Word development in children



Language Development 2179

Photo by: Dusaleev V.

Definition

Language development is the process by which children come to understand
and communicate language during early childhood.

Description

From birth up to the age of five, children develop language at a very
rapid pace. The stages of language development are universal among humans.
However, the age and the pace at which a child reaches each milestone of
language development vary greatly among children. Thus, language
development in an individual child must be compared with norms rather than
with other individual children. In general girls develop language at a
faster rate than boys. More than any other aspect of development, language
development reflects the growth and maturation of the brain. After the age
of five it becomes much more difficult for most children to learn
language.

Receptive language development (the ability to comprehend language)
usually develops faster than expressive language (the ability to
communicate). Two different styles of language development are recognized.
In referential language development, children first speak single words and
then join words together, first into two-word sentences and then into
three-word sentences. In expressive language development, children first
speak in long unintelligible babbles that mimic the cadence and rhythm of
adult speech. Most children use a combination these styles.

Infancy

Language development begins before birth. Towards the end of pregnancy, a
fetus begins to hear sounds and speech coming from outside the
mother’s body. Infants are acutely attuned to the human voice and
prefer it to other sounds. In particular they prefer the higher pitch
characteristic of female voices. They also are very attentive to the human
face, especially when the face is talking. Although crying is a
child’s primary means of communication at birth, language
immediately begins to develop via repetition and imitation.

Between birth and three months of age, most infants acquire the following
abilities:

  • seem to recognize their mother’s voice
  • quiet down or smile when spoken to
  • turn toward familiar voices and sounds
  • make sounds indicating pleasure
  • cry differently to express different needs
  • grunt, chuckle, whimper, and gurgle
  • begin to coo (repeating the same sounds frequently) in response to
    voices
  • make vowel-like sounds such as «ooh» and
    «ah»

Between three and six months, most infants can do the following:

  • turn their head toward a speaker
  • watch a speaker’s mouth movements
  • respond to changes in a tone of voice
  • make louder sounds including screeches
  • vocalize excitement, pleasure, and displeasure
  • cry differently out of

    pain


    or hunger

  • laugh, squeal, and sigh
  • sputter loudly and blow bubbles
  • shape their mouths to change sounds
  • vocalize different sounds for different needs
  • communicate desires with gestures
  • babble for attention
  • mimic sounds, inflections, and gestures
  • make many new sounds, including «p,» «b,»
    and «m,» that may sound almost speech-like

The sounds and babblings of this stage of language development are
identical in babies throughout the world, even among those who are
profoundly deaf. Thus all babies are born with the capacity to learn any
language. Social interaction determines which language they eventually
learn.

Six to 12 months is a crucial age for receptive language development.
Between six and nine months babies begin to do the following:

  • search for sources of sound
  • listen intently to speech and other sounds
  • take an active interest in conversation even if it is not directed at
    them
  • recognize «dada,» «mama,»
    «bye-bye»
  • consistently respond to their names
  • respond appropriately to friendly and angry tones
  • express their moods by sound and body language
  • play


    with sounds

  • make long, more varied sounds
  • babble random combinations of consonants and vowels
  • babble in singsong with as many as 12 different sounds
  • experiment with pitch, intonation, and volume
  • use their tongues to change sounds
  • repeat syllables
  • imitate intonation and speech sounds

Between nine and 12 months babies may begin to do the following:

  • listen when spoken to
  • recognize words for common objects and names of

    family


    members

  • respond to simple requests
  • understand «no»
  • understand gestures
  • associate voices and names with people
  • know their own names
  • babble both short and long groups of sounds and two-to-three-syllable
    repeated sounds (The babble begins to have characteristic sounds of
    their native language.)
  • use sounds other than crying to get attention
  • use «mama» and «dada» for any person
  • shout and scream
  • repeat sounds
  • use most consonant and vowel sounds
  • practice inflections
  • engage in much vocal play

Toddlerhood

During the second year of life language development proceeds at very
different rates in different children. By the age of 12 months, most
children use «mama/dada» appropriately. They add new words
each month and temporarily lose words. Between 12 and 15 months children
begin to do the following:

  • recognize names
  • understand and follow one-step directions
  • laugh appropriately
  • use four to six intelligible words, usually those starting with
    «b,» «c,» «d,» and
    «g,» although less than 20 percent of their language is
    comprehensible to outsiders
  • use partial words
  • gesture and speak «no»
  • ask for help with gestures and sounds

At 15 to 18 months of age children usually do the following:

  • understand «up,» «down,»
    «hot,» «off»
  • use 10 to 20 intelligible words, mostly nouns
  • use complete words
  • put two short words together to form sentences
  • chatter and imitate, use some echolalia (repetitions of words and
    phrases)
  • have 20 to 25 percent of their speech understood by outsiders

At 18 to 24 months of age toddlers come to understand that there are words
for everything and their language development gains momentum. About 50 of
a child’s first words are universal: names of foods, animals,
family members,

toys


, vehicles, and clothing. Usually children first learn general nouns, such
as «flower» instead of «dandelion,» and they
may overgeneralize words, such as calling all toys «balls.»
Some children learn words for social situations, greetings, and
expressions of love more readily than others. At this age children usually
have 20 to 50 intelligible words and can do the following:

  • follow two-step directions
  • point to parts of the body
  • attempt multi-syllable words
  • speak three-word sentences
  • ask two-word questions
  • enjoy challenge words such as «helicopter»
  • hum and sing
  • express pain verbally
  • have 50 to 70 percent of their speech understood by outsiders

After several months of slower development, children often have a
«word spurt» (an explosion of new words). Between the ages
of two and 18 years, it is estimated that children add nine new words per
day. Between two and three years of age children acquire:

  • a 400-word vocabulary including names
  • a word for most everything
  • the use of pronouns
  • three to five-word sentences
  • the ability to describe what they just saw or experienced
  • the use of the past tense and plurals
  • names for body parts, colors, toys, people, and objects
  • the ability to repeat rhymes, songs, and stories
  • the ability to answer «what» questions

Children constantly produce sentences that they have not heard before,
creating rather than imitating. This

creativity


is based on the general principles and rules of language that they have
mastered. By the time a child is three years of age, most of a
child’s speech can be understood. However, like adults, children
vary greatly in how much they choose to talk.

Preschool

Three to four-year-olds usually can do the following:

  • understand most of what they hear
  • converse
  • have 900 to 1,000-word vocabularies, with verbs starting to predominate
  • usually talk without repeating syllables or words
  • use pronouns correctly
  • use three to six-word sentences
  • ask questions
  • relate experiences and activities
  • tell stories (Occasional

    stuttering


    and stammering is normal in preschoolers.)

Language skills usually blossom between four and five years of age.
Children of this age can do the following:

  • verbalize extensively
  • communicate easily with other children and adults
  • articulate most English sounds correctly
  • know 1,500 to 2,500 words
  • use detailed six to eight-word sentences
  • can repeat four-syllable words
  • use at least four prepositions
  • tell stories that stay on topic
  • can answer questions about stories

School age

At age five most children can do the following:

  • follow three consecutive commands
  • talk constantly
  • ask innumerable questions
  • use descriptive words and compound and complex sentences
  • know all the vowels and consonants
  • use generally correct grammar

Six-year-olds usually can correct their own grammar and mispronunciations.
Most children double their vocabularies between six and eight years of age
and begin reading at about age seven. A major leap in reading
comprehension occurs at about nine. Ten-year-olds begin to understand
figurative word meanings.

Adolescents generally speak in an adult manner, gaining language maturity
throughout high school.

Common problems


Language delay

is the most common

developmental delay


in children. There are many causes for language delay, both environmental
and physical. About 60 percent of language delays in children under age
three resolve spontaneously. Early intervention often helps other children
to catch up to their age group.

Common circumstances that can result in language delay include:

  • concentration on developing skills other than language
  • siblings who are very close in age or older siblings who interpret for
    the younger child
  • inadequate language stimulation and one-on-one attention
  • bilingualism, in which a child’s combined comprehension of two
    languages usually is equivalent to other children’s comprehension
    of one language
  • psychosocial deprivation

Language delay can result from a variety of physical disorders, including
the following:

  • mental retardation
  • maturation delay (the slower-than-usual development of the speech
    centers of the brain), a common cause of late talking
  • a hearing impairment
  • a learning disability
  • cerebral palsy
  • autism (a developmental disorder in which, among other things, children
    do not use language or use it abnormally)
  • congenital blindness, even in the absence of other neurological
    impairment
  • Klinefelter syndrome, a disorder in which males are born with an extra X
    chromosome

Brain damage or disorders of the central nervous system can cause the
following:

  • receptive aphasia or receptive language disorder, a deficit in spoken
    language comprehension or in the ability to respond to spoken language
  • expressive aphasia, an inability to speak or write despite normal
    language comprehension
  • childhood apraxia of speech, in which a sound is substituted for the
    desired syllable or word

Parental concerns

Language development is enriched by verbal interactions with other
children and adults. Parents and care-givers can have a significant impact
on early language development. Studies have shown that children of
talkative parents have twice the vocabulary as those of quiet parents. A
study from the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development
(NICHD) found that children in high-quality childcare environments have
larger vocabularies and more complex language skills than children in
lower-quality situations. In addition language-based interactions appear
to increase a child’s capacity to learn. Recommendations for
encouraging language development in infants include:

  • talking to them as much as possible and giving them opportunities to
    respond, perhaps with a smile; short periods of silence help teach the
    give-and-take of conversation
  • talking to infants in a singsong, high-pitched speech, called
    «parentese» or «motherese» (This is a
    universal method for enhancing language development.)
  • using one- or two-syllable words and two to three-word sentences
  • using proper words rather than baby words
  • speaking slowly, drawing-out vowels, and exaggerating main syllables
  • avoiding pronouns and articles
  • using animated gestures along with words
  • addressing the baby by name
  • talking about on-going activities
  • asking questions
  • singing songs
  • commenting on sounds in the environment
  • encouraging the baby to make vowel-like and consonant-vowel sounds such
    as «ma,» «da,» and «ba»
  • repeating recognizable syllables and repeating words that contain the
    syllable

When babies reach six to 12 months-of-age, parents should play word games
with them, label objects with words, and allow the baby to listen and
participate in conversations. Parents of toddlers should do the following:

  • talk to the child in simple sentences and ask questions
  • expand on the toddler’s single words
  • use gestures that reinforce words
  • put words to the child’s gestures
  • name colors
  • count items
  • gently repeat correctly any words that the child has mispronounced,
    rather than criticizing the child

Parents of two to three-year-olds should do the following:

  • talk about what the child and parent are doing each day
  • encourage the child to use new words
  • repeat and expand on what the child says
  • ask the child yes-or-no questions and questions that require a simple
    choice

(Table by GGS Information Services.)

