Definition. The Word Memory Test (WMT; Green, 2003; Green, Allen, & Astner, 1996) is a well-validated effort test. The WMT is a computerized test of the ability to learn a list of 20 word pairs. It takes about 7 min of the tester’s time and about 20 min of the patient’s time.
How do you test short term memory?
A short term memory test is very self-explanatory. The user is shown multiple digits of numbers and asked to remember them. Then, the numbers are taken off the screen and the user is asked to type the numbers they remembered in.
How many words can the average person remember?
On the other hand, a passive vocabulary refers to words we’ve assimilated but have not been able to use. According to lexicographer and dictionary expert Susie Dent, “the average active vocabulary of an adult English speaker is around 20,000 words, while his passive vocabulary is around 40,000 words.”
Is there a limit to human memory?
You might have only a few gigabytes of storage space, similar to the space in an iPod or a USB flash drive. Yet neurons combine so that each one helps with many memories at a time, exponentially increasing the brain’s memory storage capacity to something closer to around 2.5 petabytes (or a million gigabytes).
Can the human brain run out of memory?
In one sense, yes. Memory depends on forming new neural connections, and the brain has a finite number of neurones and a limited space in which to add more connections between them. Yet in another sense a healthy brain can never stop learning. There is really no such thing as ‘a memory’.
Can your brain ever be full?
In other words, can the brain be “full”? The answer is a resounding no, because, well, brains are more sophisticated than that. A study published in Nature Neuroscience earlier this year shows that instead of just crowding in, old information is sometimes pushed out of the brain for new memories to form.
Does our brain have a capacity?
Most computational neuroscientists tend to estimate human storage capacity somewhere between 10 terabytes and 100 terabytes, though the full spectrum of guesses ranges from 1 terabyte to 2.5 petabytes. (One terabyte is equal to about 1,000 gigabytes or about 1 million megabytes; a petabyte is about 1,000 terabytes.)
Assessment |
Biopsychology |
Comparative |
Cognitive |
Developmental |
Language |
Individual differences |
Personality |
Philosophy |
Social |
Methods |
Statistics |
Clinical |
Educational |
Industrial |
Professional items |
World psychology |
Social Processes:
Methodology ·
Types of test
The Word Memory Test was developed by Paul Green. It is a computer-based test that is designed to measure both verbal memory and biased responding (i.e. malingering). It measures memory on a number of dimensions and contains hidden scales, which serve to check the validity of the persons test scores.
See also
- Medical Symptom Validity Test
- Memory Complaints Inventory
- Nonverbal Medical Symptom Validity Test
References & Bibliography
Key texts
Books
Papers
- Drane, D., Williamson, D.J., Stroup, E.S., Holmes, M.D., Jung, M., Koerner, E., Chayter, N., Wilensky, A.J. & Miller. J.W. (2006) Impairment is not equal in patients with epileptic and psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsia, 47 (11) 1879-1886.
- Richman, J., Green, P. Gervais, R., Flaro, L., Merten, T., Brockhaus, R. Ranks, D. (2006) Objective Tests of Symptom Exaggeration in Independent Medical Examinations. Journal of Occupational & Environmental Medicine. 48(3):303-311, March 2006.
- Gervais, R., Rohling, M. Green, P. & Ford, W. (2004) A comparison of WMT, CARB and TOMM failure rates in non-head injury disability claimants. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19 (4) 475-487.
- Green, P. (in press) Spoiled for choice: Making comparisons between forced-choice effort tests. For K. Boone (Ed) Malingering of Neurocognitive Disorders. Guilford Press
- Green, P. (in press) Questioning common assumptions in depression. For J. Morgan & J. Sweet (Eds.). Neuropsychology of Malingering Casebook, New York: Taylor & Francis.
- Merten, T., Green, P., Henry, M., Blaskewitz, N., & Brockhaus, R. (2005) Analog validation of German language symptom validity tests and the influence of coaching. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 20, 6, 719-727.
- Green, P. & Flaro, L. (2003), Word Memory Test performance in children. Child Neuropsychology, 9, 3, 189-207
- Green, P., Rohling, M.L, Iverson, G. & Gervais, R. (2003) Relationships between olfactory discrimination and head injury severity. Brain Injury, 17 (6) 479-496
- Rohling, M.L., Green, P., Allen, L. & Iverson, G.L. (2002) Depressive symptoms and neurocognitive test scores in patients passing symptom validity tests. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 17 (3), 205-222
- Green, P. and Iverson, G. (2001) Validation of the Computerized Assessment of Response Bias in litigating patients with head injuries. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 15 (4), 492-497.
