Word can be recognized


На основании Вашего запроса эти примеры могут содержать грубую лексику.


На основании Вашего запроса эти примеры могут содержать разговорную лексику.

можно распознать

можно узнать

могут быть признаны

может быть признано

можно признать

могут быть распознаны

могут распознаваться

может быть распознан

может быть распознано

можно опознать

могут признать

может быть признан

может быть признана

может распознаваться

может быть распознана


It has certain characteristics by which it can be recognized.



Однако у него есть несколько характерных особенностей, по которым его можно распознать.


Deactivated plugins can be recognized by their gray background.



Деактивированные плагины можно распознать из-за того, что они выделены серым цветом.


Both can be recognized by direct experiments.



И то и другое можно узнать путем непосредственных экспериментов.


Psychologists say that a person’s character can be recognized by many signs, and musical taste is one of them.



Психологи говорят, что характер человека можно узнать по множеству признаков, и музыкальный вкус — один из них.


Some can be recognized immediately as the work of a particular artist.



Некоторые из них могут быть признаны сразу, как работа конкретного художника.


Expert authors can be recognized with a special role, but membership is open to all.



Опытные авторы могут быть признаны с особой ролью, но членство открыто для всех.


It can be recognized in many small situations.


Given all of the above, diplomas and certificates of private institutions can be recognized by the state.



Учитывая все вышесказанное, дипломы и сертификаты частных учреждений могут быть признаны государством.


Now it can be recognized — and cured — far earlier, often before any symptoms have even appeared.



Теперь его можно распознать — и вылечить — намного раньше, часто до появления каких-либо симптомов.


A human motion (for example standing up from a chair) can be recognized as an aggregate of several elemental movements.



Движения человека (например, вставание со стула), могут быть признаны как совокупность нескольких элементарных движений.


But very often an overly selfish man can be recognized at the initial stage of acquaintance with him.



Но очень часто излишне эгоистичного мужчину можно распознать на начальном этапе знакомства с ним.


They can be recognized as part of it if they lay down their arms and join the political process.



Они могут быть признаны в рамках, когда сложат оружие и присоединяться к политическому процессу.


Its buses, which here are called the word «guaguas», can be recognized by the bright blue color.



Её автобусы, которые здесь называют словом «гуагуас» можно узнать по ярко-голубому цвету.


Indian style can be recognized on the photo and by the characteristic feature — furniture items can easily be transformed.



Индийский стиль можно узнать на фото и по характерной особенности: предметы мебели легко трансформируются.


Today these remarkable horses can be recognized by their tails and head form.



Сегодня этих замечательных лошадей можно узнать по их хвостам и форме головы.


This noble animal can be recognized by tufted ears and a short tail.



Это благородное животное можно узнать по кисточкам на кончиках ушей и короткому хвосту.


Persecution mania can be recognized by some symptoms in humans.



Манию преследования можно распознать по некоторым симптомам, проявляющимся у человека.


Technical and infrastructural issues can be recognized and the effects of these on processes determined and brought under control.



Можно распознать технические и инфраструктурные ошибки, определить их влияние на процессы и держать их под контролем.


It can be recognized by the characteristic yellow-brown, sometimes with a reddish tint, color.



Его можно узнать по характерной желто-коричневой, порой с красноватым оттенком, окраске.


It can be recognized by the letter O. At the moment it is in one of the private collections.



Ее можно распознать под буквой О. На данный момент она пребывает в одной из частных коллекций.

Ничего не найдено для этого значения.

Результатов: 854. Точных совпадений: 854. Затраченное время: 209 мс

Documents

Корпоративные решения

Спряжение

Синонимы

Корректор

Справка и о нас

Индекс слова: 1-300, 301-600, 601-900

Индекс выражения: 1-400, 401-800, 801-1200

Индекс фразы: 1-400, 401-800, 801-1200

Deficits in word recognition, reading rate, reading fluency, decoding skills, vocabulary, or weaknesses in listening and language comprehension can underlie an impairment in the development of reading comprehension as an academic skill set.

From: Handbook of Psychological Assessment (Fourth Edition), 2019

Educational therapy

Louise Spear-Swerling, in The Clinical Guide to Assessment and Treatment of Childhood Learning and Attention Problems, 2020

Three common profiles of poor reading

SWRDs involve core weaknesses in decoding, usually related to phonological weaknesses, coupled with at least average vocabulary knowledge and broad language comprehension (Catts, Adlof, & Weismer, 2006; Leach, Scarborough, & Rescorla, 2003). In addition to their difficulties reading individual words, children with SWRDs also frequently have problems with reading fluency and reading comprehension. However, their difficulties in these latter areas are associated entirely with problems in word reading, not language comprehension. Poor fluency may be due either to inaccurate or nonautomatic reading of individual words. Likewise, for these children, poor reading comprehension is caused entirely by problems in word reading; if children can decode a text accurately and with reasonable fluency, then they can comprehend it. Jamie, the first child described at the outset of this chapter, had a profile of SWRD. SWRD is a common profile in students with dyslexia, which was Jamie’s diagnosis.

SRCDs involve the opposite profile, one that is distinct from dyslexia (Cutting et al., 2013). Poor readers with SRCDs have at least average decoding and phonological skills, but nevertheless, their reading comprehension is impaired (Leach et al., 2003). Usually students’ reading comprehension difficulties are associated with oral language weaknesses in areas such as vocabulary, broad listening comprehension, or higher-level language abilities such as pragmatics (Catts et al., 2006; Norbury & Nation, 2011). Therefore these students tend to display comprehension weaknesses not only during reading but also in listening activities, such as during teacher read-alouds or class discussions. If students with SRCDs have problems with reading fluency, those problems are based in language, not decoding. For example, students with this profile might read text slowly because they are having trouble comprehending it (Valencia, 2011). Eli’s profile exemplified SRCDs. He had a reading disability, but one different from dyslexia.

Children with a mixed profile, or MRDs, have a combination of the above types of difficulties (Catts et al., 2006; Leach et al., 2003). They not only have weaknesses in decoding and phonological skills but also have a core comprehension component to their reading problems. Clinicians might encounter a profile of MRD in students with broad language disabilities or in some students with autism spectrum disorders (Norbury & Nation, 2011). Although students with MRDs often have weaknesses in listening comprehension, their reading comprehension typically is more impaired than their listening due to the additional influence of poor decoding. Fluency problems in students with MRDs may relate to both factors, poor decoding and language weaknesses.

Knowledge about individual students’ profiles can help clinicians understand and integrate a wide array of assessment data, with important implications for instruction, assessment, and accommodations (Catts, Kamhi, & Adlof, 2012; Spear-Swerling, 2015). To be effective, interventions must properly target a child’s component weaknesses. Children with SWRDs and MRDs generally benefit from phonics interventions, whereas those with SRCDs do not, because their difficulties lie outside the domain of decoding (Aaron, Joshi, Gooden, & Bentum, 2008). Students with MRDs need more than phonics intervention to be successful; they also require intervention targeting the source(s) of their comprehension weakness, such as vocabulary, background knowledge, syntax, or comprehension monitoring (Oakhill, Cain, & Elbro, 2015). In assessment, oral reading fluency CBMs may be very useful for monitoring progress in children with SWRDs and MRDs but probably not for students with SRCDs, who will require assessments more focused on comprehension. Students with SWRDs often benefit from accommodations that involve listening to text that is too difficult for them to read, whereas for those with SRCDs, merely hearing a text read aloud is less helpful (Erickson, 2013).

Read full chapter

URL: 

https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128157558000058

Aptitude and achievement testing

Lynda J. Katz, Franklin C. Brown, in Handbook of Psychological Assessment (Fourth Edition), 2019

Measurement of underlying cognitive processes involved in reading achievement

The measurement of reading achievement is a complex undertaking because it includes a series of separate but integrated processes. Deficits in word recognition, reading rate, reading fluency, decoding skills, vocabulary, or weaknesses in listening and language comprehension can underlie an impairment in the development of reading comprehension as an academic skill set. To date, extensive research studies have documented that problems in several distinct areas can, and do, contribute to differences between good and poor readers (Swanson & Hsieh, 2009; Wagner & Torgesen, 1987; Wolf & Bowers, 1999). As a result, a number of instruments have been developed to further explicate specific underlying deficits if problems in reading comprehension are found during the measurement of achievement. There are at least eight commonly used measures of phonological awareness itself. A list of these can be found in the Mather and Abu-Hamour (2013). In addition, the various test publishing companies have several others on the market at this time.