(Table by GGS Information Services.)

Language development  

Age

Activity
Two months Cries, coos, and grunts.
Four months Begins babbling. Makes most vowel sounds and
  about half of consonant sounds.
Six months Vocalizes with intonation. Responds to own
name.  
Eight months Combines syllables when babbling, such «Ba-ba.»
Eleven months Says one word (or fragment of a word) with
meaning.  
Twelve months Says two or three words with meaning. Practices
  inflection, such as raising pitch of voice at the
  end of a question.
Eighteen months Has a vocabulary between five and 20 words,
  mostly nouns. Repeats word or phrase over and
  over. May start to join two words together.
Two years Has a vocabulary of 150–300 words. Uses I, me,
  and you. Uses at least two prepositions (in, on,
  under). Combines words in short sentences.
  About two-thirds of what is spoken is
understandable.  
Three years Has a vocabulary of 900–1000 words. Uses more
  verbs, some past tenses, and some plural nouns.
  Easily handles three-word sentences. Can give
  own name, sex, and age. About 90% of speech is
understandable.  
Four years Can use at least four prepositions. Can usually
  repeat words of four syllables. Knows some
  colors and numbers. Has most vowels and
  diphthongs and consonants p, b, m, w, and n
  established. Talks a lot and repeats often.
Five years Can count to ten. Speech is completely
  understandable, although articulation might not
  be perfect. Should have all vowels and
  consonants m, p, b, h, w, k, g, t, d, n, ng, y. Can
  repeat sentences as long as nine words. Speech
  is mostly grammatically correct.
Six years Should have all vowels and consonants listed
  above, has added, f, v, sh, zh, th, l. Should be able
  to tell a connected story about a picture.
Seven years Should have consonants s–z, r, voiceless th, ch,
  wh, and soft g. Should be able to do simple
  reading and print many words.
Eight years All speech sounds established. Carries on
  conversation at a more adult level. Can tell
  complicated stories of past events. Easily uses
  complex and compound sentences. Reads simple
  stories with ease and can write simple
compositions.  


SOURCE

:

Child Development Institute. 2004.
http://www.childdevelopmentinfo.com.
 
  • encourage the child to ask questions
  • read books about familiar things, with pictures, rhymes, repetitive
    lines, and few words
  • read favorite books repeatedly, allowing the child to join in with
    familiar words
  • encourage the child to pretend to read
  • not interrupt children when they are speaking

Parents of four to six-year-olds should:

  • not speak until the child is fully attentive
  • pause after speaking to give the child a chance to respond
  • acknowledge, encourage, and praise speech
  • introduce new words
  • talk about spatial relationships and opposites
  • introduce limericks, songs, and poems
  • talk about the television programs that they watch
  • encourage the child to give directions
  • give their full attention when the child initiates a conversation

Parents of six to 12-year-olds should talk to the children, not at them,
encourage conversation by asking questions that require more than a
yes-or-no answer, and listen attentively as the child recounts the
day’s activities.

Additional recommendations for parents and care-givers, by the American
Academy of Pediatrics and others, include:

  • talking at eye level with a child and supplementing words with body
    language, gestures, and facial expressions to enhance language
    comprehension
  • talking in ways that catch a child’s attention
  • using language to comfort a child
  • using correct pronunciations
  • using expressive language to discuss objects, actions, and emotions
  • playing with sounds and words
  • labeling objects and actions with words
  • providing objects and experiences to talk about
  • choosing activities that promote language
  • listening carefully to children and responding in ways that let them
    know that they have been understood, as well as encouraging further
    communication
  • using complete sentences and adding detail to expand on what a child has
    said
  • knowing when to remain silent
  • reading to a child by six months of age at the latest
  • encouraging children to ask questions and seek new information
  • encouraging children to listen to and ask questions of each other

Television viewing does not promote language development.

When to call the doctor

Parents should call the pediatrician immediately if they suspect that
their child may have a language delay or a hearing problem. Warning signs
of language delay in toddlers include:

  • avoiding eye contact
  • neither understanding nor speaking words by 18 months of age
  • difficulty learning nursery rhymes or simple songs
  • not recognizing or labeling common objects
  • inability to pay attention to a book or movie
  • poor articulation, such that a parent cannot understand the child more
    than 50 percent of the time

KEY TERMS


Apraxia

—Impairment of the ability to make purposeful movements, but not
paralysis or loss of sensation.


Expressive aphasia

—A developmental disorder in which a child has lower-than-normal
proficiency in vocabulary, production of complex sentences, and word
recall, although language comprehension is normal.


Expressive language

—Communicating with language.


Expressive language development

—A style of language development in which a child’s babble
mimics the cadence and rhythm of adult speech.


Receptive aphasia

—A developmental disorder in which a child has difficulty
comprehending spoken and written language.


Receptive language

—The comprehension of language.


Referential language development

—A style of language development in which a child first speaks
single words and then joins words together into two- and three-word
sentences.

Resources

BOOKS

Bochner, Sandra, and Jane Jones.

Child Language Development: Learning to Talk.

London: Whurr Publishers, 2003.

Buckley, Belinda.

Children’s Communications Skills: From Birth to Five Years.

New York: Routledge, 2003.

Oates, John, and Andrew Grayson.

Cognitive and Language Development in Children.

Malden, MA: Blackwell, 2004.

PERIODICALS

Howard, Melanie. «How Babies Learn to Talk.»

Baby Talk

69, no. 3 (April 2004): 69–72.

Tsao, Feng-Ming, et al. «Speech Perception in Infancy Predicts
Language Development in the Second Year of Life: A Longitudinal
Study.»

Child Development

75, no. 4 (July/August 2004): 1067–84.

Van Hulle, Carol A., et al. «Genetic, Environmental, and Gender
Effects on Individual Differences in Toddler Expressive Language.»

Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research

47, no. 4 (August 2004): 904–12.

ORGANIZATIONS


American Academy of Pediatrics.

141 Northwest Point Blvd., Elk Grove Village, IL 60007. Web site:
http://www.aap.org.


American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.

10801 Rockville Pike, Rockville, MD 20852. Web site:

http://asha.org

.


Child Development Institute.

3528 E. Ridgeway Road, Orange, CA 92867. Web site:
http://www.cdipage.com/index.htm.

WEB SITES

«Activities to Encourage Speech and Language Development.»

American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.

Available online at
http://www.asha.org/public/speech/development/Parent-Stim-Activities.htm (accessed December 29, 2004).

Dougherty, Dorthy P. «Developing Your Baby’s Language
Skills.»

KidsGrowth.

Available online at
http://www.kidsgrowth.com/resources/articledetail.cfm?id=714
(accessed December 29, 2004).

Genishi, Celia. «Young Children’s Oral Language
Development.»

Child Development Institute.

Available online at
http://www.childdevelopmentinfo.com/development/oral_language_development.shtml (accessed December 29, 2004).

«How Does Your Child Hear and Talk?»

American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.

Available online at
http://www.asha.org/public/speech/development/child_hear_talk.htm
(accessed December 29, 2004).

«Language Development in Children.»

Child Development Institute.

Available online at

http://www.childdevelopmentinfo.com/development/language_development.shtml (accessed December 29, 2004).

Lorenz, Joan Monchak. «Common Concerns about Speech Development:
Part I.»

KidsGrowth.

Available online at
<www.kidsgrowth.com/resources/articledetail.cfm?id=965<
(accessed December 29, 2004).

Rafanello, Donna. «Facilitating Language Development.»

Healthy Child Care America

, Summer 2000.Available online at
http://www.healthychildcare.org/pdf/LangDev.pdf (accessed
December 29, 2004).

Margaret Alic, PhD



Other articles you might like:

This article is about learning vocabulary during childhood, as part of a first language. For learning vocabulary while learning a second language, see Vocabulary learning.

Vocabulary development is a process by which people acquire words. Babbling shifts towards meaningful speech as infants grow and produce their first words around the age of one year. In early word learning, infants build their vocabulary slowly. By the age of 18 months, infants can typically produce about 50 words and begin to make word combinations.

In order to build their vocabularies, infants must learn about the meanings that words carry. The mapping problem asks how infants correctly learn to attach words to referents. Constraints theories, domain-general views, social-pragmatic accounts, and an emergentist coalition model have been proposed[1] to account for the mapping problem.

From an early age, infants use language to communicate. Caregivers and other family members use language to teach children how to act in society. In their interactions with peers, children have the opportunity to learn about unique conversational roles. Through pragmatic directions, adults often offer children cues for understanding the meaning of words.

Throughout their school years, children continue to build their vocabulary. In particular, children begin to learn abstract words. Beginning around age 3–5, word learning takes place both in conversation and through reading. Word learning often involves physical context, builds on prior knowledge, takes place in social context, and includes semantic support. The phonological loop and serial order short-term memory may both play an important role in vocabulary development.

A girl is sitting and looking down at a picture book that rests on her legs, while she rests her finger on an image.

Reading is an important means through which children develop their vocabulary.

Early word learning[edit]

Infants begin to understand words such as «Mommy», «Daddy», «hands» and «feet» when they are approximately 6 months old.[2][3] Initially, these words refer to their own mother or father or hands or feet. Infants begin to produce their first words when they are approximately one year old.[4][5] Infants’ first words are normally used in reference to things that are of importance to them, such as objects, body parts, people, and relevant actions. Also, the first words that infants produce are mostly single-syllabic or repeated single syllables, such as «no» and «dada».[5] By 12 to 18 months of age, children’s vocabularies often contain words such as «kitty», «bottle», «doll», «car» and «eye». Children’s understanding of names for objects and people usually precedes their understanding of words that describe actions and relationships. «One» and «two» are the first number words that children learn between the ages of one and two.[6] Infants must be able to hear and play with sounds in their environment, and to break up various phonetic units to discover words and their related meanings.

