverant responding in speeded naming of pictures: it’s in the links. J. Exp. Psychol. 17, 664–680. doi: 10.1037/0278-7393.17.4.664

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warrington, E. K., and McCarthy, R. (1983). Category specific access dysphasia. Brain 106, 859–878. doi: 10.1093/brain/106.4.859

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wei, T., and Schnur, T. T. (2016). Long-term interference at the semantic level: evidence from blocked-cyclic picture matching. J. Exp. Psychol. 42, 149–157. doi: 10.1037/xlm0000164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wilson, M. (1988). MRC psycholinguistic database: machine-usable dictionary, version 2.00. Behav. Res. Methods 20, 6–10. doi: 10.3758/BF03202594

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wingfield, A. (1968). Effects of frequency on identification and naming of objects. Am. J. Psychol. 81, 226–234. doi: 10.2307/1421267

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Learning another language is not only learning different words for the same things, but learning another way to think about things.

—Flora Lewis

Vocabulary Preview & Review

Have your students tried ESL Library’s Image Match tasks on our digital platform? These visual matching tasks are similar to ones you would find on paper, where students need to draw a line between a word and a picture to demonstrate their knowledge. On our updated digital tasks, students will now tap on a picture and then tap on a matching word.

While our printable beginner lessons feature this preview task with 8–10 images and definitions to match, our digital matching tasks typically split the word list into two tasks (e.g., Image Match A & B).

Updated Functionality

Have you already been assigning this task type to your students? The updated version of this matching task has a new functionality that your students will need to learn if they are already familiar with this digital task.

Instead of dragging and dropping (which was glitchy on some devices), your students will now tap (or click if using a laptop or desktop computer) on an image and then tap (or click) on the word. The word and image will then connect.

Your students can change their answers by tapping again to pop the image back out to the top and then tapping a new word. When they are finished, they will click the I’m Finished button as they did in the previous version. (Scroll down for video demo.)

With your Plus subscription, you can now assign this digital task to your students for homework or in‑class work. Your students can do the task on any mobile device or computer. You can also do this task together as a class by using the Launch feature. The Task Info feature gives you, the teacher, easy access to the answers.

Note

This digital task type is usually called Image Match in the digital task list. It is sometimes used in other task types such as grammar preview.

Assign Tasks

Go to a Word Bank or beginner Holidays & Events lesson and toggle to Digital. Then choose Assign Tasks and assign the Image Match task(s) with the due date of your choice to the class of your choice.

How to Assign Tasks (Digital Homework)

Task Features

After launching the task, students will see a row of images at the top with question boxes and words below. Students try to match the images to the words by tapping on the image (which will then become highlighted) and then tapping on the word in the box below (which will turn yellow and then fill with the image).

To change an answer, students tap on the word to pop the picture back out to the top of the list. They then choose a different match in the same way. Students can keep rearranging the images and words until they think everything is correct.

Review

After students complete the Image Match task, they click on I’m Finished to submit their answers and review their results.

If they are happy with their results, they can move on to a different task that you, the teacher, have assigned (e.g., Image Match B) by choosing Back to the Lesson. If they want to try the task again to achieve better results, they can choose Try Again.

Viewing Results as a Teacher

As a teacher, you can see your students’ Image Match results after they complete the task. Make sure you are logged in. Then go to Class Prep and navigate to Results.

How to View Digital Homework Results

Lesson Sections

The image matching task is currently available in our low-level sections for ESL Library members who have a Plus account, such as Word Bank, Holidays & Events (Beg), Simple Sentences, Super Simple Grammar, and Phonics & Pronunciation. For intermediate learners, we have a similar task type called Matching where students match words with definitions.

Look for the computer screen icon to check if a lesson has digital tasks.