The particular measure with which we are most familiar is the Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing (CTOPP) (Wagner, Torgesen, & Rashotte, 1999). The CTOPP is a measure of phonological awareness, phonological memory, and rapid naming. Difficulty in any of these areas is considered a common cause of reading disability. The first version of the test was developed for children between the ages of 5 and 6, but we are most familiar with the second version, available for individuals between the ages of 7 and 24. The second version consists of seven core subtests and six supplemental subtests. In our experience, it is necessary to administer all 12 subtests with adolescents and young adults as there appears to be a ceiling effect when just the core subtests are given. There is now a CTOPP-2 that contains normative data collected in 2008 and 2009 which was published in 2013 (Wagner, Torgesen, Rashatte, & Person, 2013). However, the basic instrument remains the same and results in five composite scores: Phonological Awareness; Phonological Memory; Rapid Symbolic Naming; Rapid Non-Symbolic Naming; and an Alternate Phonological Awareness Composite Score.

The CTOPP does a remarkable job of sorting out phonological deficits from rapid naming deficits. Rapid naming deficit is a factor important for reading skills that was identified early in the work of Denckla and Rudel (1976). Rapid naming skills have more recently become the focus of research and intervention studies that are concerned with identifying and diagnosing an underlying developmentally based learning disorder involving reading fluency and its impact on comprehension (Wolf & Bowers, 1999; Wolf & Katzir-Cohen, 2001; Wolf, 1986, 2016; Wolf, Miller, & Donnelly, 2000). This research has found that the majority of children with developmental reading disabilities start with weaknesses in naming speed, and then go on to develop problems in reading fluency. Thus the ability of the CTOPP to differentiate these various skills is crucial to better understand an individual’s reading disorder.

The role of fluency is also an important factor in reading disorders. As discussed earlier, reading fluency has been added to several test batteries (e.g., WIAT-III and WJ-IV). This is at least in part due to the work of Wolf. Indeed, in recent years, Wolf and Katzir-Cohen (2001) explicated a new conceptualization for fluency. They propose that reading fluency is the product of a combination of the initial development of accuracy and automaticity in core reading that include the sublexical and lexical processes. These processes are perceptual, phonological, orthographic, and syntactic in nature, and important for integrating single word and connected text. Once fully developed, fluency relies on a combination of accuracy and rate in which the actual decoding is relatively effortless, oral reading is smooth with correct prosody, and attention can be focused on comprehension rather than the basic mechanics of reading fluency. Based on these considerations, various strategies have been formulated to increase reading fluency. There are several excellent papers (Alber-Morgan, 2006; Mastropieri, Leinart, & Scruggs, 1999) detailing research validated strategies, which can be incorporated into achievement findings and recommendations.

Read full chapter

URL: 

https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128022030000055

Executive function training for children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder

Mark D. Rapport, … Lauren M. Friedman, in The Clinical Guide to Assessment and Treatment of Childhood Learning and Attention Problems, 2020

Information input channel

The interplay between stimulus-receiving modalities has important implications for learning. Verbally presented information (e.g., classroom instruction and audiobook) gains direct access to the auditory cortex of the brain, whereas reading or pictorial information must be visually inspected and orthographically converted to PH code before it can enter the auditory cortex and be held temporarily in PH STM for additional processing by the CE. This is why some parents report that their children are better able to comprehend and recall information when it is read to them orally rather than having to read and orthographically convert the information themselves. These children are unable to convert the read information efficiently and lose significant information before it becomes available in the PH STM store to be used for passage comprehension-related tasks (i.e., the system becomes bottlenecked).

A recent study of the bottleneck phenomenon involving children with ADHD revealed that two interleaved processes contributed to reading comprehension deficits in ADHD: slowed orthographic conversion and underdeveloped CE processes (Friedman et al., 2017). Slowed orthographic conversion can occur for several reasons; however, one of the most common causes is that automaticity has not been developed for a large number of basic words, math facts (addition, subtraction, and multiplication), and relevant rules (literacy and numeracy) when attempting to activate this information in long-term memory (LTM). The bottleneck occurs because the pursuit of the needed words or math facts slows the speed by which it reaches the limited capacity PH STM store, which can only maintain the information for several seconds unless it is constantly refreshed via covert rehearsal. The resulting consequence is that many children will forget the task instructions—particularly multistep instructions—prior to fully retrieving the needed information from LTM.

Practitioner recommendations to accommodate and strengthen orthographic conversion

Use flash cards with printed words and associated pictures beginning with easily recognized two- and three-letter words to increase automaticity of word recognition. Begin at a slow speed and use a game-like format (points or check marks that can be traded in for preferred activities or other incentives) to gradually increase the speed by which the words can be recognized and stated orally. Once words can be recognized quickly, gradually introduce unknown words of the same length prior to introducing longer words. Have the child continue this exercise a minimum of 5 days per week for approximately 15-minutes per day (e.g., during breakfast) throughout the year until a 400-word fast-recognition vocabulary has developed.

For children devoid of a strong knowledge of phonics, adopt a similar flash card approach beginning with easily recognized sound combinations (e.g., “th”), and eventually use two cards at a time to combine (blend) sounds to make a whole word (e.g., bi+rd).

Use flash cards of simple number combinations (begin with addition), which can be solved mentally without the benefit of a pencil/paper or calculator. Begin with the 1’s (1+3=4 with the answer shown on the reverse side), then introduce the 2’s, then the 3’s, and so on until all combinations up to the 9’s have been mastered and can be answered quickly and accurately. Use a game-like format similar to the one recommended immediately above and limit training sessions to 15 minutes.

Read full chapter

URL: 

https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128157558000083

Speech-Based Interfaces

Sadaoki Furui, in Text Entry Systems, 2007

Isolated Word Scoring

The error rate of speech recognition is defined as “the average fraction of items incorrectly recognized.” Here, an item can be a word, a subword unit (e.g., a phone), or an entire utterance. For an isolated word recognition system, the error rate is defined as

Here, N is the number of words in the test utterance and NE the number of words incorrectly recognized. The latter can be subdivided into substitution error, NS, and deletion (incorrect rejection) error, ND:

Sometimes the fraction of correctly recognized words, C = 1 – E, called correctness, is used:

(8.10)C = NCN = N − NS −NDN.

These measures do not include so-called insertions, since it is assumed that the beginning and the end of each word can be detected directly from the energy of the signal. However, in real applications in which speech is contaminated by noise, it is not always easy to detect word boundaries, and sometimes noise signals cause insertion errors. Therefore, under these real-world conditions, the same measure as that used in continuous word scoring, which will be described later, is also used in the isolated recognition task.

For isolated word recognizers, a measure more specific than the various contributions to the error rate, a confusion matrix, has also been used, in which the class of substitutions is divided into all possible confusions between words. The confusion Cijis defined as the probability that word i is recognized as word j. The value Cii is the fraction of times word i is correctly recognized. These probabilities are estimated by measuring the number of times the confusion took place,

where Nij is the number of times word j was recognized on the input word i. The confusion matrix gives more detailed information than the error rates. Insertions and deletions can also be included in the matrix by adding a null word i = 0 (nonvocabulary word). Then, the row C0j contains insertions, the column Ci0 the deletions, and C00 = 0. Using this expanded confusion matrix, the error rate can be calculated from the diagonal elements, i.e., E = 1 – σiCii = σijCij. The elements Cij for ij are called the off-diagonal elements.