Development in oral languages[edit]

Studies related to vocabulary development show that children’s language competence depends upon their ability to hear sounds during infancy.[4][7][8] Infants’ perception of speech is distinct. Between six and ten months of age, infants can discriminate sounds used in the languages of the world.[4] By 10 to 12 months, infants can no longer discriminate between speech sounds that are not used in the language(s) to which they are exposed.[4] Among six-month-old infants, seen articulations (i.e. the mouth movements they observe others make while talking) actually enhance their ability to discriminate sounds, and may also contribute to infants’ ability to learn phonemic boundaries.[9] Infants’ phonological register is completed between the ages of 18 months and 7 years.[4]

Children’s phonological development normally proceeds as follows:[4]

6–8 weeks: Cooing appears

16 weeks: Laughter and vocal play appear

6–9 months: Reduplicated (canonical) babbling appears

12 months: First words use a limited sound repertoire

18 months: Phonological processes (deformations of target sounds) become systematic

18 months–7 years: Phonological inventory completion

At each stage mentioned above, children play with sounds and learn methods to help them learn words.[7] There is a relationship between children’s prelinguistic phonetic skills and their lexical progress at age two: failure to develop the required phonetic skills in their prelinguistic period results in children’s delay in producing words.[10] Environmental influences may affect children’s phonological development, such as hearing loss as a result of ear infections.[4] Deaf infants and children with hearing problems due to infections are usually delayed in the beginning of vocal babbling.

Babbling[edit]

Babbling is an important aspect of vocabulary development in infants, since it appears to help practice producing speech sounds.[11] Babbling begins between five and seven months of age. At this stage, babies start to play with sounds that are not used to express their emotional or physical states, such as sounds of consonants and vowels.[7] Babies begin to babble in real syllables such as «ba-ba-ba, neh-neh-neh, and dee-dee-dee,»[7] between the ages of seven and eight months; this is known as canonical babbling.[4] Jargon babbling includes strings of such sounds; this type of babbling uses intonation but doesn’t convey meaning. The phonemes and syllabic patterns produced by infants begin to be distinctive to particular languages during this period (e.g., increased nasal stops in French and Japanese babies) though most of their sounds are similar.[4][7] There is a shift from babbling to the use of words as the infant grows.[12]

Vocabulary spurt[edit]

As children get older their rate of vocabulary growth increases. Children probably understand their first 50 words before they produce them. By the age of eighteen months, children typically attain a vocabulary of 50 words in production, and between two and three times greater in comprehension.[5][7] A switch from an early stage of slow vocabulary growth to a later stage of faster growth is referred to as the vocabulary spurt.[13] Young toddlers acquire one to three words per month. A vocabulary spurt often occurs over time as the number of words learned accelerates. It is believed that most children add about 10 to 20 new words a week.[13] Between the ages of 18 to 24 months, children learn how to combine two words such as no bye-bye and more please.[5] Three-word and four-word combinations appear when most of the child’s utterances are two-word productions. In addition, children are able to form conjoined sentences, using and.[5] This suggests that there is a vocabulary spurt between the time that the child’s first word appears, and when the child is able to form more than two words, and eventually, sentences. However, there have been arguments as to whether or not there is a spurt in acquisition of words. In one study of 38 children, only five of the children had an inflection point in their rate of word acquisition as opposed to a quadratic growth.[13]

Development in sign languages[edit]

The learning mechanisms involved in language acquisition are not specific to oral languages. The developmental stages in learning a sign language and an oral language are generally the same. Deaf babies who are exposed to sign language from birth will start babbling with their hands from 10 to 14 months. Just as in oral languages, manual babbling consists of a syllabic structure and is often reduplicated. The first symbolic sign is produced around the age of 1 year.[14]

Young children will simplify complex adult signs, especially those with difficult handshapes. This is likely due to fine motor control not having fully developed yet. The sign’s movement is also often proximalized: the child will articulate the sign with a body part that is closer to the torso. For example, a sign that requires bending the elbow might be produced by using the shoulder instead. This simplification is systematic in that these errors are not random, but predictable.[14]

Signers can represent the alphabet through the use of fingerspelling.[15] Children start fingerspelling as early as the age of 2.[14] However, they are not aware of the association between fingerspelling and alphabet. It is not until the age of 4 that they realize that fingerspelling consists of a fixed sequence of units.[14]

Mapping problem[edit]

In word learning, the mapping problem refers to the question of how infants attach the forms of language to the things that they experience in the world.[16] There are infinite objects, concepts, and actions in the world that words could be mapped onto.[16] Many theories have been proposed to account for the way in which the language learner successfully maps words onto the correct objects, concepts, and actions.

While domain-specific accounts of word learning argue for innate constraints that limit infants’ hypotheses about word meanings,[17] domain-general perspectives argue that word learning can be accounted for by general cognitive processes, such as learning and memory, which are not specific to language.[18] Yet other theorists have proposed social pragmatic accounts, which stress the role of caregivers in guiding infants through the word learning process.[19] According to some[who?] research, however, children are active participants in their own word learning, although caregivers may still play an important role in this process.[20][21] Recently, an emergentist coalition model has also been proposed to suggest that word learning cannot be fully attributed to a single factor. Instead, a variety of cues, including salient and social cues, may be utilized by infants at different points in their vocabulary development.[1]

Theories of constraints[edit]

Theories of word-learning constraints argue for biases or default assumptions that guide the infant through the word learning process. Constraints are outside of the infant’s control and are believed to help the infant limit their hypotheses about the meaning of words that they encounter daily.[17][22] Constraints can be considered domain-specific (unique to language).

Critics[who?] argue that theories of constraints focus on how children learn nouns, but ignore other aspects of their word learning.[23] Although constraints are useful in explaining how children limit possible meanings when learning novel words, the same constraints would eventually need to be overridden because they are not utilized in adult language.[24] For instance, adult speakers often use several terms, each term meaning something slightly different, when referring to one entity, such as a family pet. This practice would violate the mutual exclusivity constraint.[24]

Below, the most prominent constraints in the literature are detailed:

  • Reference is the notion that a word symbolizes or stands in for an object, action, or event.[25] Words consistently stand for their referents, even if referents are not physically present in context.[25]
  • Mutual Exclusivity is the assumption that each object in the world can only be referred to by a single label.[17][26]
  • Shape has been considered to be one of the most critical properties for identifying members of an object category.[27] Infants assume that objects that have the same shape also share a name.[28] Shape plays an important role in both appropriate and inappropriate extensions.[27]
  • The Whole Object Assumption is the belief that labels refer to whole objects instead of parts or properties of those objects.[17][29] Children are believed to hold this assumption because they typically label whole objects first, and parts of properties of objects later in development.[29]
  • The Taxonomic Assumption reflects the belief that speakers use words to refer to categories that are internally consistent.[30] Labels to pick out coherent categories of objects, rather than those objects and the things that are related to them.[17][30] For example, children assume that the word «dog» refers to the category of «dogs», not to «dogs with bones», or «dogs chasing cats».[30]

Domain-general views[edit]

Domain-general views of vocabulary development argue that children do not need principles or constraints in order to successfully develop word-world mappings.[18] Instead, word learning can be accounted for through general learning mechanisms such as salience, association, and frequency.[18] Children are thought to notice the objects, actions, or events that are most salient in context, and then to associate them with the words that are most frequently used in their presence.[18] Additionally, research on word learning suggests that fast mapping, the rapid learning that children display after a single exposure to new information, is not specific to word learning. Children can also successfully fast map when exposed to a novel fact, remembering both words and facts after a time delay.[23]

Domain-general views have been criticized for not fully explaining how children manage to avoid mapping errors when there are numerous possible referents to which objects, actions, or events might point.[31] For instance, if biases are not present from birth, why do infants assume that labels refer to whole objects, instead of salient parts of these objects?[31] However, domain-general perspectives do not dismiss the notion of biases. Rather, they suggest biases develop through learning strategies instead of existing as built-in constraints. For instance, the whole object bias could be explained as a strategy that humans use to reason about the world; perhaps we are prone to thinking about our environment in terms of whole objects, and this strategy is not specific to the language domain.[23] Additionally, children may be exposed to cues associated with categorization by shape early in the word learning process, which would draw their attention to shape when presented with novel objects and labels.[32] Ordinary learning could, then, lead to a shape bias.[32]

[edit]

Social pragmatic theories, also in contrast to the constraints view, focus on the social context in which the infant is embedded.[19] According to this approach, environmental input removes the ambiguity of the word learning situation.[19] Cues such as the caregiver’s gaze, body language, gesture, and smile help infants to understand the meanings of words.[19] Social pragmatic theories stress the role of the caregiver in talking about objects, actions, or events that the infant is already focused-in upon.[19]

Joint attention is an important mechanism through which children learn to map words-to-world, and vice versa.[33] Adults commonly make an attempt to establish joint attention with a child before they convey something to the child. Joint attention is often accompanied by physical co-presence, since children are often focused on what is in their immediate environment.[33] As well, conversational co-presence is likely to occur; the caregiver and child typically talk together about whatever is taking place at their locus of joint attention.[33] Social pragmatic perspectives often present children as covariation detectors, who simply associate the words that they hear with whatever they are attending to in the world at the same time.[34] The co-variation detection model of joint attention seems problematic when we consider that many caregiver utterances do not refer to things that occupy the immediate attentional focus of infants. For instance, caregivers among the Kaluli, a group of indigenous peoples living in New Guinea, rarely provide labels in the context of their referents.[34] While the covariation detection model emphasizes the caregiver’s role in the meaning-making process, some theorists[who?] argue that infants also play an important role in their own word learning, actively avoiding mapping errors.[21] When infants are in situations where their own attentional focus differs from that of a speaker, they seek out information about the speaker’s focus, and then use that information to establish correct word-referent mappings.[20][34] Joint attention can be created through infant agency, in an attempt to gather information about a speaker’s intent.[34]

From early on, children also assume that language is designed for communication. Infants treat communication as a cooperative process.[35] Specifically, infants observe the principles of conventionality and contrast. According to conventionality, infants believe that for a particular meaning that they wish to convey, there is a term that everyone in the community would expect to be used.[35][36] According to contrast, infants act according to the notion that differences in form mark differences in meaning.[35][36] Children’s attention to conventionality and contrast is demonstrated in their language use, even before the age of 2 years; they direct their early words towards adult targets, repair mispronunciations quickly if possible, ask for words to relate to the world around them, and maintain contrast in their own word use.[35]