- Green, P, Rohling, ML, Lees-Haley, PR & Allen LM. (2001) Effort has a greater effect on test scores than severe brain injury in compensation claimants. Brain Injury, 15 (12) 1045-1060
- Green, P. & Iverson, G.L. (2001) Effects of injury severity and cognitive exaggeration on olfactory deficits in head injury compensation claims. Neurorehabilitation, 16, 237-243
- Green, P. (2001), Why clinicians often disagree about the validity of test results, Neurorehabilitation, 16, 231-236
- Green, P. (2001) Comment on article «Does pain confound interpretation of neuropsychological test results?» Neurorehabilitation, 16, 305-306
- Green, P., Iverson, G. & Allen, L. Detecting malingering in head injury litigation with the Word Memory Test. Brain Injury, 1999, 13 (10) 813-819
- Green, P., Gervais, R., & Merten, T. (2005) Das Memory Complaints Inventory (MCI): Gedächtnisstörungen, Beschwerdenschilderung und Leistungsmotivation [The Memory Complaints Inventory (MCI): Memory impairment, symptom presentation, and test effort.] Neurologie & Rehabilitation, 11, 3, 139-144
- Green, P. (2004) Testmotivation und ihre Messung. Reportpsychologie, 29, 5, 303-308
- Brockhaus, R. & Merten, T.(2004), «Neuropsychologische Diagnostik suboptimalen Leistungsverhaltens mit dem Word Memory Test», Nervenarzt, 75, (9), 882-887.
- Green, P., Lees-Haley, P.R. & Allen, L.M. (2003) The Word Memory Test and the validity of neuropsychological test scores. In J. Hom & R.L. Denney (Eds) Detection of Response Bias in Forensic Neuropsychology New York, Haworth Medical Press
- Green, P. & Josey, F. (2002) The use of an earplug to increase speech comprehension in a subgroup of children with learning disabilities: an experimental treatment. Applied Neuropsychology, 9 (1) 13-22
- Green, P., Lees-Haley, P.R. & Allen, L.M. (2002) The Word Memory Test and the validity of neuropsychological test scores. Journal of Forensic Neuropsychology, 2, 3 / 4, 97-124
- Rohling, M.L., Allen, L.M. & Green, P. (2002) Who is exaggerating cognitive impairment and who is not? CNS Spectrums, 7 (5), 387-395
- Iverson, GL, Lange, RT, Green, P & Franzen, MD. (2002) Detecting exaggeration and malingering with the Trail Making Test. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 16 (3) 398-406
- Ferrari, R., Obelieniene, D., Russell, A.S., Darlington, P., Gervais, R.O. & Green, P. (2002) Laypersons expectations of the sequelae of whiplash injury. A cross-cultural comparative study between Canada and Lithuania. Medical Science Monitor, 8 (11), 728-734
- Iverson, G., Turner, R.A. and Green, P. Predictive validity of WAIS-R VIQ-PIQ splits in persons with major depression. Journal of Clinical Psychology. 1999, 55 (4), 519-524
- Williamson, D, Rohling, M, Green, P & Allen, L. Evaluating effort with the Word Memory Test and Category Test Or not: Inconsistencies in a forensic sample. Accepted, Journal of Forensic Neuropsychology, 2001
- Iverson, G.L. & Green, P. (2001) Measuring improvement or decline on the WAIS-R in inpatient psychiatry. Psychological Reports, 89, 457-462
- Ferrari, R., Obelieniene, D., Russell, A.S., Darlington, P., Gervais, R.O. & Green, P. (2001) Symptom expectation after minor head injury. A comparative study between Canada and Lithuania. Clinical Neurology and Neurosurgery, 103, 184-190
- Gervais, R.O., Russell, A.S., Green, P., Ferrari, R. and Pieschl, S D. (2001) Effort testing in patients with fibromyalgia and disability incentives. Journal of Rheumatology, 28, 1892-1899.
- Slick, D.J., Iverson, G.L. & Green, P. California Verbal Learning Test indicators of suboptimal performance in a sample of head-injury litigants. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. Vol. 22 (5) 569-579, 2000.
- Gervais, R., Green, P., Allen, L.M. & Iverson, G. (2001). Effects of coaching on symptom validity testing in chronic pain patients presenting for disability assessments. Journal of Forensic Neuropsychology, 2 (2), 1-19.