Read full chapter

URL: 

https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780123735911500085

Assessment and identification of learning disabilities

Emily A. Farris, … Timothy N. Odegard, in The Clinical Guide to Assessment and Treatment of Childhood Learning and Attention Problems, 2020

Achievement

Most methods used for the identification of students with LDs include low achievement as a defining characteristic, which is necessary but insufficient for the identification of LDs. Identification procedures based on any model used for the identification of LDs will include measures of a child’s level of achievement. Such information is crucial because it provides an observation of the exact behaviors of interest, the child’s ability to read, write, spell, and solve mathematical problems (Fletcher & Miciak, 2017; Siegel, 1999; Stanovich, 1999). Furthermore, empirical data suggest that there are five different forms of LDs that each impact a different area of achievement: word recognition and spelling, reading comprehension, mathematical computations, mathematical problem-solving, and written expression (Fletcher et al., 2018).

Standardized norm-referenced measures of achievement are used to assess a child’s level of performance in these areas. Common examples of such measures include the Woodcock–Johnson Test of Achievement, currently in the fourth edition (Schrank, Mather, & McGrew, 2014), the Kaufman Test of Educational Achievement, currently in the third edition (Kaufman & Kaufman, 2014), and the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test, currently in the third edition (Psychological Corporation, 2009). Each of these test batteries measures the specific domains impacted by LDs and includes norms for age and grade that allow for standard scores to be computed. The provision of standard scores supports efforts to determine if a child is achieving at a level comparable to his or her peers. This is often operationalized as a cut point. For example, a cut point of the 25th percentile could be adopted. Those children for whom 75% of their age group outperformed them on a given measure of academic achievement (e.g., untimed isolated word reading) would be deemed as exhibiting low achievement in this area. Yet, difficulties arise in determining the exact placement of cut points that impact the provision of services to individual children (Francis et al., 2005). These difficulties emphasize that if they are to be used, the creation of cut points should be made with great care.

Read full chapter

URL: 

https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128157558000010

Definition of MIR methodology

Roberto Raieli, in Multimedia Information Retrieval, 2013

Tempo properties of AR

The specific issues of AR with regards to the form of MIR’s relationship to VR and VDR, differ in the fact that an audio data stream is connoted by tempo-related properties, and also properties relating to frequency and sound characteristics such as tone, pitch, timbre, melody and harmony. Again, an audio stream is simply a set of data that must be elaborated and made useable by the processing system by identifying its defining characteristics. The same thing can be said for document treatment, where the theory and technique of AR share things common to all MIR, but specialize in some particular respects.

It is clear that with regard to audio, it is a completely different language and medium from those dealt with in TR, VR and VDR, although the latter has a number of relationships to sound. In the case of content-based treatment of audio objects and audio documents, the same theoretical issues concerning the whole of MIR can be proposed. In fact, it still means working directly with certain content-related elements and concrete and specific objects, as intangible as sounds may seem, without accepting excessive mediation of systems based on the terms and the translation of documented content in text.

Even here it is possible to speak of contentual elements being treated within themselves with consistent tools, and semantic aspects that can be examined later for a total treatment of the sound object. Treatment, in addition to considering the characteristics of sound data as such, and the feelings that they derive from, must also give an account of the complex and abstract interpretation that sounds, and particularly music, may be given. What remains permanent is the non-solvability of issues such as harmonics, where neither contentual nor semantic considerations can come to final conclusions, and where even human emotivity struggles to define their quality – let alone a machine. Content-based treatment of audio documents, based on the same common problems of processing all document types, can boast some real revolutionary developments, effectiveness and limitations.

Among the more problematic points of AR, is the transcription of sound tracks. Music, dialogue or other sounds must be registered in the language of the system with propriety and adherence to the original object in order to be effectively dealt with, whatever the status of the document to which the technique is applied. Nothing is too different, in essence, with problems occurring in other forms of MIR when dealing with a poorly digitized document, which cannot give a correct representation of its content. Digital audio technology does seem, however, to already be at the right level in order to define the best conditions for confronting these issues.

Among other problems is the alignment of different versions of the same sound track, as well as comparing changes between tracks. In such cases the process can be tricked by simple changes in interpretation or performance, and not by recognizing similarities between two objects. This is parallel to the cases of different points of view in VR and VDR, or of homonymy in TR. Specific emphasis will be given to the issues of audio-thumbnailing, or audio abstracts and audio browsing. They are connected, in as much as the ability to draw a complete effective sound synthesis allows the ability to browse and evaluate an audio document in analysis and search.

Two main models of AR may be considered: Automatic Speech Recognition (ASR) and Music Information Retrieval – which some scholars name MIR. Different but intersecting search paths have defined over time the techniques and methods for processing speech on one hand and music on the other. To these were added research on sound or noise in general, or whatever else they may be. Throughout the whole of AR, however, the three lines intersect constantly, since the system must allow for a unique perspective on the treatment of all types of audio documents, and it is not uncommon for a track to have a mixture of words, music and other sounds. An advanced AR system must be equipped with adequate tools for speech recognition, identification of words, recognition of voices, and reconstruction and transcription of dialogue. It must also be able to highlight music in the background, and able to distinguish different sounds in a collection, just as it must be able to separate music and words. Speech recognition and MIR techniques, in addition to the possibility of being individually perfected, and in certain cases individually applied, must therefore be integrated with each other.

Figure 5.16. ‘Talk to Me’ interface, didactic system of Automatic Speech Recognition32

Figure 5.17. AudioFex, AR module of the MUVIS system33

Finally, there can be different types of audio document processing allowed by AR systems: general analysis and research based on sound data; the comparison between segments of a single sound file; the comparison of sound segments of different types; analysis and recovery of short individual sounds; description and retrieval of sound tracks in audio-visual objects; search by selecting words in songs or dialogues; the processing and indexing of speech; and the treatment of various sounds and noises.

Some modalities of AR already functioning are: speaker identification, based on the ability to recognize human voices regardless of the words spoken; a typical similarity query, which queries a database using sample tracks; and query-by-humming, which has the advantage of allowing search by similarity to an audio model hummed by a user — more or less similar to visual sketches.

George Tzanetakis provides a concise presentation of the extensive problems of AR and, while declaring that attention to audio document content-based search systems are fairly recent, he states how these techniques are the first to be commercially exploited thanks to the increasing dissemination of music files.34

Lately there have been instances of experimental algorithms best capable of processing the sound content of a document, even if such general methods are derived from other methods of MIR or ASR. Music data – with which the author deals exclusively – due to its complexity requires specific systems and techniques which are now on their way to a major development, and AR systems even now can deal with both simple sound files and the most complex musical documents. The ‘feature vectors’ of musical documents represent models of content characteristics, and are treated by MIR systems for the typical content-based operations of ‘indexing’, ‘similarity search’, ‘recognition’ and ‘music browsing’ all taking place via the specific methods of AR.35

Tzanetakis describes four characteristics of automatic analysis of music contents: ‘rhythm’, ‘pitch content’, ‘structural analysis’ and ‘linking audio to scores’. With regards to ‘rhythm’, the importance of sound analysis in tempo for recognizing and processing music is evident. Automatic discovery of rhythm, and the definition of its histograms, is one of the main techniques of AR, known as ‘Beat Histogram’, and is based on the possibility of identifying and calculating the salient features of the periodic repetitions of a sound signal. These features presented in data sets can accurately represent even the minimum level of rhythmical variations in a piece, thus allowing it to be identified and compared to other pieces of music.

Figure 5.18. ‘Beat Histogram’ of different styles of music36

The analysis of ‘pitch content’, or ‘harmonics’, has one of its most critical points in ‘polyphonic transcription’, the process of converting a recording into a symbolic representation, similar to that of a musical score containing the notes played by several instruments. Translation and transcription of sounds onto a data table is an essential element for analysis. However, it is very difficult to obtain this automatically. The best tool currently available for this transcription is ‘Pitch Histogram’, consisting of statistical representations of the harmonic development characteristics in a piece of music. The machine cannot give anything more than a statistical representation of the rich harmonies existing in music.

The ‘structural analysis’, or musical ‘form’, aims to discover the proper configuration of a piece, using a matrix to compare notes or the strings of notes in a track. The comprehensive analysis of the music’s structure is based on the calculation of that array, and is called the

Figure 5.19. ‘Similarity matrix’ of a Bach Prelude37

‘similarity matrix’. The diagonal of the matrix has its highest value when it corresponds to the similarity of a segment. The visual transposition of this structure can reveal the hierarchical and regular composition of musical signals, and reveal the repetitions within a piece, their degree of similarity, and in general the structural explanation of the sound of a piece.