Emergentist coalition model[edit]

The emergentist coalition model suggests that children make use of multiple cues to successfully attach a novel label to a novel object.[1] The word learning situation may offer an infant combinations of social, perceptual, cognitive, and linguistic cues. While a range of cues are available from the start of word learning, it may be the case that not all cues are utilized by the infant when they begin the word learning process.[1] While younger children may only be able to detect a limited number of cues, older, more experienced word learners may be able to make use of a range of cues. For instance, young children seem to focus primarily on perceptual salience, but older children attend to the gaze of caregivers and use the focus of caregivers to direct their word mapping.[1] Therefore, this model argues that principles or cues may be present from the onset of word learning, but the use of a wide range of cues develops over time.[37]

Supporters of the emergentist coalition model argue that, as a hybrid, this model moves towards a more holistic explanation of word learning that is not captured by models with a singular focus. For instance, constraints theories typically argue that constraints/principles are available to children from the onset of word learning, but do not explain how children develop into expert speakers who are not limited by constraints.[38] Additionally, some argue[who?] that domain-general perspectives do not fully address the question of how children sort through numerous potential referents in order to correctly sort out meaning.[38] Lastly, social pragmatic theories claim that social encounters guide word learning. Although these theories describe how children become more advanced word learners, they seem to tell us little about children’s capacities at the start of word learning.[38] According to its proponents, the emergentist coalition model incorporates constraints/principles, but argues for the development and change in these principles over time, while simultaneously taking into consideration social aspects of word learning alongside other cues, such as salience.[39]

Pragmatic development[edit]

Both linguistic and socio-cultural factors affect the rate at which vocabulary develops.[40] Children must learn to use their words appropriately and strategically in social situations.[41] They have flexible and powerful social-cognitive skills that allow them to understand the communicative intentions of others in a wide variety of interactive situations. Children learn new words in communicative situations.[42] Children rely on pragmatic skills to build more extensive vocabularies.[43] Some aspects of pragmatic behaviour can predict later literacy and mathematical achievement, as children who are pragmatically skilled often function better in school. These children are also generally better liked.[44]

Children use words differently for objects, spatial relations and actions. Children ages one to three often rely on general purpose deictic words such as «here», «that» or «look» accompanied by a gesture, which is most often pointing, to pick out specific objects.[43] Children also stretch already known or partly known words to cover other objects that appear similar to the original. This can result in word overextension or misuses of words. Word overextension is governed by the perceptual similarities children notice among the different referents. Misuses of words indirectly provide ways of finding out which meanings children have attached to particular words.[43] When children come into contact with spatial relations, they talk about the location of one object with respect to another. They name the object located and use a deictic term, such as here or «there» for location, or they name both the object located and its location. They can also use a general purpose locative marker, which is a preposition, postposition or suffix depending on the language that is linked in some way to the word for location.[43] Children’s earliest words for actions usually encode both the action and its result. Children use a small number of general purpose verbs, such as «do» and «make» for a large variety of actions because their resources are limited. Children acquiring a second language seem to use the same production strategies for talking about actions. Sometimes children use a highly specific verb instead of a general purpose verb. In both cases children stretch their resources to communicate what they want to say.[43]

Infants use words to communicate early in life and their communication skills develop as they grow older. Communication skills aid in word learning. Infants learn to take turns while communicating with adults. While preschoolers lack precise timing and rely on obvious speaker cues, older children are more precise in their timing and take fewer long pauses.[45] Children get better at initiating and sustaining coherent conversations as they age. Toddlers and preschoolers use strategies such as repeating and recasting their partners’ utterances to keep the conversation going. Older children add new relevant information to conversations. Connectives such as then, so, and because are more frequently used as children get older.[46] When giving and responding to feedback, preschoolers are inconsistent, but around the age of six, children can mark corrections with phrases and head nods to indicate their continued attention. As children continue to age they provide more constructive interpretations back to listeners, which helps prompt conversations.[47]

Pragmatic influences[edit]

Caregivers use language to help children become competent members of society and culture. From birth, infants receive pragmatic information. They learn structure of conversations from early interactions with caregivers. Actions and speech are organized in games, such as peekaboo to provide children with information about words and phrases. Caregivers find many ways to help infants interact and respond. As children advance and participate more actively in interactions, caregivers adapt their interactions accordingly.[48] Caregivers also prompt children to produce correct pragmatic behaviours. They provide input about what children are expected to say, how to speak, when they should speak, and how they can stay on topic. Caregivers may model the appropriate behaviour, using verbal reinforcement, posing a hypothetical situation, addressing children’s comments, or evaluating another person.[49]

Family members contribute to pragmatic development in different ways. Fathers often act as secondary caregivers, and may know the child less intimately. Older siblings may lack the capacity to acknowledge the child’s needs. As a result, both fathers and siblings may pressure children to communicate more clearly. They often challenge children to improve their communication skills, therefore preparing them to communicate with strangers about unfamiliar topics. Fathers have more breakdowns when communicating with infants, and spend less time focused on the same objects or actions as infants. Siblings are more directive and less responsive to infants, which motivates infants to participate in conversations with their older siblings.[50] There are limitations to studies that focus on the influences of fathers and siblings, as most research is descriptive and correlational. In reality, there are many variations of family configurations, and context influences parent behaviour more than parent gender does.[51] The majority of research in this field is conducted with mother/child pairs.

Peers help expose children to multi-party conversations. This allows children to hear a greater variety of speech, and to observe different conversational roles. Peers may be uncooperative conversation partners, which pressures the children to communicate more effectively. Speaking to peers is different from speaking to adults, but children may still correct their peers. Peer interaction provides children with a different experience filled with special humour, disagreements and conversational topics.[44]

Culture and context in infants’ linguistic environment shape their vocabulary development. English learners have been found to map novel labels to objects more reliably than to actions compared to Mandarin learners. This early noun bias in English learners is caused by the culturally reinforced tendency for English speaking caregivers to engage in a significant amount of ostensive labelling as well as noun-friendly activities such as picture book reading.[52] Adult speech provides children with grammatical input. Both Mandarin and Cantonese languages have a category of grammatical function word called a noun classifier, which is also common across many genetically unrelated East Asian languages. In Cantonese, classifiers are obligatory and specific in more situations than in Mandarin. This accounts for the research found on Mandarin-speaking children outperforming Cantonese-speaking children in relation to the size of their vocabulary.[40]

Pragmatic directions[edit]

Pragmatic directions provide children with additional information about the speaker’s intended meaning. Children’s learning of new word meanings is guided by the pragmatic directions that adults offer, such as explicit links to word meanings.[53] Adults present young children with information about how words are related to each other through connections, such as «is a part of», «is a kind of», «belongs to», or «is used for». These pragmatic directions provide children with essential information about language, allowing them to make inferences about possible meanings for unfamiliar words.[54] This is also called inclusion. When children are provided with two words related by inclusion, they hold on to that information. When children hear an adult say an incorrect word, and then repair their mistake by stating the correct word, children take into account the repair when assigning meanings to the two words.[53]

In school-age children[edit]

A man sits with a group of children, and points to an image in a picture book.

Children in school share an interactive reading experience.

Vocabulary development during the school years builds upon what the child already knows, and the child uses this knowledge to broaden their vocabulary. Once children have gained a level of vocabulary knowledge, new words are learned through explanations using familiar, or «old» words. This is done either explicitly, when a new word is defined using old words, or implicitly, when the word is set in the context of old words so that the meaning of the new word is constrained.[55] When children reach school-age, context and implicit learning are the most common ways in which their vocabularies continue to develop.[56] By this time, children learn new vocabulary mostly through conversation and reading.[57] Throughout schooling and adulthood, conversation and reading are the main methods in which vocabulary develops. This growth tends to slow once a person finishes schooling, as they have already acquired the vocabulary used in everyday conversation and reading material and generally are not engaging in activities that require additional vocabulary development.[55][58]

During the first few years of life, children are mastering concrete words such as «car», «bottle», «dog», «cat». By age 3, children are likely able to learn these concrete words without the need for a visual reference, so word learning tends to accelerate around this age.[59] Once children reach school-age, they learn abstract words (e.g. «love», «freedom», «success»).[60] This broadens the vocabulary available for children to learn, which helps to account for the increase in word learning evident at school age.[61] By age 5, children tend to have an expressive vocabulary of 2,100–2,200 words. By age 6, they have approximately 2,600 words of expressive vocabulary and 20,000–24,000 words of receptive vocabulary.[62] Some claim that children experience a sudden acceleration in word learning, upwards of 20 words per day,[58] but it tends to be much more gradual than this. From age 6 to 8, the average child in school is learning 6–7 words per day, and from age 8 to 10, approximately 12 words per day.[23]

Means[edit]

Exposure to conversations and engaging in conversation with others help school-age children develop vocabulary. Fast mapping is the process of learning a new concept upon a single exposure and is used in word learning not only by infants and toddlers, but by preschool children and adults as well.[23] This principle is very useful for word learning in conversational settings, as words tend not to be explained explicitly in conversation, but may be referred to frequently throughout the span of a conversation.

Reading is considered to be a key element of vocabulary development in school-age children.[55][62][63][64] Before children are able to read on their own, children can learn from others reading to them. Learning vocabulary from these experiences includes using context, as well as explicit explanations of words and/or events in the story.[65] This may be done using illustrations in the book to guide explanation and provide a visual reference or comparisons, usually to prior knowledge and past experiences.[66] Interactions between the adult and the child often include the child’s repetition of the new word back to the adult.[67] When a child begins to learn to read, their print vocabulary and oral vocabulary tend to be the same, as children use their vocabulary knowledge to match verbal forms of words with written forms. These two forms of vocabulary are usually equal up until grade 3. Because written language is much more diverse than spoken language, print vocabulary begins to expand beyond oral vocabulary.[68] By age 10, children’s vocabulary development through reading moves away from learning concrete words to learning abstract words.[69]

Generally, both conversation and reading involve at least one of the four principles of context that are used in word learning and vocabulary development: physical context, prior knowledge, social context and semantic support.[70]

Physical context[edit]

Physical context involves the presence of an object or action that is also the topic of conversation. With the use of physical context, the child is exposed to both the words and a visual reference of the word. This is frequently used with infants and toddlers, but can be very beneficial for school-age children, especially when learning rare or infrequently used words.[64] Physical context may include props such as in toy play. When engaging in play with an adult, a child’s vocabulary is developed through discussion of the toys, such as naming the object (e.g. «dinosaur») or labeling it with the use of a rare word (e.g., stegosaurus).[70] These sorts of interactions expose the child to words they may not otherwise encounter in day-to-day conversation.