-
- Iverson, G., Green, P. & Gervais, R. (1999) Using the Word Memory Test to detect biased responding in head injury litigation. The Journal of Cognitive Rehabilitation, 17 (2), 4-8,
Additional material
Books
Papers
- Google Scholar
External links
WMT website
Memory |
---|
Types of memory |
Articulatory suppression | Auditory memory | Autobiographical memory | Collective memory | Early memories | Echoic Memory | Eidetic memory | Episodic memory | Episodic-like memory | Explicit memory |Exosomatic memory | False memory |Flashbulb memory | Iconic memory | Implicit memory | Institutional memory | Long term memory | Music-related memory | Procedural memory | Prospective memory | Repressed memory | Retrospective memory | Semantic memory | Sensory memory | Short term memory | Spatial memory | State-dependent memory | Tonal memory | Transactive memory | Transsaccadic memory | Verbal memory | Visual memory | Visuospatial memory | Working memory | |
Aspects of memory |
Childhood amnesia | Cryptomnesia |Cued recall | Eye-witness testimony | Memory and emotion | Forgetting |Forgetting curve | Free recall | Levels-of-processing effect | Memory consolidation |Memory decay | Memory distrust syndrome |Memory inhibition | Memory and smell | Memory for the future | Memory loss | Memory optimization | Memory trace | Mnemonic | Memory biases | Modality effect | Tip of the tongue | Lethologica | Memory loss |Priming | Primacy effect | Reconstruction | Proactive interference | Prompting | Recency effect | Recall (learning) | Recognition (learning) | Reminiscence | Retention | Retroactive interference | Serial position effect | Serial recall | Source amnesia | |
Memory theory |
Atkinson-Shiffrin | Baddeley | CLARION | Decay theory | Dual-coding theory | Interference theory |Memory consolidation | Memory encoding | Memory-prediction framework | Forgetting | Recall | Recognition | |
Mnemonics |
Method of loci | Mnemonic room system | Mnemonic dominic system | Mnemonic learning | Mnemonic link system |Mnemonic major system | Mnemonic peg system | [[]] |[[]] | |
Neuroanatomy of memory |
Amygdala | Hippocampus | prefrontal cortex | Neurobiology of working memory | Neurophysiology of memory | Rhinal cortex | Synapses |[[]] | |
Neurochemistry of memory |
Glutamatergic system | of short term memory | [[]] |[[]] | [[]] | [[]] | [[]] | [[]] |[[]] | |
Developmental aspects of memory |
Prenatal memory | |Childhood memory | Memory and aging | [[]] | [[]] | |
Memory in clinical settings |
Alcohol amnestic disorder | Amnesia | Dissociative fugue | False memory syndrome | False memory | Hyperthymesia | Memory and aging | Memory disorders | Memory distrust syndrome Repressed memory Traumatic memory | |
Retention measures |
Benton | CAMPROMPT | Implicit memory testing | Indirect tests of memory | MAS | Memory tests for children | MERMER | Rey-15 | Rivermead | TOMM | Wechsler | WMT | WRAML2 | |
Treating memory problems |
CBT | EMDR | Psychotherapy | Recovered memory therapy |Reminiscence therapy | Memory clinic | Memory training | Rewind technique | |
Prominant workers in memory|- |
Baddeley | Broadbent |Ebbinghaus | Kandel |McGaugh | Schacter | Treisman | Tulving | |
Philosophy and historical views of memory |
Aristotle | [[]] |[[]] |[[]] |[[]] | [[]] | [[]] | [[]] | |
Miscellaneous |
Journals | Learning, Memory, and Cognition |Journal of Memory and Language |Memory |Memory and Cognition | [[]] | [[]] | [[]] | |
A list of ten unrelated words are orally presented one by one, and subjects are instructed to recall as many items as possible immediately after their presentation (immediate free recall, the traditional span task) and after a predetermined time, in general 5 to 10 minutes (delayed free recall).
What does a cognitive score of 10 mean?
According to the frequently asked questions section of the MoCA website, the following result ranges may indicate cognitive impairment: 18–25 points: Mild cognitive impairment. 10–17 points: Moderate cognitive impairment. Fewer than 10 points: Severe cognitive impairment.
What is a normal score on a memory test?
A score of 26 and higher is considered normal. In the initial study data, normal controls had an average score of 27.4. People with mild cognitive impairment (MCI) scored an average of 22.1. People with Alzheimer’s disease had an average score of 16.2.
What questions are asked on a memory test?
The MMSE includes questions that measure:
- Sense of date and time.
- Sense of location.
- Ability to remember a short list of common objects and later, repeat it back.
- Attention and ability to do basic math, like counting backward from 100 by increments of 7.
- Ability to name a couple of common objects.
How does the word memory test work?
The WMT is a computerized test of the ability to learn a list of 20 word pairs. It takes about 7 min of the tester’s time and about 20 min of the patient’s time. The examinee is instructed to watch and remember a list of 20 semantically related word pairs (e.g., dog/cat, man/woman, pig/bacon, fish/fin).
Simple word memory test may point to early signs of dementia — Dr Davide Bruno
What does it mean if you fail a memory test?
If your test results were not normal, it means you have some problem with memory or other mental function. But it won’t diagnose the cause. Your health care provider may need to do more tests to find out the reason. Some types of cognitive impairment are caused by treatable medical conditions.
What are the 3 words on a memory test?
Recall of Three Little Words Helps Quick Diagnosis of Mild Cognitive Impairment. ATLANTA, Nov. 19 — Mild cognitive impairment can be determined in less than five minutes with a three-word memory test and a clock-drawing task, according to researchers here. The words are apple, penny, and table.
What is a memory test for seniors?
The Self-Administered Gerocognitive Exam (SAGE) is a brief self-administered cognitive screening instrument used to identify mild cognitive impairment (MCI) from any cause and early dementia.
What is the 30 question test for dementia?