The fourth area of interest in AR is that of ‘linking audio to scores’. Sound tracks can be represented both by proper audio signals and by symbolic transcripts, and it is theoretically possible to perform search and confrontational operations through the symbolic tables, presenting results in sound form. The symbols can permit a comparison of concrete sound signals, providing that the transcription is very accurate and allowing for the systematic errors that a machine makes. A practical case for this treatment is ‘query-by-humming’, where the user can hum a melody sample which is then translated by the system into appropriate symbols. Obviously, to associate symbolic and sonorous representations, the system must develop so that it can identify the similarity of audio signals with symbols.38

Fully aligning the basic principles of MIR, Tzanetakis concludes that the ability to ‘automatically comprehend’ audio signals by ‘actual musical means’ is essential to developing and solving one of AR’s key issues: to search and recover a sound track without any knowledge or need for terminological metadata or any kind of external reference.

Read full chapter

URL: 

https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9781843347224500059

Alcohol and the legal system: Effects of alcohol on eyewitness testimony

Julie Gawrylowicz, Georgina Bartlett, in The Handbook of Alcohol Use, 2021

Methodological challenges and future research directions

One methodological issue present in alcohol administration studies is the timing and rate of alcohol absorption. An examination into the variability in alcohol absorption during a drinking session found that the average peak BAC was 0.073 with a range of 0.047–1.00 g/dL (Winek, Wahba, & Dowdell, 1996). The average peak time, i.e. when the BAC reached its highest was 17.4 minutes, ranging from 0 to 74 minutes. Other factors such as the timing and nature of one’s last meal and participants’ gender can further impact upon absorption rate and target BAC, as seen in Hildebrand Karlén et al. (2015) where female participants reached significantly higher BACs than males.

To complicate things even further, research has shown that memory encoding and retrieval might be affected differently depending on the specific limb of the BAC curve. Söderlund, Parker, Schwartz, and Tulving (2005) showed that alcohol impaired encoding in cued and free recall and recognition of completed word fragments regardless of limb, but word recognition was only impaired during the ascending BAC. Thus, how alcohol affects our memory not only depends on the memory task utilized, but also on the specific timing of when the memory was tested, that is on the ascending or the descending limb of the BAC curve. Future studies should therefore examine the effects of alcohol on eyewitness memory performance on different limbs on the BAC curve to shed more light on when exactly episodic memory might be impaired.

Whilst there are methodological difficulties associated with the timing of the alcohol administration and absorption, there are also challenges associated with assigning appropriate control groups. Studies typically use a control group, in which participants are knowingly not consuming alcohol, and/or a placebo group, in which participants are under the impression that they are consuming alcohol but do actually not receive any. The placebo condition is useful in measuring the behavioral and cognitive effects of expecting alcohol in the absence of pharmacological effects. Eyewitness memory studies have now begun to use fully-balanced placebo designs to control for alcohol expectancies (see Flowe et al., 2019; Gawrylowicz et al., 2019). In addition, to the usual alcohol, placebo and sober control group, a fourth group is included: the reverse placebo group (individuals do not believe that they received alcohol when they actually did). Gawrylowicz et al. (2019) found that their reverse placebo group performed consistently poorer on a cued recall task, they gave fewer correct responses and made more errors compared to the alcohol, control and placebo group. Flowe et al. (2019) found a significant expectancy effect for recall completeness when participants were interviewed with the Self-administered Interview. There was no significant effect of actual alcohol consumption. These findings suggest that pharmacological effects of alcohol might not be solely responsible for differences in memory performance, but that alcohol-related expectancies play a crucial role too.

Including placebo groups can be challenging, as it requires convincing people that they did or did not receive alcohol when in fact they did or did not. In Flowe et al.’s (2019) study 27% of participants who had been told that they received tonic water thought that they drank alcohol, whereas 22% believed they had alcohol when in fact they consumed tonic. Similarly, Schreiber Compo et al. (2017) reported that 16% of placebo participants believed that they had not consumed alcohol. Even if the placebo manipulation is successful, placebo participants often report feeling less intoxicated than their intoxicated counterparts (Kneller & Harvey, 2016).

To summarize, the administration of alcohol in laboratory settings comes with a myriad of methodological challenges. These challenges range from variations in peak BACs to ensuring timing of administration is appropriate and equal. What’s more is that the inclusion of viable placebo groups is often not possible, as the drink deception is often difficult to execute, especially in the reverse placebo condition.

Read full chapter

URL: 

https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128167205000165

WISC–V and the Evolving Role of Intelligence Testing in the Assessment of Learning Disabilities

Donald H. Saklofske, … Lawrence G. Weiss, in WISC-V (Second Edition), 2019

Subtypes of Learning Disabilities

An enormous body of research has accumulated with various approaches for identifying subtypes of LDs. Inquiry into possible subtypes of LDs began with Johnson and Myklebust (1967) and colleagues (e.g., Boshes & Myklebust, 1964; Myklebust & Boshes, 1960). They proposed a nonverbal disability characterized by the absence of serious problems in areas of language, reading, and writing, but with deficiencies in social perception, visual-spatial processing, spatial and right–left orientation, temporal perception, handwriting, mathematics, and executive functions such as disinhibition and perseveration. The neuropsychology literature later characterized a nonverbal LD as reflecting a right hemisphere deficit (Pennington, 1991; Rourke, 1989); however, it remains the least well understood.

Subtyping holds widespread appeal because it offers a way to explain the heterogeneity within the category of LDs, to describe the learning profile of students with LDs more specifically, leading to an individualized approach to intervention. Still, there is considerable debate over the existence of LDs subtypes as distinct categories that can be reliably identified, how best to categorize LDs subtypes, and whether instructional implications differ by subtype.

According to Fletcher et al. (2003), there are three main approaches to subtyping LDs: achievement subtypes, clinical inferential (rational) subtypes, and empirically based subtypes.

The first approach, achievement subtypes, relies on achievement testing profiles. For example, subgroups of reading difficulties are differentiated by performance on measures of word recognition, fluency, and comprehension. Subgroups of reading disability, math disability, and persons with a FSIQ score below 80 exhibit different patterns of cognitive attributes (Fletcher et al., 2003; Grigorenko, 2001). Compton et al. (2011) found distinctive patterns of strengths and weaknesses in abilities for several LD subgroups, in contrast to the flat pattern of cognitive and academic performance manifested by the normally achieving group. For example, students with LDs in reading comprehension showed a strength in math calculation alongside weaknesses in language involving listening comprehension, oral vocabulary, and syntax, whereas students with LDs in word reading showed strengths in math problem-solving and reading comprehension alongside weaknesses in working memory and oral language.

The second approach, clinical inferential, involves rationally defining subgroups based on clinical observations, typically by selecting individuals with similar characteristics. One example is students with a core deficit in phonological processing. The double-deficit model of subtypes distinguishes three subtypes: two subtypes with a single deficit in either phonological processing or rapid automatic naming, and a third subtype with a double-deficit in both areas (Wolf & Bowers, 1999; Wolf, Bowers, & Biddle, 2000). Another subtype model classifies two types of poor decoders: those having phonological dyslexia, orthographic/surface dyslexia, and those having mixed dyslexia, meaning weaknesses in both phonological and orthographic processing based on performance on reading nonwords and irregular exception words (Castles & Coltheart, 1993; Feifer & De Fina, 2000).

The third approach, empirically based subtypes of LDs, is based on multivariate empirical classification. It uses techniques such as Q-factor analysis and cluster analysis, and subsequent measures of external validity. Empirical subtyping models have been criticized for being atheoretical and unreliable; however, these models have provided additional support for rational subtyping methods, including the double-deficit model and differentiating garden-variety from specific reading disabilities (Fletcher et al., 2003). For example, a study by Pieters, Roeyers, Rosseel, Van Waelvelde, and Desoete (2013) used data-driven model-based clustering to identify two clusters of math disorder: one with number fact-retrieval weaknesses, and one with procedural calculation problems. When both motor and mathematical variables were included in the analysis, two clusters were identified: one with weaknesses in number fact retrieval, procedural calculation, as well as motor and visual-motor integration skills; a second with weaknesses in procedural calculation and visual-motor skills.