Prior knowledge[edit]

Past experiences or general knowledge is often called upon in conversation, so it is a useful context for children to learn words. Recalling past experiences allows the child to call upon their own visual, tactical, oral, and/or auditory references.[70] For example, if a child once went to a zoo and saw an elephant, but did not know the word elephant, an adult could later help the child recall this event, describing the size and color of the animal, how big its ears were, its trunk, and the sound it made, then using the word elephant to refer to the animal. Calling upon prior knowledge is used not only in conversation, but often in book reading as well to help explain what is happening in a story by relating it back to the child’s own experiences.[71]

[edit]

Social context involves pointing out social norms and violations of these norms.[72] This form of context is most commonly found in conversation, as opposed to reading or other word learning environments. A child’s understanding of social norms can help them to infer the meaning of words that occur in conversation. In an English-speaking tradition, «please» and «thank you» are taught to children at a very early age, so they are very familiar to the child by school-age. For example, if a group of people is eating a meal with the child present and one person says, «give me the bread» and another responds with, «that was rude. What do you say?», and the person responds with «please», the child may not know the meaning of «rude», but can infer its meaning through social context and understanding the necessity of saying «please».[72]

Semantic support[edit]

Semantic support is the most obvious method of vocabulary development in school-age children. It involves giving direct verbal information of the meaning of a word.[63][73] By the time children are in school, they are active participants in conversation, so they are very capable and willing to ask questions when they do not understand a word or concept. For example, a child might see a zebra for the first time and ask, what is that? and the parent might respond, that is a zebra. It is like a horse with stripes and it is wild so you cannot ride it.[73]

Memory[edit]

Memory plays an important role in vocabulary development, however the exact role that it plays is disputed in the literature. Specifically, short-term memory and how its capacities work with vocabulary development is questioned by many researchers[who?].

The phonology of words has proven to be beneficial to vocabulary development when children begin school. Once children have developed a vocabulary, they utilize the sounds that they already know to learn new words.[74] The phonological loop encodes, maintains and manipulates speech-based information that a person encounters. This information is then stored in the phonological memory, a part of short-term memory. Research shows that children’s capacities in the area of phonological memory are linked to vocabulary knowledge when children first begin school at age 4–5 years old. As memory capabilities tend to increase with age (between age 4 and adolescence), so does an individual’s ability to learn more complex vocabulary.[74]

Serial-order short-term memory may be critical to the development of vocabulary.[75] As lexical knowledge increases, phonological representations have to become more precise to determine the differences between similar sound words (i.e. «calm», «come»). In this theory, the specific order or sequence of phonological events is used to learn new words, rather than phonology as a whole.[75]

See also[edit]

  • Semantic mapping (literacy)
  • Vocabulary learning

References[edit]

  1. ^ a b c d e Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Hollich 2000, p. 145.
  2. ^ Tincoff & Jusczyk 1999.
  3. ^ Tincoff & Jusczyk 2012.
  4. ^ a b c d e f g h i Hoff 2006.
  5. ^ a b c d e Hulit & Howard 2002.
  6. ^ Barner, Zapf & Lui 2012.
  7. ^ a b c d e f Pinker 1994.
  8. ^ Waxman & Booth 2000.
  9. ^ Teinonen et al. 2008.
  10. ^ Keren-Portnoy, Majorano & Vihman 2009.
  11. ^ Fagan 2009.
  12. ^ Vihman 1993.
  13. ^ a b c Ganger & Brent 2004.
  14. ^ a b c d Emmorey 2001.
  15. ^ Baker 2016.
  16. ^ a b Bloom 2000, p. 22.
  17. ^ a b c d e Clark 2009, p. 284.
  18. ^ a b c d Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Hollich 2000, p. 143.
  19. ^ a b c d e Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Hollich 2000, p. 141.
  20. ^ a b Baldwin 1995.
  21. ^ a b Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Hollich 2000, p. 142.
  22. ^ Bloom 2000, p. 20.
  23. ^ a b c d e Bloom & Markson 1998.
  24. ^ a b Clark 1993, p. 53.
  25. ^ a b Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Hollich 2000, p. 139.
  26. ^ Merriman, Bowman & MacWhinney 1989, p. 3.
  27. ^ a b Clark 1993, p. 45.
  28. ^ Smith 2000, p. 52.
  29. ^ a b Clark 1993, p. 50.
  30. ^ a b c Clark 1993, p. 52.
  31. ^ a b Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Hollich 2000, p. 144.
  32. ^ a b Smith 2000, p. 55.
  33. ^ a b c Clark 2009, p. 285.
  34. ^ a b c d Sabbagh & Baldwin 2005.
  35. ^ a b c d Clark 2009, p. 286.
  36. ^ a b Clark 1993, p. 64.
  37. ^ Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Hollich 2000, p. 146.
  38. ^ a b c Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Hollich 2000, p. 159.
  39. ^ Hirsh-Pasek, Golinkoff & Hollich 2000, p. 160.
  40. ^ a b Tardif et al. 2009.
  41. ^ Bryant 2009, p. 339.
  42. ^ Tomasello 2000.
  43. ^ a b c d e Clark 1978.
  44. ^ a b Bryant 2009, pp. 352–353.
  45. ^ Bryant 2009, p. 342.
  46. ^ Bryant 2009, pp. 342–343.
  47. ^ Bryant 2009, pp. 343–345.
  48. ^ Bryant 2009, p. 348.
  49. ^ Bryant 2009, pp. 348–349.
  50. ^ Bryant 2009, pp. 350–351.
  51. ^ Bryant 2009, p. 351.
  52. ^ Chan et al. 2011.
  53. ^ a b Clark & Grossman 1998.
  54. ^ Clark & Andrew 2002.
  55. ^ a b c Baker, Simmons & Kameenui 1995.
  56. ^ Newton, Padak & Rasinski 2008, pp. 7–8.
  57. ^ Tabors, Beals & Weizman 2001, pp. 93–110.
  58. ^ a b Anglin & Miller 2000.
  59. ^ Tabors, Beals & Weizman 2001, p. 103.
  60. ^ Nippold 2004, pp. 1–8.
  61. ^ McKeown & Curtis 1987, p. 7.
  62. ^ a b Lorraine 2008.
  63. ^ a b Newton, Padak & Rasinski 2008.
  64. ^ a b Tabors, Beals & Weizman 2001, p. 97.
  65. ^ Nagy, Herman & Anderson 1985.
  66. ^ Tabors, Beals & Weizman 2001, pp. 101–103.
  67. ^ Tabors, Beals & Weizman 2001, p. 101.
  68. ^ Kamil & Hiebert 2005.
  69. ^ McKeown & Curtis 1987, p. 8.
  70. ^ a b c Tabors, Beals & Weizman 2001, p. 105.
  71. ^ Newton, Padak & Rasinski 2008, pp. xvii.
  72. ^ a b Tabors, Beals & Weizman 2001, p. 106.
  73. ^ a b Tabors, Beals & Weizman 2001, pp. 107.
  74. ^ a b Gathercole et al. 1992.
  75. ^ a b Leclercq & Majerus 2010.

Bibliography[edit]