Image Match: Demo

Related

  • Matching: Interactive Vocabulary Preview Task for English Learners
  • See & Spell: Interactive Spelling & Listening Practice for Beginner English Learners
  • Listen & Type: Blended Learning
  • Launch or Preview Digital Tasks

Young minds are highly curious and perceptive – kids are keen on learning about things around them. An important aspect of early learning is teaching children how to express themselves.

A robust vocabulary is a key to being able to verbalize and communicate with people. But the value of a broad vocabulary is beyond just that – it has been directly related to better performance in school, vast knowledge of words allows children to read and learn more information and helps them pick up reading faster.

But mindlessly learning words from a list is no fun, is it?

Well, we have the perfect alternative! These worksheets are a fun and interactive way for children to learn new words and expand their word library. The task here is simple – match the correct word to the picture depicted. This provides children with the opportunity to learn new words and their meanings all through visual representation making it easier for them to recall the word in the future.

This is the first worksheet depicting common and familiar images. Match the word to the correct image.

This is the second worksheet. While matching the words to the images, sound the words out loud to understand the pronunciation of the word.

Bright and colourful pictures help engage children and keep them motivated in any activity.

While most kids have seen these picture multiple times, either on the wall of the classroom or in their books, they may not necessarily know the word for them. So, this is a great way for them to learn new words.

Matching words is a fun and unique alternative to learning than simply memorizing words and their meanings. Visual representation is a great way to understand new things and greatly helps children improve their memory.

This is the sixth worksheet of the set. A great way to motivate children is to set a time limit for finishing a task – it pushes them to concentrate and work harder.

As children at this point already know the alphabet and how to write, they can also practice how to write each word along the way as they match it to the images.

Familiar words are used in this exercise as reliability helps children better associate with what they’re learning and feel comfortable.

Children are quick to get distracted by fun and colourful images. So make sure they are focussed on the task at hand.

Give your child or student a few worksheets to solve every week – with or without a time limit. Regular practice will help them learn and grow.

So, what are you waiting for?

Download, print and get started today!

Check out our extensive collection of free printable Picture Matching worksheets. These worksheets assist students in honing their visual discriminating skills. You can either download or print these worksheets straight away. This fascinating and interactive worksheet will keep kids entertained and engaged. The exercises include a variety of objects, shapes, and words. All of the worksheets are available as printable pdf documents.

Recently Added Worksheets

Click Here to create your own customized Matching Lists Worksheets.

These free printable matching worksheets are ideal for helping your child to develop and improve visual discrimination skills in a fun and engaging way. There are a plethora of them, each with a different level of difficulty to help you improve your logical reasoning skills.

An excellent free printable matching worksheet is provided here to assist your child in developing important recognition and matching skills.

Your child will benefit from free printable matching worksheets because they will learn how to take intricate designs and learn how to look for and recognize them in other places. This will allow your child to practice critical problem-solving skills while trying to find the design that is the most similar to the original design. Fill in the blanks with these free printable matching worksheets and amaze your child with the variety of activities available!

To ensure that their children can concentrate on the tasks at hand when using these free printable matching worksheets at home, parents should consider creating a designated workspace that is fully stocked with all of the supplies they will need. Make sure to provide clear instructions for each worksheet and to set time limits for completion in order to reduce the likelihood of frustration. All of these steps will assist your child in maximizing his or her abilities and becoming more comfortable with the daily rigors of school life. And don’t forget to supplement the academic kindergarten worksheets with some artistic pages to give the child a mental and creative break in addition to the academic kindergarten worksheets.

As a child matures, it is critical to steer them in the appropriate way. Different worksheets, such as picture to picture match worksheets, match pictures to words, match words to pictures, and match the same picture worksheets, assist mold their still-developing brain. If you have a kindergartner at home, download the free printable shadow matching worksheets and keep your kids occupied like never before.

Понравилась статья? Поделить с друзьями:
  • Word to php online
  • Word to spread feat tom thum the funk hunters chali 2na tom thum
  • Word to sound wave
  • Word to sound online
  • Word to songs for free