The identification and classification of a LD relies on either a dimensional or a categorical framework. Subtyping efforts are based on evidence that the heterogeneity within LDs is best represented as distinct subtypes. For example, reading and math LDs can be differentiated because students with reading LDs tend to have a relative strength in mathematics, whereas students with mathematics LDs tend to have a relative strength in reading (Compton et al., 2011). However, some researchers contend that the attributes of reading disability and math disability are dimensional, and efforts to categorize these as distinct subtypes are based on cut scores and correlated assessments (Branum–Martin, Fletcher, & Stuebing, 2013). Continued research is needed to advance our understanding of LD subtypes and their instructional implications for providing tailored intervention to a heterogeneous population of individuals with LDs.

Read full chapter

URL: 

https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128157442000094

The role of word-recognition accuracy in the development of word-recognition speed and reading comprehension in primary school: A longitudinal examination

Panagiotis Karageorgos, … Johannes Naumann, in Cognitive Development, 2020

3.3.1 Word-recognition accuracy and speed

Word-recognition accuracy and speed were assessed with the computerized lexical decision subtest of the ProDi-L test battery (Richter et al., 2017; see also Richter, Isberner, Naumann, & Kutzner, 2013). Children were presented with 16 words (e.g., Traktor [tractor]) and pseudowords (e.g., Spinfen) in randomized order. Their task was to decide whether the presented letter string was a real word or not by using two response keys (yes/no). Pseudowords were orthographically and phonologically legal letter strings and varied in their similarity to actual German words. Pseudowords similar to actual words were constructed by changing the first character of an existing word (e.g, NameBame). Pseudowords dissimilar to actual words were constructed by combining the syllables of two existing words with irregular spellings. For example, the pseudoword Chilance was constructed by combining the first syllable of the word Chili and the second and third syllables of the word Balance. The pseudowords also included pseudohomophones (1–3 per measurement point), which sound like real words but have a different orthographical form (e.g., Heckse instead of Hexe/witch). These items cannot be solved via the application of phoneme-grapheme translation rules but require direct word recognition via the lexical route. Seven items in the first measurement point, nine items in the second measurement point and eight items in the last two measurement points were regular and irregular real German words. Different but parallel words and pseudowords were used at all four measurement points. Apart from the slight difference in the proportions of words and pseudowords in the first and second item set (which was due to an error), the item sets were strictly parallelized according to the item features, mean accuracy and mean response time of each item set which were obtained in another cross-sectional study (Richter et al., 2013).

The word stimuli were systematically varied in frequency and number of orthographical neighbors. They had an average frequency of 1.25 (SD = .87), retrieved from the CELEX German lemma lexicon (metric: Mannheim written frequency, logarithmic; Baayen, Piepenbrock, & Gulikers, 1995; Baayen, Piepenbrock & van Rijn, 1993), an average length of 5.62 (SD = 1.56) characters and on average 1.75 (SD = 2.46) orthographical neighbours. The pseudowords were matched in length and frequency to the word stimuli. Pseudowords were based on words with an average frequency of 1.03 (SD = .66), they had an average length of 6.31 (SD = 2.16) characters and on average 1.69 (SD = 3.25) orthographical neighbours. In order to examine whether words and pseudowords differed in frequency, length, and orthographical neighbours across the measurement points we ran three separate analyses of variance. The results indicated no significant differences (for all comparisons, p > .17) between words and pseudowords across the measurement points. These results suggest that largely parallel items were used at each measurement point.

Following the ProDi-L manual, two criteria were applied to identify and remove outliers. Logarithmic latencies that were three standard deviations below or above the mean logarithmic latency for the item in the norming sample were coded as missing. The idea behind this criterion is that very short response times are likely to indicate an irregular response, such as clicking through items without reading them, and thus they should not be included in further analyses. Likewise, very long response times are likely due to disturbances, mind wandering, etc. Furthermore, for each child, response times that deviated more than two standard deviations from the average of the individual logarithmic response times were also coded as missing. Further data preparation was performed separately for each measurement point according to the procedure reported by Karageorgos et al. (2019). The sum of correct responses was transformed into proportions representing word recognition accuracy. Furthermore, a words-per-minute score was calculated as an indicator of word-recognition speed. The number of correct and incorrect responses to words and pseudowords was multiplied by 60 000 ms and then divided by the overall latency across all items measured in ms. A child, for example, who responded to 10 items in 10 000 ms received a score of 60 words per minute. Words-per-minute scores were not computed for participants with more than 10 % missing values (due to the outlier removal criteria discussed above) at the relevant measurement point. Thus, word-recognition speed scores were missing for 449 of 4380 data points. The test-retest reliability between measurement points was computed as the intraclass correlation of word-recognition scores at the end of each school year for a total of 692 children (those with complete data sets) using the R-package irr (Gamer, Lemon, Fellows, & Singh, 2019). A two-way mixed-effects model for mean rating and absolute agreement was used for computing the ICC (Koo & Li, 2016; McGraw & Wong, 1996; Price et al., 2015). According to the interpretation guidelines proposed by Ciccheti (1994), the estimated test-retest reliability (i.e., stability) was good for the accuracy score, ρI = .624, F(691, 32.9) = 3.48, p < 0.001, 95 % CI [.40, .75], and fair for the words-per-minute score, ρI = .50, F(543, 6.7) = 4.46, p = 0.005, 95 % CI [.01, .73].

Read full article

URL: 

https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0885201420301039

Intelligence, dual coding theory, and the brain

Allan Paivio, in Intelligence, 2014

3.1.1 Logogens and imagens

Morton’s (1979) word-recognition model uses modality-specific visual and auditory input logogens and output logogens to account for recognition performance. In DCT, the variety of modality-specific representations expanded to include auditory, visual, haptic, and motor logogens, as well as separate logogen systems for the different languages of multilingual individuals. Moreover, DCT treats logogens as hierarchical sequential structures of increasing length, from phonemes (or letters) to syllables, conventional words, fixed phrases, idioms, sentences, and longer discourse units – anything learned and remembered as an integrated language sequence. Imagens also are multimodal (visual, auditory, haptic, motor) representations organized hierarchically into spatial nested sets that are most apparent in visual objects – for example, we see pupils within eyes within faces within rooms within houses within larger scenes, and so on. Importantly, the modality–specificity of logogens and imagens excludes abstract mental representations such as propositions. Thus the functional domains associated with stimulus meaning and cognitive abilities are conceptualized entirely in terms of modality specific logogens and imagens.

Read full article

URL: 

https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0160289614001305

From Wikipedia, the free encyclopedia

Word recognition, according to Literacy Information and Communication System (LINCS) is «the ability of a reader to recognize written words correctly and virtually effortlessly». It is sometimes referred to as «isolated word recognition» because it involves a reader’s ability to recognize words individually from a list without needing similar words for contextual help.[1] LINCS continues to say that «rapid and effortless word recognition is the main component of fluent reading» and explains that these skills can be improved by «practic[ing] with flashcards, lists, and word grids».

In her 1990 review of the science of learning to read, psychologist Marilyn Jager Adams wrote that «the single immutable and nonoptional fact about skilful reading is that it involves relatively complete processing of the individual letters of print.»[2] The article «The Science of Word Recognition» says that «evidence from the last 20 years of work in cognitive psychology indicates that we use the letters within a word to recognize a word». Over time, other theories have been put forth proposing the mechanisms by which words are recognized in isolation, yet with both speed and accuracy.[3] These theories focus more on the significance of individual letters and letter-shape recognition (ex. serial letter recognition and parallel letter recognition). Other factors such as saccadic eye movements and the linear relationship between letters also affect the way we recognize words.[4]

An article in ScienceDaily suggests that «early word recognition is key to lifelong reading skills».[5] There are different ways to develop these skills. For example, creating flash cards for words that appear at a high frequency is considered a tool for overcoming dyslexia.[6] It has been argued that prosody, the patterns of rhythm and sound used in poetry, can improve word recognition.[7]

Word recognition is a manner of reading based upon the immediate perception of what word a familiar grouping of letters represents. This process exists in opposition to phonetics and word analysis, as a different method of recognizing and verbalizing visual language (i.e. reading).[8] Word recognition functions primarily on automaticity. On the other hand, phonetics and word analysis rely on the basis of cognitively applying learned grammatical rules for the blending of letters, sounds, graphemes, and morphemes.