  • Anglin, Jeremy M.; Miller, George A. (2000). Vocabulary Development: A Morphological Analysis. Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 131–132, 136. ISBN 978-0-631-22443-3.
  • Baker, Anne (2016). The linguistics of sign languages: an introduction. Amsterdam: John Benjamins Publishing Company. ISBN 9789027212306. OCLC 932169688.
  • Baker, S. K.; Simmons, D. C.; Kameenui, E. J. (1995). Vocabulary acquisition: Synthesis of the research. Technical Report No. 13. Eugene, OR.: National Center to Improve the Tools of Educators.
  • Baldwin, D. (1995). «Understanding the link between joint attention and language». In Moore, C.; Dunham, P. J. (eds.). Joint attention: Its origins and role in development. Hillsdale, NJ: LEA. pp. 131–158.
  • Barner, D.; Zapf, J.; Lui, T. (2012). «Is two a plural marker in early child language?». Developmental Psychology. 48 (1): 10–17. doi:10.1037/a0025283. PMID 21928879.
  • Bloom, L. (2000). «The intentionality model of word learning: How to learn a word, any word». In Golinkoff, R. M.; Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Bloom, L.; Smith, L. B.; Woodward, A. L.; Akhtar, N.; Hollich, G. (eds.). Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 19–50.
  • Bloom, P.; Markson, L. (1998). «Capacities underlying word learning». Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2 (2): 67–73. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(98)01121-8. PMID 21227068. S2CID 18751927.
  • Bryant, J. B. (2009). «Pragmatic development». In Bavin, E. L. (ed.). The Cambridge Handbook of Child Language. New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 339–357.
  • Chan, C.Y.; Tardif, T.; Chen, J.; Meng, X.; Zhu, Liqi; Meng, Xiangzhi (2011). «English- and Chinese-learning infants map novel labels to objects and actions differently». Developmental Psychology. 47 (5): 1459–1471. doi:10.1037/a0024049. PMID 21744954.
  • Clark, E. V. (1978). «Strategies for communicating». Child Development. 49 (4): 953–959. doi:10.2307/1128734. JSTOR 1128734.
  • Clark, E. V. (1993). The lexicon in acquisition. New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 32–66. ISBN 978-0-521-48464-0.
  • Clark, E. V. (2009). «Lexical meaning». In Bavin, E. L. (ed.). The Cambridge Handbook of Child Language. New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 283–300.
  • Clark, E. V.; Andrew, D.-W., W. (2002). «Pragmatic directions about language use: Offers of words and relations». Language in Society. 31 (2): 181–212. doi:10.1017/s0047404501020152. S2CID 145266963.
  • Clark, E. V.; Grossman, J. B. (1998). «Pragmatics directions and children’s word learning». Journal of Child Language. 25 (1): 1–18. doi:10.1017/S0305000997003309. PMID 9604566. S2CID 40731908.
  • Emmorey, Karen (2001). Language, cognition, and the brain : insights from sign language research. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 190–191. ISBN 0585390606. OCLC 49570210.
  • Fagan, M. (June 2009). «Mean length of utterance before words and grammar: Longitudinal trends and developmental implications of infant vocalizations». Journal of Child Language. 36 (3): 495–527. doi:10.1017/S0305000908009070. PMID 18922207. S2CID 41964944.
  • Ganger, J.; Brent, M. R. (2004). «Reexamining the vocabulary spurt». Developmental Psychology. 40 (4): 621–632. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.40.4.621. PMID 15238048.
  • Gathercole, S. E.; Willis, C. S.; Emslie, H.; Baddeley, A. D. (1992). «Phonological memory and vocabulary development during the early school years: A longitudinal study». Developmental Psychology. 28 (5): 887–898. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.28.5.887.
  • Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Golinkoff, R. M.; Hollich, G. (2000). «An emergentist coalition model for word learning: Mapping words to objects is a product of the interaction of multiple cues». In Golinkoff, R. M.; Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Bloom, L.; Smith, L. B.; Woodward, A. L.; Akhtar, N.; Hollich, G. (eds.). Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 136–164.
  • Hoff, E. (2006). «Language Experience and Language Milestones During Early Childhood». In McCartney, K.; Phillips, D. (eds.). Blackwell Handbook of Early Childhood Development. Blackwell. pp. 233–251. doi:10.1002/9780470757703.ch12. ISBN 9780470757703.
  • Hulit, L. M.; Howard, M. R. (2002). Born to talk. Toronto: Allyn and Bacon. ISBN 9780205342969.
  • Kamil, M. L.; Hiebert, E. H. (2005). «Teaching and learning vocabulary: Perspectives and persistent issues». In Hiebert, E. H.; Kamil, M. L. (eds.). Teaching and learning vocabulary: Bringing research to practice. Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. pp. 1–26.
  • Keren-Portnoy, T.; Majorano, M.; Vihman, M. M. (2009). «From phonetics to phonology: The emergence of first words in Italian» (PDF). Journal of Child Language. 36 (2): 235–267. doi:10.1017/S0305000908008933. PMID 18789180. S2CID 3119762.
  • Leclercq, A.; Majerus, S. (2010). «Serial-order short-term memory predicts vocabulary development: Evidence from a longitudinal study». Developmental Psychology. 46 (2): 417–427. doi:10.1037/a0018540. hdl:2268/29034. PMID 20210500.
  • Lorraine, S. (2008). Vocabulary development: Super duper handouts number 149. Greenville, SC: Super Duper Publications.[1]
  • McKeown, M. G.; Curtis, M. E. (1987). The nature of vocabulary acquisition. Hillside, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. ISBN 978-0-89859-548-2.
  • Merriman, W. E.; Bowman, L. L.; MacWhinney, B. (1989). «The mutual exclusivity bias in children’s word learning». Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 54 (3–4, Serial No. 220): 1–30. doi:10.2307/1166130. JSTOR 1166130. PMID 2608077.
  • Nagy, W. E.; Herman, P. A.; Anderson, R. C. (1985). «Learning words from context». Reading Research Quarterly. 22 (2): 233–253. doi:10.2307/747758. JSTOR 747758.
  • Newton, E.; Padak, N. D.; Rasinski, T. V. (2008). Evidence-based instruction in reading: A professional development guide to vocabulary. Boston, MA: Pearson Education.
  • Nippold, M. (2004). «Research on later language development:International perspectives». In Berman, R. A. (ed.). Language Development Across Childhood and Adolescence. Amsterdam: John Benjamins. pp. 1–8.
  • Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct: How the mind creates language. New York: Morrow and Co. pp. 262–296. ISBN 978-0-688-12141-9.
  • Sabbagh, M. A.; Baldwin, D. (2005). «Understanding the role of communicative intentions in word learning». In Elain, N.; Hoerl, C.; McCormack, T.; et al. (eds.). Joint Attention: Communication and Other Minds: Issues in Philosophy and Psychology. New York: Clarendon/Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199245635.001.0001. ISBN 9780199245635.
  • Smith, L. B. (2000). «Learning how to learn words: An associative crane». In Golinkoff, R. M.; Hirsh-Pasek, K.; Bloom, L.; Smith, L. B.; Woodward, A. L.; Akhtar, N.; Hollich, G. (eds.). Becoming a word learner: A debate on lexical acquisition. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 51–80.
  • Tabors, P. O.; Beals, D. E.; Weizman, Z. O. (2001). «‘You know what oxygen is?’: Learning new words at home». In Dickinson, D. K.; Tabor, P. O. (eds.). Beginning literacy with language. Baltimore, ML: Paul H. Brookes. pp. 93–110.
  • Tardif, T.; Fletcher, P.; Liang, W.; Kaciroti, N. (2009). «Early vocabulary development in Mandarin (Putonghua) and Cantonese». Journal of Child Language. 36 (5): 1115–1144. doi:10.1017/S0305000908009185. PMID 19435545. S2CID 22135359.
  • Teinonen, T.; Aslin, R.; Alku, P.; Csibra, G. (2008). «Visual speech contributes to phonetic learning in 6-month-old infants». Cognition. 108 (3): 850–855. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2008.05.009. PMID 18590910. S2CID 3351413.
  • Tincoff, Ruth; Jusczyk, Peter W. (1999). «Some Beginnings of Word Comprehension in 6-Month-Olds». Psychological Science. 10 (2): 172–175. doi:10.1111/1467-9280.00127. S2CID 143425608.
  • Tincoff, Ruth; Jusczyk, Peter W. (2012). «Six-Month-Olds Comprehend Words That Refer to Parts of the Body». Infancy. 17 (4): 432–444. doi:10.1111/j.1532-7078.2011.00084.x. PMID 32693484.
  • Tomasello, M. (2000). «The social-pragmatic theory of word learning». Pragmatics: Quarterly Publication of the International Pragmatics Association. 10 (4): 401–413. doi:10.1075/prag.10.4.01tom.
  • Vihman, M. (1993). «Variable paths to early word production». Journal of Phonetics. 21 (1–2): 61–82. doi:10.1016/S0095-4470(19)31321-X.
  • Waxman, S. R.; Booth, A. E. (2000). «Principles that are invoked in the acquisition of words, but not facts». Cognition. 77 (2): B33–B43. doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00103-7. PMID 10986366. S2CID 17009990.

In this Article

  • Stages of Language Development in Early Childhood
  • Effective Ways to Encourage Language Development in Kids
  • Signs of Developmental Delay in Kids

Language development in your child is one of the most important developmental milestones which prepares them to communicate effectively as they grow older. Understanding how you can help your child achieve this can be essential in building a strong foundation to help him through this process.

Learning how to communicate with others using words and sentences is a skill that your baby learns through active listening in his early childhood. This skill can help him express himself in the later stages and become a solid foundation for literacy and communication.

Stages of Language Development in Early Childhood

Language development in children takes place gradually and in different stages since childhood. Listed below are the various stages of language development since early childhood.

Three to twelve months

Your baby has a basic understanding of emotions like love, anger, and affection and responds to them with his unique coos and babbles. At this stage, if you notice that the babbles resemble any word, you can encourage him to speak. Babbling can be an important aspect of language development as this is the first manner of communication your baby has. At this stage, your baby will respond to his name being called and may even be able to speak out simple words like ‘ba’, ‘ma’, ‘da’, etc, as these are easier to pronounce.

Twelve to eighteen months

By now, your baby should be able to say single words and will try to imitate what you speak. He will also try to communicate more often and it is important that you encourage him by responding to him. You will also need to speak to him in order to help him grasp new words and sentence.

Eighteen months to two years

Your baby will have included more words to his vocabulary by this age. He can understand and speak around 300 words and also try to string a short sentence together. Although language development in each child varies, if your child hasn’t been able to speak simple words at this stage, it is essential that you consult a professional for further guidance.

Two to three years

Your baby’s vocabulary would have further improved and he will be able to speak longer sentences. He will also be able to comprehend what you are saying much better. His communication ability will improve and others would be able to understand what he is saying. He may also be able to multi-task as he plays and speaks at the same time.

Three to five years

As his curiosity grows, you can expect your child to ask you many questions and speak more complex sentences. He will able be able to understand more complex grammar and use it in his conversation. This can be a great time to encourage him to read books and enhance his vocabulary as well as improve his grammar.

Five to six years

By now, your child can understand what his teacher says and responds in a coherent manner. He should also be able to recognize more words and use them in sentences. Reading a book to him and engaging in intelligent conversations with him can help him improve his vocabulary and grammar.

Effective Ways to Encourage Language Development in Kids

During these stages, you can engage your child in different simple and fun activities to encourage his language. Here are a few activities for language development in early childhood.

1. Read him books

Reading bedtime stories is one of the best ways to introduce new words to your kid. You can practice this from a very young age and make a habit out of it.

2. Have intelligent conversations

Even though your child may not be able to respond effectively, you can still speak to him like he understands you. He will be listening to you and may even attempt a proper response.

3. Listen to music

Listening to and singing rhymes can help him pick up certain words and understand their rhythm.

4. Repeat

Make it a habit to repeat what your child says with the right pronunciation and grammar rather than criticizing him for it.

5. Limit the use of computers and television

Excess screen time is linked to delayed language development by some studies. Hence, only permit your child to watch activities that will allow him to improve his language.

6. Take your child out

Visiting a new environment can feed your child’s curiosity and he will want to know what the new things around him are called. This can be a great way to improve his vocabulary.

7. Speak about things your child is interested in

This can encourage him to be an active part of the conversation and speak more.

8. Introduce new words gradually

Don’t try to flood him with words. Add new words to the conversations step by step so that he understands and retains their meaning.

What Is the Difference Between Language and Speech?

The words that your child understands and uses during his communication form a language. This includes the ability to write as well as speak. Speech, on the other hand, is your child’s ability to create sounds that form words.