The Mini–Mental State Examination (MMSE) or Folstein test is a 30-point questionnaire that is used extensively in clinical and research settings to measure cognitive impairment. It is commonly used in medicine and allied health to screen for dementia.
Why would a doctor order a memory test?
These exams can determine if you are showing signs that line up with the symptoms of dementia. Once your doctor has a combination of test results, along with your detailed medical history, they should have the evidence needed to give you an official diagnosis.
How do you pass a memory test?
Memory Techniques for Exam Preparation: 10 Astonishing Ways to Harness the Power of Your Brain
- Get organised. …
- Mind palaces. …
- Mnemonics. …
- Rhyming. …
- Making the most of a photographic memory. …
- Setting facts and figures to music. …
- Experience things practically. …
- Utilise your sense of smell.
What is the clock test for dementia?
The clock-drawing test is a quick way to screen for early dementia, including Alzheimer’s disease. It involves drawing a clock on a piece of paper with numbers, clock hands, and a specific time. The inability to do so is a strong indication of mental decline.
How does a neurologist test for dementia?
Brain scans.
These tests can identify strokes, tumors, and other problems that can cause dementia. Scans also identify changes in the brain’s structure and function. The most common scans are: Computed tomography (CT), which uses X-rays to produce images of the brain and other organs.
What is the passing score for cognitive test?
A score of 30 is a very low score, a performance similar to the lowest 2% of all candidates globally. A score of 50 marks a performance better than or equal to 50% of all candidates. A score of 70 marks a performance better or equal to 98% of all candidates.
What is the passing score for cognitive evaluation?
Exam Results
Examination results are reported on a scale of 200 to 800 and an overall scaled score of 500 or greater is required to pass.
What is the average score on a cognitive test?
What is the average cognitive score? The average cognitive Scaled Score is 250, which translates to a raw score of 20. This average score was established during assessment development, using what’s called a “Reference Group.” That group, on average, scored 19.8 points out of a possible 50, which translates to 250.
What are the 3 D’s of dementia?
Understanding the Three D’s: Dementia, Delirium and Depression — For Health Care Professionals.
Can I give myself a dementia test?
The Self-Administered Gerocognitive Exam, known as SAGE, is a brief, pen-and-paper cognitive assessment tool designed to detect the early signs of cognitive, memory, or thinking impairments. The test evaluates your thinking abilities.
What is the 5 minute test for early dementia?
The five-minute cognitive test (FCT) was designed to capture deficits in five domains of cognitive abilities, including episodic memory, language fluency, time orientation, visuospatial function, and executive function.
Does Medicare require a memory test?
Detecting cognitive impairment is a required element of Medicare’s Annual Wellness Visit (AWV).
What are the first signs of memory loss?
Memory loss and dementia
- Asking the same questions repeatedly.
- Forgetting common words when speaking.
- Mixing words up — saying «bed» instead of «table,» for example.
- Taking longer to complete familiar tasks, such as following a recipe.
- Misplacing items in inappropriate places, such as putting a wallet in a kitchen drawer.
What are signs of cognitive decline?
Symptoms
- You forget things more often.
- You miss appointments or social events.
- You lose your train of thought. …
- You have trouble following a conversation.
- You find it hard to make decisions, finish a task or follow instructions.
- You start to have trouble finding your way around places you know well.
What are some memory words?
- recollection.
- memorial.
- reminiscence.
- recall.
- remembrance.
- anamnesis.
- token.
- reminder.
What is the average amount of words a person can remember?
The average is about 7 items, plus or minus 2, depending on the individual. You can easily confirm this by reading someone a series of words that have no connection with one another, then asking this person to repeat them.
What are the good marks of memory?
Marks of Good Memory: The marks of good memory, according to Stout, are ease and rapidity of learning or memorizing, permanence of retention, rapidity of actual revival, accuracy of the actual recall, or and serviceableness of the revival or its relevance to purpose.
Sleep Disorders Part I
Hawley E. Montgomery-Downs, David Gozal, in Handbook of Clinical Neurology, 2011
Memory
Following acute sleep restriction, no deficits are usually apparent in word memory tasks (Carskadon et al., 1981a), yet such deficits emerge following 38 hours of sleep deprivation in a sample of similar-aged children (Carskadon et al., 1981b). Performance of a verbal memory task was unaffected by acute sleep restriction (Randazzo et al., 1998a) and 3 nights of restricted sleep in children aged 10–14 years did not suggest the presence of any deficits on a working-memory task (Randazzo et al., 1998b). However, in children with OSA, memory performance on standardized psychometric tests is significantly affected compared with control children (Rhodes et al., 1995; Blunden et al., 2000), with children with higher respiratory disturbance indices showing greater memory deficits (Rhodes et al., 1995). These findings are not consistently reported; neither Owens-Stively and colleagues (1997) nor O’Brien and colleagues (2004a, b) found any differences in memory performance in children with varying degrees of OSA severity when compared to control children. The limited number of studies in this area and the contradictory results emphasize the need for further investigation.