Word recognition is measured as a matter of speed, such that a word with a high level of recognition is read faster than a novel one.[3] This manner of testing suggests that comprehension of the meaning of the words being read is not required, but rather the ability to recognize them in a way that allows proper pronunciation. Therefore, context is unimportant, and word recognition is often assessed with words presented in isolation in formats such as flash cards[8] Nevertheless, ease in word recognition, as in fluency, enables proficiency that fosters comprehension of the text being read.[9]

The intrinsic value of word recognition may be obvious due to the prevalence of literacy in modern society. However, its role may be less conspicuous in the areas of literacy learning, second-language learning, and developmental delays in reading. As word recognition is better understood, more reliable and efficient forms of teaching may be discovered for both children and adult learners of first-language literacy. Such information may also benefit second-language learners with acquisition of novel words and letter characters.[10] Furthermore, a better understanding of the processes involved in word recognition may enable more specific treatments for individuals with reading disabilities.

Theories[edit]

Bouma shape[edit]

Bouma shape, named after the Dutch vision researcher Herman Bouma, refers to the overall outline, or shape, of a word.[11] Herman Bouma discussed the role of «global word shape» in his word recognition experiment conducted in 1973.[12] Theories of bouma shape became popular in word recognition, suggesting people recognize words from the shape the letters make in a group relative to each other.[3] This contrasts the idea that letters are read individually. Instead, via prior exposure, people become familiar with outlines, and thereby recognize them the next time they are presented with the same word, or bouma.

The slower pace with which people read words written entirely in upper-case, or with alternating upper- and lower-case letters, supports the bouma theory.[3] The theory holds that a novel bouma shape created by changing the lower-case letters to upper-case hinders a person’s recall ability. James Cattell also supported this theory through his study, which gave evidence for an effect he called word superiority. This referred to the improved ability of people to deduce letters if the letters were presented within a word, rather than a mix of random letters. Furthermore, multiple studies have demonstrated that readers are less likely to notice misspelled words with a similar bouma shape than misspelled words with a different bouma shape.

Though these effects have been consistently replicated, many of their findings have been contested. Some have suggested that the reading ability of upper-case words is due to the amount of practice a person has with them. People who practice become faster at reading upper-case words, countering the importance of the bouma. Additionally, the word superiority effect might result from familiarity with phonetic combinations of letters, rather than the outlines of words, according to psychologists James McClelland and James Johnson.[13]

Parallel recognition vs. serial recognition[edit]

Parallel letter recognition is the most widely accepted model of word recognition by psychologists today.[3] In this model, all letters within a group are perceived simultaneously for word recognition. In contrast, the serial recognition model proposes that letters are recognized individually, one by one, before being integrated for word recognition. It predicts that single letters are identified faster and more accurately than many letters together, as in a word. However, this model was rejected because it cannot explain the word superiority effect, which states that readers can identify letters more quickly and accurately in the context of a word rather than in isolation.

Neural networks[edit]

A more modern approach to word recognition has been based on recent research on neuron functioning.[3] The visual aspects of a word, such as horizontal and vertical lines or curves, are thought to activate word-recognizing receptors. From those receptors, neural signals are sent to either excite or inhibit connections to other words in a person’s memory. The words with characters that match the visual representation of the observed word receive excitatory signals. As the mind further processes the appearance of the word, inhibitory signals simultaneously reduce activation to words in one’s memory with a dissimilar appearance. This neural strengthening of connections to relevant letters and words, as well as the simultaneous weakening of associations with irrelevant ones, eventually activates the correct word as part of word recognition in the neural network.

Physiological background[edit]

The brain[edit]

Using positron emission tomography (PET) scans and event-related potentials, researchers have located two separate areas in the fusiform gyrus that respond specifically to strings of letters. The posterior fusiform gyrus responds to words and non-words, regardless of their semantic context.[14] The anterior fusiform gyrus is affected by the semantic context, and whether letter combinations are words or pseudowords (novel letter combinations that mimic phonetic conventions, ex. shing). This role of the anterior fusiform gyrus may correlate to higher processing of the word’s concept and meaning. Both these regions are distinct from areas that respond to other types of complex stimuli, such as faces or colored patterns, and are part of a functionally specialized ventral pathway. Within 100 milliseconds (ms) of fixating on a word, an area of the left inferotemporal cortex processes its surface structure. Semantic information begins to be processed after 150 ms and shows widely distributed cortical network activation. After 200 ms, the integration of the different kinds of information occurs.[15]

The accuracy with which readers recognize words depends on the area of the retina that is stimulated.[16] Reading in English selectively trains specific regions of the left hemiretina for processing this type of visual information, making this part of the visual field optimal for word recognition. As words drift from this optimal area, word recognition accuracy declines. Because of this training, effective neural organization develops in the corresponding left cerebral hemisphere.[16]

Saccadic eye movements and fixations[edit]

Eyes make brief, unnoticeable movements called saccades approximately three to four times per second.[17] Saccades are separated by fixations, which are moments when the eyes are not moving. During saccades, visual sensitivity is diminished, which is called saccadic suppression. This ensures that the majority of the intake of visual information occurs during fixations. Lexical processing does, however, continue during saccades. The timing and accuracy of word recognition relies on where in the word the eye is currently fixating. Recognition is fastest and most accurate when fixating in the middle of the word. This is due to a decrease in visual acuity that results as letters are situated farther from the fixated location and become harder to see.[18]

Frequency effects[edit]

The word frequency effect suggests that words that appear the most in printed language are easier to recognize than words that appear less frequently.[19] Recognition of these words is faster and more accurate than other words. The word frequency effect is one of the most robust and most commonly reported effects in contemporary literature on word recognition. It has played a role in the development of many theories, such as the bouma shape. Furthermore, the neighborhood frequency effect states that word recognition is slower and less accurate when the target has an orthographic neighbor that is higher in frequency than itself. Orthographic neighbors are words of all the same length that differ by only one letter of that word.[19]

Real world applications[edit]

Inter-letter spacing[edit]

Serif fonts, i.e.: fonts with small appendages at the end of strokes, hinder lexical access. Word recognition is quicker with sans-serif fonts by an average of 8 ms.[20] These fonts have significantly more inter-letter spacing, and studies have shown that responses to words with increased inter-letter spacing were faster, regardless of word frequency and length.[21] This demonstrates an inverse relationship between fixation duration and small increases in inter-letter spacing,[22] most likely due to a reduction in lateral inhibition in the neural network.[20] When letters are farther apart, it is more likely that individuals will focus their fixations at the beginning of words, whereas default letter spacing on word processing software encourages fixation at the center of words.[22]

Tools and measurements[edit]

Both PET and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) are used to study the activation of various parts of the brain while participants perform reading-based tasks.[23] However, magnetoencephalography (MEG) and electroencephalography (EEG) provide a more accurate temporal measurement by recording event-related potentials each millisecond. Though identifying where the electrical responses occur can be easier with an MEG, an EEG is a more pervasive form of research in word recognition. Event-related potentials help measure both the strength and the latency of brain activity in certain areas during readings. Furthermore, by combining the usefulness of the event-related potentials with eye movement monitoring, researchers are able to correlate fixations during readings with word recognition in the brain in real-time. Since saccades and fixations are indicative of word recognition, electrooculography (EOG) is used to measure eye movements and the amount of time required for lexical access to target words. This has been demonstrated by studies in which longer, less common words induce longer fixations, and smaller, less important words may not be fixated on at all while reading a sentence.