Signs of Developmental Delay in Kids

Your child may have a developmental delay in learning his language if the following signs are observed:

  • He doesn’t babble or try to talk
  • He hasn’t said ‘mama’, ‘dada’ or other simple phrases
  • He doesn’t point out things and try to speak out their names
  • He hasn’t learned to say single words
  • He hasn’t been using hand gestures, pointing or waving
  • He does not respond when you give simple instructions
  • He does not imitate words or actions
  • He hasn’t been able to string words together to form simple phrases
  • He stutters often
  • He cannot interact with others effectively
  • He does not use pronouns like ‘I’, ‘me’, ‘you’ properly

Language learning in early childhood is important to ensure that your child understands what is being said to him and is able to respond. It also forms a key aspect of communication, learning, and even his future relationships. Once your kid is able to use words in accordance with their meanings and purpose, he will be able to express himself better. This will help boost his confidence and help him give his opinions more articulately. In case you notice that your child hasn’t been able to communicate well or say simple words, you will need to consult an expert to understand the reason and get him the required therapy.

Also Read: Tips on Personality Development for Kids

​Children​ learn new words at an incredibly fast rate. The most important predictor of vocabulary learning is the number of words that children hear from adults (for example during interactions, conversations, play, book reading).

As children learn new words, they begin to understand and describe increasingly complex concepts (for example concepts to do with space, size, quantity, categories and time).​​

The importance of concept development and vocabulary

The words in our vocabulary are the building blocks for understanding and expressing ideas. As children are exposed to complex language (see the Grammar section), they begin to use more advanced vocabulary. They also begin to use more complex words to explain concepts, describe their observations, and make predictions.

Children’s vocabulary and concept development is dependent on consistent, nurturing and interacting learning experiences with adults and peers. Children need to have a large and varied vocabulary that continually grows.

Thus, concept development and vocabulary are key components for language learning. Developing these skills can pave the way for learning in language, the arts, sciences, technology, and mathematics.

  • Grammar

Key developmental milestones

The following ages and stages (adapted from Munro and McGregor, 2017) are a guide that reflects broad developmental norms, but does not limit the expectations for every child (see VEYLDF Practice Principle: High expectations for every child). It is always important to understand children’s development as a continuum of growth, irrespective of their age.

There is wide variation in the first words that children produce. However, data from English-speaking countries reflect the following general milestones for spoken (expressive) vocabulary: 

  • 12 months: 2 words plus mummy and daddy (or equivalent in languages other than English)
  • 18 months: 10-50 words
  • 2 years: 300 words
  • 2.5 years: 450 words
  • 3 years: 1000 words
  • 4 years: 2000 words
  • 5 years: 5000 plus words
  • 17 years: 36 000 to 136 000 words.

The words that children tend to say first are naming words (Nouns and Proper Nouns). Then action words (Verbs) are the second earliest type of word. Other words which are learnt early on are a few examples of modifiers (for example ‘more’), and personal-social phrases (for example ‘please’, ‘no’).

Word types

There are eight main types of words. Each word type is learned at different stages, and do different “jobs” when used in sentences. The speed and order that children learn word types is dependent on age, but also on their language-learning experiences. Children need to learn different word types, so they can start building their own sentences.

These include:

  • nouns (including Proper Nouns)—for example, Mum dog idea rainbow question Alex
  • pronouns—for example, I you they him she this these some their his myself ourselves each other
  • determiners—for example, the  a  an  her  their  our  those  this  that  many  more  neither  another
  • adjectives—for example, long pointy childish imaginary sisterly
  • verbs—for example, run play decide sorted thinking
  • adverbs—for example, slowly foolishly very mostly
  • prepositions—for example, in at on off into onto towards to about as with
  • conjunctions—for example, and or but because whenever after before.

Nouns, adjectives, and verbs are the most commonly encountered word types.

But there are also prepositions, pronouns, adverbs, determiners, and conjunctions.  

Concepts

Concepts are the ‘’big ideas’’ that children learn as they engage in a range of experiences.

For example, a baby drinks milk. As the baby grows older, the baby drinks water as well. The concept of ‘liquids that can be drunk’ expands as the child drinks different liquids.

Later, the concept that some things can be drunk but others cannot adds to the concept of liquids. This also shows how concept development supports reasoning skills.

Encouraging concept development in children is an important step in building knowledge of the arts, mathematics, science, and technology, and other aspects of everyday life.

Understanding the concepts—for example ‘’measurement’’—needs to come before learning the procedures of measurement.

Helping children to learn the words that represent the concept helps children to learn procedures later on.

  • spatial—for example, inside/outside behind/in front  around/through  side/middle  between/either side of
  • directional—for example, in/out up/down
  • numerical—for example, numbers and counting once twice
  • ordinal—for example, first, second, third … last next one by one    
  • shapes—for example, circle triangle rectangle curve straight pointy   
  • measurement—for example, describing items according to size, weight, volume, height, length, speed, temperature etc.   

    This will involve comparison: for example big/bigger/biggest/ tall/taller/tallest
  • pattern and structure—for example, patterns on clothes, in pictures, music, speech (rhyming)
  • temporal—for example, before/after, while/during,   today/yesterday/tomorrow, 10:00am/half past two days of week/months of year etc.
  • categorical—for example, types of fruit, clothes, vehicles, animals, plants, actions, shapes, feelings etc.
  • comparative—for example, big/bigger/biggest/tall/taller/tallest
  • descriptive—for example, describing items according to colour, pattern, texture, size, smell, taste, hardness/density etc.

Everyday interactions and planned experiences can be opportunities for learning about different concepts, and the words we can use to describe them.

Word relationships   

Another way to look at vocabulary is to think about how words relate to other words. Semantics is the study of word meanings. Semantic relationships are the ways that words are related to each other. Semantic knowledge is an important part of children’s language development, that helps them understand and express more complex concepts and ideas.

Here are some examples of types of word/semantic relationships:

Categories

  • Words can be put into categories (for example foods, emotions, buildings).   
  • Many categories also have subcategories
  • Fruit—apple, banana, watermelon
  • Apples—granny smith, golden delicious
  • Emotions—happiness anger sadness surprise
  • Happiness—joy delight

Antonyms

A pair of words with opposite meanings for example

  • hot/cold
  • fast/slow
  • big/small

Antonyms can be gradable (on a continuum)

  • you can use very or not very on these words
  • young/old, long/short, empty/full

They can be complementary

  • these antonyms mean one thing or another (there isn’t an in between)
  • for example sleep/wake,right/wrong, alive/dead

Or they can be relational

  • these opposites are about how concepts are related to each other
  • for example parent/child, brother/sister, doctor/patient, predator/prey

Synonyms

Words that have the same or similar meaning to each other. For example

  • hop/bounce/leap/spring/bound
  • nice/good/kind/lovely/fine
  • yummy/good/tasty/delicious

Theory to practice 

Learning vocabulary is a continual process of language and literacy development, which begins in the early years of life, and continues through schooling and beyond. 

Knowledge of vocabulary meanings affects children’s abilities to understand and use words appropriately during the language acts of listening, speaking, reading, and writing.”

— Sinatra, Zygouris-Coe, and Dasinger (2011, p. 333)

It is crucial that children have rich, implicit and explicit opportunities to learn new, and more advanced vocabulary.

Vocabulary knowledge “influences the complexities and nuances of children’s thinking … and how well they will understand printed texts» (Sinatra, Zygouris-Coe, and Dasinger, 2011, p. 333).    Having more complex language allows children to make meaning from what people say, and engage with new concepts. These are relevant for early numeracy, science, and literacy (and other disciplinary) knowledge, as well as for equipping children to share their own thoughts and feelings.

According to Cambourne’s Conditions of Learning (1988) and brain-research principles, children’s cognitive and language development is maximised when they are provided with meaningful opportunities to engage in independent and shared discussions, and exposed to many different forms of text that are appropriate to their world (Rushton, Eitelgeorge and Zickafoose, 2003).

A literature-based and print-rich classroom allows for different forms of texts that are created by children and reflects the real world … providing ample opportunity [for children] to see and experience the language.  — Rushton, Eitelgeorge and Zickafoose, 2003, p.13

Evidence base      

Research shows that when young children have meaningful interactions and are exposed to lots of words, they quickly develop wider vocabularies. The landmark research by Hart and Risley (1995) revealed the importance of interacting with children and exposing them to a high quantity and quality of language learning experiences. They also demonstrated that having more advanced vocabularies in the early years led to greater educational achievement in the middle years of primary school (Hart and Risley, 1995). Later research has echoed this importance of adult interacting for children’s vocabulary and general oral language development (for example Weisleder and Fernald, 2013).

It is important to embed opportunities for children to engage with various concepts (including shapes and spatial thinking). This enables children to “rehearse” the language needed to explore various concepts (Cohrssen, de Quadros-Wander, Page, and Klarin, 2017).   

Making sure to pause is an important strategy during concept development experiences. Pausing gives children a chance to think about more complex concepts, and allows educators to listen closely to children, and respond to them more supportively (Cohrssen, Church, and Tayler, 2014). See the teaching practices for interacting with others for more pedagogical strategies.

Getting started

Concept development and vocabulary

First words

  • use lots of language with children in every interaction
  • turn everyday situations into opportunities for discussion and description
  • repeat and reward each time a child attempts a word, giving them attention and affection.

Concept development

  • explore and describe the objects, movements and qualities that are around you
  • help children to explain their thoughts and feelings with words
  • incorporate counting, naming, and describing into everyday activities
  • brainstorm members of categories (for example who can think of types of vehicles?)
  • what kind of word is that? categorise vocabulary as they come up in interactions (for example run skip glide prance are all ways of moving)
  • show how It fits: use objects/pictures (to represent words/concepts) and sort words to categories and subcategories.

Antonyms

  • point out antonyms when they arise in conversation or book reading
  • play opposites! play words games where you guess the antonym (for example go/come, do/undo, quietly/loudly)
  • always introduce antonyms (opposites) in their pairs (for example hot/cold, big/little, nice/mean, in front/behind).