Read full chapter
URL:
https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780444520067000320
Psychometric Concerns
Mary Pepping Ph.D., ABPP-CN, in Successful Private Practice in Neuropsychology and Neuro-Rehabilitation (Second Edition), 2015
My Typical Comprehensive Battery
-
TOMM with immediate, delay and recognition trials, or the Word Memory Test
-
Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS-IV): full test, including Comprehension and Picture Completion subtests
-
Portions of the Wechsler Memory Scale-IV (WMS-IV): Logical Memory I/II, Verbal Paired Associates I/II, Visual Reproduction I/II
-
California Verbal Learning Test-2 (CVLT-2), or Buschke Selective Reminding Test (SRT)-Form 1 or Form 3, or Fuld Object Memory Test (Fuld), Form I or II
-
Orientation and Mental Control subtests from the Wechsler Memory Scale-Revised (WMS-R)
-
Color-Word Subtest from the Delis Kaplan Executive Function System (DKEFS)
-
Aphasia Screening Test
-
Boston Naming Test
-
Letter Fluency and Animal Fluency tests
-
Complex Ideational Material Subtest from the Boston Diagnostic
-
Aphasia Exam (BDAE:CIMS), including inference questions
-
Wide Range Achievement Test-Form 3 (WRAT-3)
-
Trail Making Tests Parts A and B (Trails)
-
Rey Osterrieth Complex Figure with 30 min delay
-
Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST)
-
Halstead Category Test
-
Tower Test from the DKEFS
-
Tactual Performance Test (TPT)
-
Finger Tapping Test
-
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 (MMPI-2) or MMPI-Adolescent Version (MMPI-A)
-
Frontal Systems Behavioral Scale (FrSBe)—patient and relative forms
It is also not uncommon for me to supplement for further exploration of a particular domain. For example, with attention, I might add-in the Brief Test of Attention or the Ruff 2 and 7, or the Paced Auditory Serial Addition Test. To explore reading comprehension skills, I might have the patient complete the comprehension subtest from the WRAT-IV or use the Gates-MacGinitie Reading Comprehension Test.
Read full chapter
URL:
https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780128002582000104
Obstructive Sleep Apnea and the Central Nervous System
Ivana Rosenzweig, … Mary J. Morrell, in Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine (Sixth Edition), 2017
Memory
Behavior
-
Decreased ability to register, store, retain, and retrieve information
Measures Commonly Used to Assess Deficit
-
Short-term memory: timed tasks of up to 10 minutes that require free recall of words, numbers, paragraphs, or figures
- •
-
Probed, Recall Memory Task (words)
- •
-
Digit Span Forward (numbers)
- •
-
Wechsler Memory Scale Story Task (paragraph)
- •
-
Rey Auditory-Verbal Learning Test (figure)
-
Long-term memory: presenting the subject with lists of items that are longer than the seven-item memory capacity
- •
-
California Verbal Learning Test
-
Procedural memory: gradual acquisition and maintenance of motor skills and procedures
- •
-
Mirror Tracing Task
- •
-
Rotary Pursuit Task
Read full chapter
URL:
https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780323242882001173
Assessing Performance Validity with the ACS
James A. Holdnack, … Grant L. Iverson, in WAIS-IV, WMS-IV, and ACS, 2013
External/Stand-Alone Validity Checks
External or stand-alone PVTs are developed solely to measure invalid performance, that is, they are not used to measure any other construct. These tests are designed to be easily performed by patients with significant cognitive impairment. In past surveys, the most common stand alone performance validity measures used by experts include: the Test of Memory Malingering, Rey 15-Item, and Recognition Memory Test (Slick et al., 2004) and clinical neuropsychologists most commonly use the Test of Memory Malingering and Rey 15-Item (Sharland & Gfeller, 2007). Other PVTs shown to be sensitive in differentiating invalid from valid performance include: Word Memory Test, Medical Symptom Validity Test, Victoria Symptom Validity Test, Portland Digit Recognition, Dot Counting Test, Digit Memory Test and 21-Item Test (Iverson, 2010; Mossman, Wygant, & Gervais, 2012; Strauss et al., 2002; Vickery, Berry, Inman, Harris, & Orey, 2001). Many external PVTs use a forced choice memory paradigm. The examinee is given multiple stimuli to remember, in some cases the number of items to be remembered appears to be quite large, or the nature of the stimuli appears complex and difficult to memorize. In some models, immediately after the examinee sees one of the stimuli, he or she is asked to identify the item to be recalled from a foil. In other tasks, the examinee is presented with all of the stimuli and then asked to identify the target stimuli from a foil. In all cases, the foil is different enough from the original stimuli to be easily identifiable as incorrect. The sensitivity of identifying potential invalid performance in forced choice memory tasks differs with different stimuli, with word stimuli superior to digits and pictures (Gervais, Rohling, Green, & Ford, 2004). Even though these types of forced choice memory tasks appear challenging to the examinee, they are in actuality quite easy—yielding a high level of performance in clinical samples.