Learning[edit]

According to the LINCS website, the role of word recognition results in differences between the habits of adults and the habits of children learning how to read.[8] For non-literate adults learning to read, many rely more on word recognition than on phonics and word analysis. Poor readers with prior knowledge concerning the target words can recognize words and make fewer errors than poor readers with no prior knowledge.[24] Instead of blending sounds of individual letters, adult learners are more likely to recognize words automatically.[8] However, this can lead to errors when a similarly spelled, yet different word, is mistaken for one the reader is familiar with. Errors such as these are considered to be due to the learner’s experiences and exposure. Younger and newer learners tend to focus more on the implications from the text and rely less on background knowledge or experience. Poor readers with prior knowledge utilize the semantic aspects of the word, whereas proficient readers rely on only graphic information for word recognition.[24] However, practice and improved proficiency tend to lead to a more efficient use of combining reading ability and background knowledge for effective word recognition.[8]

The role of the frequency effect has been greatly incorporated into the learning process.[8] While the word analysis approach is extremely beneficial, many words defy regular grammatical structures and are more easily incorporated into the lexical memory by automatic word recognition. To facilitate this, many educational experts highlight the importance of repetition in word exposure. This utilizes the frequency effect by increasing the reader’s familiarity with the target word, and thereby improving both future speed and accuracy in reading. This repetition can be in the form of flash cards, word-tracing, reading aloud, picturing the word, and other forms of practice that improve the association of the visual text with word recall.[25]

Role of technology[edit]

Improvements in technology have greatly contributed to advances in the understanding and research in word recognition. New word recognition capabilities have made computer-based learning programs more effective and reliable.[8] Improved technology has enabled eye-tracking, which monitors individuals’ saccadic eye movements while they read. This has furthered understanding of how certain patterns of eye movement increases word recognition and processing. Furthermore, changes can be simultaneously made to text just outside the reader’s area of focus without the reader being made aware. This has provided more information on where the eye focuses when an individual is reading and where the boundaries of attention lie.

With this additional information, researchers have proposed new models of word recognition that can be programmed into computers. As a result, computers can now mimic how a human would perceive and react to language and novel words.[8] This technology has advanced to the point where models of literacy learning can be digitally demonstrated. For example, a computer can now mimic a child’s learning progress and induce general language rules when exposed to a list of words with only a limited number of explanations. Nevertheless, as no universal model has yet been agreed upon, the generalizability of word recognition models and its simulations may be limited.[26]

Despite this lack of consensus regarding parameters in simulation designs, any progress in the area of word recognition is helpful to future research regarding which learning styles may be most successful in classrooms. Correlations also exist between reading ability, spoken language development, and learning disabilities. Therefore, advances in any one of these areas may assist understanding in inter-related subjects.[27] Ultimately, the development of word recognition may facilitate the breakthrough between «learning to read» and «reading to learn».[28]

References[edit]

  1. ^ «Assessment Strategies and Reading Profiles».
  2. ^ Adams, Marilyn Jager (1990). Beginning to read : thinking and learning about print. Cambridge: MIT Press. p. 105. ISBN 978-0-262-51076-9.
  3. ^ a b c d e f (Larsen, 2004)
  4. ^ «The Science of Word Recognition». Microsoft.
  5. ^ «Early Word Recognition Is Key To Lifelong Reading Skills Says New Study». www.sciencedaily.com. Retrieved 2017-01-09.
  6. ^ «Flash Card Word Recognition Skills for Dyslexia».
  7. ^ ftp://128.46.154.21/harper/muri/Chen_PDSR_SP04.pdf
  8. ^ a b c d e f g h (Kruidenier, 2002)
  9. ^ (Luckner & Urbach, 2012)
  10. ^ (Everson, 2011)
  11. ^ (Ranum, 1998)
  12. ^ (Bouma & Bouwhuis, 1979)
  13. ^ (McClelland & Johnston, 1977)
  14. ^ (Nobre, Truett & McCarthy, 1994)
  15. ^ (Hauk, Davis, Ford, Pulvermuller & Marslen-Wilson, 2006)
  16. ^ a b (Mishkin, Mortimer, Forgays & Donald, 1952)
  17. ^ (Irwin, 1998)
  18. ^ (Nazir, Heller & Sussman, 1992
  19. ^ a b (Grainger, 1990)
  20. ^ a b (Moret-Tatay & Perea, 2011)
  21. ^ (Pereaa, Moret-Tataya & Gomezc, 2011)
  22. ^ a b (Perea & Gomez 2012)
  23. ^ (Sereno & Rayner, 2003)
  24. ^ a b (Priebe, Keenan & Miller, 2010)
  25. ^ (Literacy Information and Communication System)
  26. ^ (Davis & Mermelstein, 1980)
  27. ^ (Scarborough, 2009)
  28. ^ (Campbell, Kelly, Mullis, Martin & Sainsbury, 2001, p.6)

Citations[edit]

  • Bouma, H., & Bouwhuis, D. (1979). Visual word recognition of three-letter words as derived from the recognition of the constituent letters» Perception & Psychophysics 25(1), 12-22. Retrieved from http://alexandria.tue.nl/repository/freearticles/734512.pdf
  • Campbell, J. R., Kelly, D. L., Mullis, I. V. S., Martin, M. O., & Sainsbury, M. (2001). Framework and specifications for pirls assessment 2001 . (2nd ed., p. 6). Chestnut Hill, MA, USA: International Study Center, Lynch School of Education, Boston College. Retrieved from http://timssandpirls.bc.edu/pirls2001i/pdf/PIRLS_frame2.pdf
  • Davis, S. B.; Mermelstein, P. (1980). «Comparison of parametric representations for monosyllabic word recognition in continuously spoken sentences». IEEE Transactions on Acoustics, Speech, and Signal Processing. 28 (4): 357–366. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.462.5073. doi:10.1109/tassp.1980.1163420.
  • Everson, M. E. (2011). «Word recognition among learners of Chinese as a foreign language: Investigating the relationship between naming and knowing». The Modern Language Journal. 82 (2): 194–204. doi:10.1111/j.1540-4781.1998.tb01192.x.
  • Grainger, J (1990). «Word frequency and neighborhood frequency effects in lexical decision and naming» (PDF). Journal of Memory and Language. 29 (2): 228–244. doi:10.1016/0749-596x(90)90074-a.
  • Hauk, O.; Davis, M. H.; Ford, M.; Pulvermuller, F.; Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (2006). «The time course of visual word recognition as revealed by linear regression analysis of erp data» (PDF). NeuroImage. 30 (4): 1383–1400. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.048. PMID 16460964. S2CID 17367093.
  • Irwin, D (1998). «Lexical processing during saccadic eye movements». Cognitive Psychology. 36 (1): 1–27. doi:10.1006/cogp.1998.0682. PMID 9679075. S2CID 25066325.
  • Kruidenier, K. (2002). Research-based principles for adult basic education reading instruction (Contract no. ED-01-PO-1037). Retrieved from National Institute for Literacy website: http://lincs.ed.gov/publications/pdf/adult_ed_02.pdf
  • Larsen, K. (2004, July). The science of word recognition. Advanced Reading Technology, Microsoft Corporation, Retrieved from http://www.microsoft.com/typography/ctfonts/wordrecognition.aspx
  • Literacy Information and Communication System. (n.d.). Print skills (alphabetics). Retrieved from http://lincs.ed.gov/readingprofiles/MC_Word_Recognition.htm
  • Luckner, J. L.; Urbach, J. (2012). «Reading fluency and students who are deaf or hard of hearing: Synthesis of the research». Communication Disorders Quarterly. 33 (4): 230–241. doi:10.1177/1525740111412582. S2CID 145617612.
  • McClelland, J. L.; Johnston, J. C. (1977). «The role of familiar units in perception of words and nonwords» (PDF). Perception & Psychophysics. 22 (3): 249–261. doi:10.3758/bf03199687. S2CID 144497014.
  • Mishkin, Mortimer; Forgays; Donald (1952). «Word recognition as a function or retinal locus». Journal of Experimental Psychology. 43 (1): 43–48. doi:10.1037/h0061361. PMID 14907990.
  • Moret-Tatay, C.; Perea, M. (2011). «Do serifs provide an advantage in the recognition of written words?» (PDF). Journal of Cognitive Psychology. 23 (5): 619–624. doi:10.1080/20445911.2011.546781. S2CID 8427809.
  • Nazir, T. A.; Heller, D.; Sussman, C. (1992). «Letter visibility and word recognition: The optimal viewing position in printed words». Perception and Psychophysics. 52 (3): 315–328. doi:10.3758/bf03209148. PMID 1408642.
  • Nobre, A.; Truett, A.; McCarthy, G. (1994). «Word recognition in the human inferior temporal lobe» (PDF). Nature. 372 (6503): 260–3. Bibcode:1994Natur.372..260N. doi:10.1038/372260a0. PMID 7969469. S2CID 11167389.
  • Perea, M; Gomez, P (2012). «Subtle Increases in Interletter Spacing Facilitate the Encoding of Words during Normal Reading». PLOS ONE. 7 (10): e47568. Bibcode:2012PLoSO…747568P. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0047568. PMC 3474730. PMID 23082178.
  • Pereaa, M.; Moret-Tataya, C.; Gomezc, P. (2011). «The effects of interletter spacing in visual-word recognition». Acta Psychologica. 137 (3): 345–351. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.389.137. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2011.04.003. PMID 21545978.
  • Priebe, S. J.; Keenan, J. M.; Miller, A. C. (2010). «How prior knowledge affects word identification and comprehension». Reading and Writing. 7: 581–6. doi:10.1007/s11145-010-9260-0. PMC 3142886. PMID 21799586.
  • Ranum, O. (1998). Paul saenger’s «space between words». Retrieved from http://www.ranumspanat.com/html pages/saenger.html
  • Scarborough, H. S. (2009). Connecting early language and literacy to later reading (dis)abilities: Evidence, theory, and practice. In F. Fletcher-Campbell, J. Soler & G. Reid (Eds.), Approaching difficulties in literacy development: Assessment, pedagogy and programmes (1 ed., pp. 23–38). Retrieved from https://books.google.com/books?id=sfKpsYBGX2MC&dq=reading&pg=PA23 delays and language&ots=rwlKKbBdkp&sig=CfBJDpub7IyfHyf9Et6B9TlpZNk
  • Sereno, S. C.; Rayner, K. (2003). «Measuring word recognition in reading: Eye movements and event-related potentials». Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 7 (11): 489–493. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.09.010. PMID 14585445. S2CID 7156916.