Synonyms

  • word finder: encourage children to think deeply and try to come up with other ways of saying something (for example how else can we say “big”? What’s another word for ….?)
  • word of the day: introduce new words explicitly, discuss their meaning, and model using the word in your language throughout the day
  • word maps: explore how words are related to others
  • talk it up: incorporate more advanced vocabulary in your interactions with children

Links to VEYLDF

  • Victorian Early Years Learning and Development Framework (2016)

  • VEYLDF Illustrative maps

Outcome 4: learning

Children develop a range of skills and processes such as problem solving, inquiry, experimentation, hypothesising, researching and investigating

  • create and use representation to organise, record and communicate mathematical ideas and concepts

Children transfer and adapt what they have learnt from one context to another

  • make connections between experiences, concepts and processes

Outcome 5: communication

Children interact verbally and non-verbally with others for a range of purposes

  • interact with others to explore ideas and concepts, clarify and challenge thinking, negotiate and share new understandings
  • demonstrate an increasing understanding of measurement and number using vocabulary to describe size, length, volume, capacity and names of numbers
  • use language to communicate thinking about quantities to describe attributes of objects and collections, and to explain mathematical ideas

Children engage with a range of texts and get meaning from these texts

  • begin to understand key literacy and numeracy concepts and processes, such as the sounds of language, letter–sound relationships, concepts of print and the ways that texts are structured

Children begin to understand how symbols and pattern systems work

  • begin to sort, categorise, order and compare collections and events and attributes of objects and materials in their social and natural worlds

Experiences plans and videos

  • Gesture and Joint Attention 
  • Lots of Trucks: Play, Reading, and Extending Language (Part 2)
  • Over in the Meadow 
  • Vocabulary for Early Communicators 

For early communicators (birth — 18 months)

  • Surprise Block Tower 
  • Vocabulary for Early Language Users 
  • What’s In The Box? 
  • Where Is The Green Sheep?
  • Yarra River Guided Play

For early language users (12 — 36 months)

  • Bush Tucker Cook Up 
  • Leaf Colours: Playing with Language
  • Making Biopots
  • Making Meaning through Dance
  • River of Words: Extending Vocabulary 
  • Volcano: Investigations and Sustained Shared Thinking

For language and emergent literacy learners (30 — 60 months)

Learning foci and teaching practices:

  • Grammar

References

Cohrssen, C., Church, A., and Tayler, C. (2014). Pausing for learning: Responsive engagement in mathematics activities in early childhood settings. Australasian Journal of Early Childhood, 39(4), 95–102.

Cohrssen, C., Church, A., and Tayler, C. (2014). Purposeful pauses: Teacher talk in early childhood mathematics activities. International Journal of Early Years Education. DOI: 10.1080/09669760.2014.900476.

Cohrssen, C. (2015)
You need to use your words! The Spoke (17 June 2015).

Cohrssen, C., de Quadros-Wander, B., Page, J., and Klarin, S. (2017).
Between the big trees: A project-based approach to investigating shape and spatial thinking in a kindergarten program. Australasian Journal of Early Childhood, 42(1), 94–104.

Hart, B. M., and Risley, T. R. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.

Munro, N., and McGregor, K. (2015) Semantics, in S. McLeod and J. McCormack (Eds.), Introduction to speech, language and literacy. South Melbourne, VIC, Australia: Oxford University Press. (pp. 181-230).

Rushton, S. P., Eitelgeorge, J., and Zickafoose, R. (2003). Connecting Brian Cambourne’s conditions of learning theory to brain/mind principles: Implications for early childhood Educators. Early Childhood Education Journal, 31(1), 11-21. doi: 10.1023/A:1025128600850

Sinatra, R., Zygouris-Coe, V., and Dasinger, S., 2011, Preventing a vocabulary lag: What lessons are learned from research. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 28(4), 333-334.

Victorian State Government Department of Education and Training (2016)


Victorian early years learning and development framework (pdf — 1.14mb) (VEYLDF). Retrieved 3 March 2018.

Victorian Curriculum and Assessment Authority (2016)
Illustrative Maps from the VEYLDF to the Victorian Curriculum F–10. Retrieved 3 March 2018.

Weisleder, A., and Fernald, A. (2013). Talking to children matters: Early language experience strengthens processing and builds vocabulary. Psychological Science, 24(11), 2143–2152.

Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., and Kucan, L. (2013). Bringing Words to Life: Robust Vocabulary Instruction (2nd ed.). New York: The Guildford Press.

Borovsky, A., Elman, J. L., and Fernald, A. (2012). Knowing a lot for one’s age: Vocabulary skill and not age is associated with anticipatory incremental sentence interpretation in children and adults. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 112(4), 417-436.   

Taylor, C. L., Christensen, D., Lawrence, D., Mitrou, F., and Zubrick, S. R. (2013).
Risk factors for children’s receptive vocabulary development from four to eight years in the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children. Plos One, 8(9).

Language development is an amazing process that culminates in the ability to communicate and understand speech. In fact, learning language is an innate process babies are born knowing how to do. Interestingly, all children, no matter which language their parents speak, learn language in the same way. 

Overall, there are three stages of language development, which occur in a familiar pattern. So, when children are learning to speak, understand, and communicate, they follow an expected series of milestones as they begin to master their native tongue. However, note that individual children will develop language skills at their own pace along this timeline within an expected range of deviation. Learn more about how children learn language.

Language Development Stage 1: Learning Sounds

When babies are born, they can hear and distinguish all the sounds in all the languages in the world. That’s about 150 sounds in about 6500 languages, though no language uses all of those sounds. The sounds a language uses are called phonemes and English has about 44. Some languages use more and some use fewer.

In this stage, babies learn which phonemes belong to the language they are learning and which don’t. The ability to recognize and produce those sounds is called “phonemic awareness,” which is important for children learning to read.

The best way to promote language development for babies is simply to talk to your child. Babies learn by experiencing (and listening to) the world around them, so the more language they are exposed to the better. Additionally, you can put words to their actions. Talk to them as you would in conversation, pausing for them to respond, then you can say back what you think they might say. However, note that simply talking to them attentively is enough for them to pick up language.

Baby Language Milestones

Though all children learn in basic stages, language develops at different rates in different children. Most children follow a familiar pattern.

  • Birth: When babies are born, they can already respond to the rhythm of language. They can recognize stress, pace, and the rise and fall of pitch.
  • 4 months: As early as 4 months, infants can distinguish between language sounds and other noise. For instance, they know the difference between a spoken word and a clap.
  • 6 months: By 6 months, babies begin to babble and coo and this is the first sign that the baby is learning a language. Babies are now capable of making all the sounds in all the languages of the world, but by the time they are a year old, they will have dropped the sounds that aren’t part of the language they are learning.

Language Development Stage 2: Learning Words

At this stage, children essentially learn how the sounds in a language go together to make meaning. For example, they learn that the sounds mahmee refer to the “being” who cuddles and feeds them, their mommy.

This is a significant step because everything we say is really just a stream of sounds. To make sense of those sounds, a child must be able to recognize where one word ends and another one begins. These are called “word boundaries.”

However, children are not learning words, exactly. They are actually learning morphemes, which are the smallest, discrete chunks words can be broken into. A morpheme may be a word on its own or may be combined with other morphemes to form a word. So in “mommy,” there are two morphemes: “ma” and “mee.”

Help your child build their language skills by reading to them often. And of course, keep having child-centric conversations with them as studies show that babies learn language best within a social context. Another way to encourage their communication and social skills is to mimic their noises (such as their babbling) and say them back to them. You can also mirror their facial expressions and describe their actions as well as narrate what is happening around them.

Understanding Plurals

The word mommies has two morphemes: mommy and –s. Children at this stage can recognize that the –s means «more than one.» They also begin to associate that meaning with other words when the sound is added.

Baby and Toddler Language Milestones

As your baby develops over the second half of their first year and into toddlerhood, their ability to make sounds and respond conversationally will continue to improve.

  • 8 months: Babies can now recognize groups of sounds and can distinguish word boundaries. Although they recognize these sound groups as words, they are still learning what these words mean. Babies of this age are more likely to comprehend the meaning of words related to their everyday experiences, particularly food and body parts.
  • 12 months: At this point, children are able to attach meanings to words. Once they can do that, they can begin to build a vocabulary. They also begin to mimic new words they hear.
  • 18 months: In order to communicate, children must know how to use the words they are learning. In this stage of language development, children are able to recognize the difference between nouns and verbs. Generally, the first words in a child’s vocabulary are nouns.

Language Development Stage 3: Learning Sentences

During this stage, children learn how to create sentences. That means they can put words in the correct order. For example, they learn that in English we say «I want a cookie» and «I want a chocolate cookie,» not «Want I a cookie» or «I want cookie chocolate.»

Children also learn the difference between grammatical correctness and meaning. Noam Chomsky created an example of this difference in the sentence “Colorless green ideas sleep furiously.” Children will know that although the sentence is grammatically correct, it doesn’t make sense. They know that green is a color and so it can’t be colorless and that ideas don’t sleep. However, they also know that the noun and verb structure of the sentence works.

To promote language development during this stage model good speech habits by speaking clearly, looking at them in the eye, not interrupting, and giving them a chance to talk. You can also add on to what they say to give them an idea of more complex ways to articulate their ideas and requests. Ask your child lots of questions and encourage their questions too to keep the dialog going.

Toddler and Preschooler Language Milestones

Your toddler and preschooler is now using full words, simple sentences, and eventually more complex dialog.

  • 24 months: At this stage, children begin to recognize more than nouns and verbs and gain an understanding of basic sentence structure. They can use pronouns, for example. They also know the right order of words in a sentence and can create simple sentences like «Me cookie?», which means «May I have a cookie?».
  • 30 to 36 months: By this age, about 90% of what children say is grammatically correct. The mistakes they make are usually things like adding -ed to irregular verbs to form the past tense. For example, they might say «I falled down» instead of «I fell down.» They learned the grammatical rule to form the past tense by adding -ed to a verb but have not yet learned the exceptions to the rule.
  • Beyond 3 years: As they grow, children continue to expand their vocabulary and develop more complex language. Their language use doesn’t completely resemble adult language until around the age of eleven. 

By the pre-teen years, kids begin to use what are called although-type sentences. These sentences show a concession such as, «Even though the man was tired, he kept working.» Young children would likely say “The man was tired, but he kept working.”

Language Development Concerns

Contact your child’s pediatrician if your child’s language skills seem to be developing slower than expected. Also, mention any other communication-related concerns you may have or if any other developmental milestones appear to be lagging to their pediatrician. Often your child may just be developing their language skills slower but within the normal expected range, but sometimes a delay may point to another issue.

Early intervention is important for many speech-related (and other developmental) concerns and can help to bolster these skills.

A Word From Verywell

Remember that children develop language at their own pace, and the best way to help is to talk, sing, and read to them. Other than that, simply enjoy your child’s coos, ma-mas, and da-das while they last.

Понравилась статья? Поделить с друзьями:
  • Word desktop что это
  • Word desktop windows 8
  • Word desire to learn
  • Word designs to draw
  • Word designs for walls