Read full chapter
URL:
https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780123869340000079
Language Impairment
R.H. Fitch, in Neural Circuit Development and Function in the Brain, 2013
40.3.4 Short-Term/Working Memory Deficits
Evidence has shown consistent evidence of deficits in language processing using working and/or verbal short-term memory (STM) to process sentence morphology, syntax, and/or semantics in LD populations (Brady et al., 1983; Shankweiler and Crain, 1986; Shankweiler et al., 1995; Smith et al., 1989). Clearly, the effective use of language requires the use of STM to hold letter sounds in memory during word processing (prior to combining the sounds into a meaningful word), as well as in processing complex semantic meaning within sentences, paragraphs, and narratives (e.g., the meaning of later parts of a sentence may be modified by early sentence structure that must be held in STM to fully interpret the complete sentence). Tasks that tap these underlying capacities include digit-span recall tasks, word-memory tasks, and more complex assessments of active narrative comprehension during reading (see Montgomery et al., 2010, for review). Evidence for STM deficits specific to processing phonological information, such as is required for repeating nonwords, has also been reviewed (Gathercole and Baddeley, 1990; see also Catts et al., 2005). In a recent review by Briscoe and Rankin (2009), the authors discuss data dissociating deficits specific to the ‘phonological memory loop’ versus more generalized deficits in executive working memory systems in SLI subjects, concluding that evidence more strongly supports a deficit in core phonological memory processes (phonological loop) as opposed to overall executive memory. However, these assertions are counted by findings such as those of Smith-Spark and Fisk (2007), who demonstrated deficits in cross-modal executive working memory, as well as phonological memory, in dyslexics.
An intriguing family-based genetic association study has also shown a genetic–behavioral linkage within a subset of dyslexic individuals, with deficits in STM appearing to correspond to variations in a segment of the dyslexia-risk gene DYX1C1 (Marino et al., 2007). Specifically, a significant linkage was observed between single-letter backward-span scores and a genetic variant within this segment of the DYX1C1 gene. Further evidence suggests associations between memory and/or attentional difficulties and anomalies in other dyslexia-risk genes such as DCDC2 (Berninger et al., 2008). These collective results support the view that ongoing genetic research into LD may eventually reveal a correspondence between specific genes (out of multiple LD risk genes that have been, and are likely yet to be, identified) and specific core functional deficits contributing to LD (e.g., STM).
Read full chapter
URL:
https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780123972675001163
Assessment of Cognitive Training
George W. Rebok, … Adam P. Spira, in Handbook of Assessment in Clinical Gerontology (Second Edition), 2010
Tools and Techniques for Initial Assessment of Memory and Cognition in Late Adulthood
Before selecting or implementing a cognitive training program, the clinician should assess the client’s neuropsychological, physical, and psychological status, as well as other contextual and life-event information. A full history is needed because the nature and severity of the cognitive impairment can render particular treatment options inappropriate. A patient’s lifestyle or personal preferences might also determine the course of treatment.
Memory Assessments
There are many neuropsychological tools and techniques available not only for evaluating effects of memory training on clients, but also for determining the need for specific kinds of training. Table 8.1 lists the major types of memory and a few of the most common measures used to assess each type, but it is by no means comprehensive. For a more complete treatment of neuropsychological assessment, please refer to Lezak (2004) or Craik and Tulving (2000). Neuropsychologists typically test several different memory domains in order to best understand the nature and severity of memory impairment because different memory abilities may show differential age decline and responsiveness to training. For instance, a patient with early signs of dementia will show deficits on delayed recall components of verbal memory tests, but their prospective memory might be relatively unaffected (Albert, 2008).
Table 8.1. Memory Domains and Measures
Memory Type | Description | Measures |
---|---|---|
Working memory | Brain’s workbench where environmental stimuli are encoded | Brown-Peterson Technique (Peterson & Peterson, 1959) |
Telephone Test (Crook, Ferris, McCarthy, & Rae, 1980) | ||
Serial Digit Learning (Benton et al., 1994) | ||
Digit Span Test (Wechsler, 1987) | ||
Episodic memory | Memory for new learning | California Verbal Learning Task (Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober, 1987) |
Hopkins Verbal Learning Task-Revised (Brandt & Benedict, 2001 | ||
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test (Rey, 1941) | ||
Logical Memory Test (Wechsler, 1987) | ||
Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test (Wilson, Cockburn, & Baddeley, 1985) | ||
Semantic memory | Memory for meanings | Word Memory Test (Green, Allen, & Astner, 1996) |
Portland Digit Recognition Test (Binder, 1993) | ||
Procedural memory | Memory for processes | Lexical Decision Task (Meyer & Schvaneveldt, 1971) |
Implicit Association Test (Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwarz, 1998) | ||
Word Stem Completion (Light, 1991) | ||
Prospective memory | Memory for future events | Cambridge Prospective Memory Test (Wilson et al., 2005) |
The term “working memory” refers to the short-term storage and manipulation of information (Baddeley, 1990). Neuropsychological tests that assess working memory include serial digit learning (Benton et al., 1994) and digit span backwards (Wechsler, 1987) tests. In contrast, memory for information that is retrieved after a longer time frame (e.g., over several days or years) is often referred to as long-term memory. Long-term memory can be further divided into declarative (explicit) and procedural (implicit) memory.