Hello,

I think I’ve read this request/feature in an older issue (can’t find it though),

I want my user to create his own phrase list and so
I want to make sure that my user added a valid word to the phrase list before initializing the recognizer.

And by valid word: a word that can be recognized, i.e a word included in the model’s vocabulary.
I am only talking about models with dynamic graphs as in #217 .

I wrote a simple function that searches the model’s symbol table with Find()
and returns >=0 if true or -1 otherwise.

Would you like to see my .diff file, and if its no trouble, add it to the API?

If it is included in the API it will save me alot of time rebuilding on every new update.

Thank you.

icon forward

The Facebook plugins can be recognized by the Facebook logo or the»Like-Button»(«Like») on our site.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

context icon

С улицы Толедо здание можно узнать по фасаду оранжевого цвета.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

The majority of Finland’s toponyms can be recognized having archaic or dialectal Finnish origins.

context icon

Большинство топонимов Финляндии можно признать имеющими архаическое или диалектное финское происхождение.

Based on the characterization by oriented trees, Ptolemaic graphs can be recognized in linear time.

context icon

Основываясь на описании ориентированными деревьями, птолемеевы графы можно распознать за линейное время.

The Facebook plugins can be recognized by the Facebook logo or the»Like-Button»(«Like») on our site.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

context icon

Плагины можно узнать по логотипу Facebook, например, кнопку Нравится.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

Depending on the actual circumstances, the token(tokens) can be recognized as an investment instrument.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

context icon

В зависимости от фактических обстоятельств, токен( токены) может быть признан как инвестиционный инструмент.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

context icon

Плотность вещества можно узнать по специальным физическим таблицам.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

It can be recognized by the characteristic yellow-brown, sometimes with a reddish tint, color.

context icon

Его можно узнать по характерной желто-коричневой, порой с красноватым оттенком, окраске.

Galfan coating can be recognized by its bright metallic and mildly cellular-patterned surface.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

context icon

Цинково- алюминиевое покрытие Galfan легко распознается по яркой металлической, чуть ячеистой, поверхности.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

Ranges simulation: the transponder can be recognized within a certain range for a specified read performance.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

context icon

Organizations can be recognized as international standard-setting bodies if they

are

open to all WTO members.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

context icon

Соответствующие организации могут быть признаны в качестве международных органов, занимающихся установлением стандартов, если они открыты для участия всех членов ВТО.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

Bodies can be recognized as international standard-setting bodies if they

are

open to all WTO members.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

context icon

Органы могут быть признаны в качестве международных органов по разработке стандартов, если они открыты для участия всех членов ВТО.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

The masters of Karate can be recognized by their black or red and white belts and call themselves»dan.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

context icon

Мастеров каратэ» дан» можно узнать по черным или бело- красным поясам.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

For the clinical diagnosis of mixed AD/VCI,

structural imaging, cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers, and glucose PET and amyloid PET imaging.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

context icon

Для клинической диагностики смешанной AD/ VCI,

построений, спинномозговой биомаркеров жидкости и глюкозы ПЭТ и амилоида ПЭТ.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

The graphs of clique-width three can be recognized, and a construction sequence found for them,

in polynomial time using an algorithm based on split decomposition.

context icon

за полиномиальное время с помощью алгоритма, основанного на расщепляемой декомпозиции.

Pupils and students study according to illegal programmes,

criteria and textbooks and

are

issued diplomas that neither

are

nor can be recognized.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

context icon

Учащиеся школ и студенты обучаются по незаконным программам, в соответствии

с незаконными критериями и по незаконным учебникам и получают дипломы, которые не признаются и не могут быть признаны.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

In interior design, Empire can be recognized through massive and simplified forms, and emphasized monumental furniture.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

context icon

В дизайне интерьера ампир можно распознать через массивные и упрощенные формы, и подчеркнутой монументальности мебели.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

Yet always- in any appearance- He can be recognized by His distinctive feature: a magnificent head of hair!

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

context icon

Но всегда- при любом облике- Его можно узнать по постоянному отличительному признаку: великолепной шевелюре!

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

Income can be recognized in both its constitutive and instrumental roles,

though in most cases, instrumentality

is

much more important than its intrinsic value.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

context icon

Можно признать, что доход выступает в качестве составной и инструментальной переменной,

хотя в большинстве случаев его инструментальная функция имеет гораздо большее значение, чем его имманентная ценность.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

oval shape, slightly narrowed posterior and characteristic protrusions on both sides of the body.

context icon

Эту разновидность клопов можно узнать по зеленовато-желтой окраске,

овальной форме, слегка зауженной кзади и характерным выступам с двух сторон туловища.

The vigilant person who really strives for ideals can be recognized by his efforts to uplift existing things on earth,

not in the intellectual sense of increasing power and position,

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

context icon

Человека, воистину стремящегося к Идеалу, можно распознать по тому, что он прилагает усилия к Возвышению всего сущего на Земле.

icon https://st.tr-ex.me/img/material-icons/svg/open_in_new/baseline.svg

Distance-hereditary graphs can be recognized, and parsed into a sequence of pendant vertex and twin operations,

in linear time.

context icon

Дистанционно- наследуемые графы могут быть распознаны и разложены на последовательность висячих вершин и операций удвоения

за линейное время.

Technical and infrastructural issues can be recognized and the effects of these on processes determined and brought under control.

context icon

Можно распознать технические и инфраструктурные ошибки, определить их влияние на процессы и держать их под контролем.

Results: 136,
Time: 0.0282

English

Russian

Russian

English

Понравилась статья? Поделить с друзьями:
  • Word call in japanese
  • Word call in french
  • Word call for help
  • Word calendars with holidays
  • Word bytes and bits