Episodic memory falls under the category of declarative long-term memory, and is most easily assessed with a long-delay recall trial from any word-list learning test such as the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test–Revised (Brandt & Benedict, 2001). This type of memory best discriminates persons with Alzheimer’s disease from cognitively normal older adults because a defining feature of the disease is rapid information loss after only brief delays. Once a person has dementia, however, severity cannot be measured with this type of assessment because episodic memory is impaired early in the disease course (Albert, 2008).
Semantic memory is also a form of declarative memory and refers to memory for meanings using general knowledge about the world. Semantic memory can be assessed through recognition tests such as the Portland Digit Recognition test (Binder, 1993) or the Word Memory Test (Green, Allen, & Astner, 1996). Because semantic memory, unlike episodic memory, does not decline with age (Craik, 1977; Zacks & Hasher, 2006), strategies that take advantage of such existing abilities may be easier for older adults to learn with training.
Procedural (implicit) memory is a type of long-term memory for skills such as riding a bicycle or typing on a keyboard that one may not consciously think about. Tests of procedural memory include the Implicit Association Task (Greenwald, McGhee, & Schwarz, 1998) and Lexical Decision Task (Meyer & Schvaneveldt, 1971).
Prospective memory is memory for future events, such as performing a task at a certain time (e.g., attending a doctor’s appointment on Tuesday, after lunch). Although prospective memory may be assessed fairly easily (such as by asking a patient to call your office in four days), validated assessments, such as the Cambridge Prospective Memory Test (Wilson et al., 2005), are also available.
Consideration of Table 8.1 is only the beginning of a clinician’s decision-making process about memory assessment. As some tasks are more burdensome and time-consuming than others, a clinician should consider how much time they have to allocate for assessments. For example, verbal memory tests such as the Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT; Rey, 1941) or the California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT; Delis, Kramer, Kaplan, & Ober, 2000) have good psychometric properties (reliability, validity), and are well-established tests. However, they take longer to administer compared with the Hopkins Verbal Learning Test-Revised (HVLT-R; Brandt & Benedict, 2001). The HVLT-R also has well-established reliability and validity, and is well tolerated, even by significantly impaired individuals. Finally, it provides environmental support for memory through semantic categories that can serve as cues for memory recall.
Other Cognitive Assessments
In addition to assessing memory function, it is equally important to assess related cognitive constructs (e.g., attention, perception, language, executive functioning) as these abilities are often related to memory performance. Further, certain cognitive impairments can limit the options for memory training. For instance, many memory-training programs, such as the ACTIVE memory training intervention, focus on attention and concentration skills (Jobe et al., 2001). Therefore, an individual demonstrating pre-existing impairment in these cognitive domains may not have the skills and abilities to adhere to, or potentially benefit from, a memory training intervention (Cahn-Weiner, Malloy, Rebok, & Ott, 2003; Unverzagt et al., 2007).
Table 8.2 presents some common assessments for cognitive domains other than memory, as well as general cognitive status. General or global cognitive status is measured by tests such as the Mini-Mental State Exam (MMSE; Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975) that assess multiple cognitive domains, using domain-specific cognitive items. For instance, the MMSE includes items that separately assess verbal memory, visuospatial ability, and orientation. Measures of general cognition can be a quick and efficient way to assess a patient’s cognitive state, but are not as sensitive as domain-specific measures of cognition in identifying particular cognitive deficits (Leroi, Sheppard, & Lyketsos, 2002; Lyketsos, Chen, & Anthony, 1999). Additionally, global measures are subject to ceiling effects among cognitively intact older adults (Leroi et al., 2002).
Table 8.2. Cognitive Domains and Measures
Cognitive Domain | Description | Measures |
---|---|---|
Global cognition | General cognitive status | Mini-Mental Status Exam (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh., 1975) |
Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (Brandt et al., 1988) | ||
Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument (Teng et al., 1994) | ||
WAIS-III (Wechsler, 1987) | ||
Attention | Selective concentration | Brief Test of Attention (Schretlen, 1989) |
Digit Symbol Substitution Test (Wechsler, 1987) | ||
Paced Auditory Serial Attention Test (Crawford, Obonsawin, & Allan, 1998) | ||
Digit Span Forward Test (Wechsler, 1987) | ||
Visuospatial or perceptual ability | Capacity to transform visual and spatial stimuli | Rey-Osterrieth Complex Figure Draw (Osterrieth, 1944; Woodrome & Fastenau, 2005) |
Brief Visuospatial Memory Test-revised (Benedict, 1997) | ||
Benton Line Orientation (Warrington & Rabin, 1970) | ||
Clock Drawing Task (Shulman, 2000) | ||
Pattern Comparison (Salthouse & Babcock, 1991) | ||
Language | Verbal fluency and expression of speech | Boston Naming Test (Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, 1983) |
Verbal Fluency Test (Harrison et al., 2000) | ||
Executive function | Shifting | Trail-Making Test (Armitage, 1946) |
Wisconsin Card Sort Test (Berg, 1948) | ||
Inhibition | Stroop Test (Kindt, Biermanm, & Brosschot, 1996) | |
Wisconsin Card Sort Test (Berg, 1948) | ||
Planning/problem solving | Candle Problem (Duncker, 1945) |
Tests of attention are typically tasks that measure an individual’s ability to concentrate and sustain focus over time and ignore irrelevant stimuli. Examples include the Brief Test of Attention (Schretlen, 1989) and the Digit Symbol Substitution task (Wechsler, 1987). Low performance on such tasks implies problems with sustained attention. Such an impairment could obviously affect performance on other cognitive tasks, such as those involving memory. Patients with mild dementia show little impairment when given simple attention tasks like the Digit Span Forward test (Wechsler, 1987), but impairment is more apparent as tasks become increasingly more complex.
Tests of visuospatial or perceptual ability include the Pattern Comparison and Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure Draw tasks (Osterrieth, 1944; Salthouse & Babcock, 1991). They test the mental capacity to remember or mentally manipulate visual and spatial stimuli, and deficits may suggest neurological injury, presence of dementia, or an agnosia of some sort (Colcombe & Kramer, 2003).
Tests of language ability, such as the Verbal Fluency Test (Harrison et al., 2000) or the Boston Naming Test (Kaplan, Goodglass, & Weintraub, 1983), assess the verbal production and expression of speech. Semantic and phonemic fluency are two types of fluency measured by the Verbal Fluency Test (Harrison et al., 2000). Semantic or category fluency is tested by giving the patient a semantic category upon which to draw, such as animals. Phonemic fluency taps into memory functions but also requires executive information processing functions to initiate and maintain systematic memory search strategies for information given an ambiguous cue. Both types of fluency are predictive of dementia (Chan, Butters, Salmon, & Maloney, 1993; Henry, Crawford, & Phillips, 2004).
Finally, executive abilities are a broad category of cognitive functions that encompasses cognitive control and coordination of multiple cognitive capacities (Duncan, Emslie, Williams, Johnson, & Freer, 1996). Examples include set-shifting and multi-tasking (Kramer & Madden, 2008; Salthouse, Hambrick, Lukas, & Dell, 1996), problem solving, inhibition (Metcalfe & Mischel, 1999; Salthouse & Meinz, 1995), and goal-directed behaviors like meal preparation and shopping (Kramer, Hahn, & Gopher, 1999; Kray, Eber, & Lindenberger, 2004).
Read full chapter
URL:
https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/B9780123749611100089
Traumatic Brain Injury: New Directions and Treatment Approaches
Paul Green PhD, in Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation Clinics of North America, 2007
Word Memory Test subtest scores by range of effort
The mean WMT DR score of those in the second effort range from the top was 88%, and of those in this range, 58% of cases failed the WMT using the standard clinical criteria. In Table 23, we can see that the group scoring in the second range on WMT effort subtests obtained a mean score of 66.9% on WMT MC. When an MC score that low occurs, the WMT Windows program [3] issues a “caution,” because in most patients, such a score would be too low to be valid. A score of 66.9% on MC would be 4.25 standard deviations below the mean of 95.4% correct (SD = 6.7) from normal adult controls listed in the WMT Windows test manual and program [3]. Neurological patients, selected because they had impaired memory on the CVLT, scored 81% (SD = 11) on MC, and a group who had severe brain injuries and an average GCS of 5 scored 88.2% correct (SD = 18.4) on MC. Similarly, those who scored in the range 81% to 90% on the WMT effort measures also scored 61% on the Paired Associates subtest, which is 3.3 standard deviations below the mean from normal adult controls (Table 24). An MC score of 66.9% or a PA score of 61% could be valid in a case of dementia or in someone who has a left medial temporal lobe brain tumor or lobectomy; however, the vast majority of cases in the current study with WMT effort scores in the second range from the top did not have dementia and did not need 24-hour-a-day care. Hence their mean scores of 66.9% on MC and 61% on PA would not be considered valid.
Table 23. WMT subtests by level of WMT effort
Mean WMT effort | WMT DR mean | N | WMT MC mean | WMT MC SD | WMT PA mean | WMT PA SD | WMT FR mean | WMT FR SD |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
91%–100% | 97.5% | 813 | 90.4 | 10.8 | 85.8 | 14.4 | 52.9 | 52.9 |
81%–90% | 88.1% | 218 | 66.9 | 14.1 | 61.4 | 16.0 | 35.6 | 35.6 |
71%–80% | 77.0% | 119 | 54.5 | 12.6 | 50.2 | 14.9 | 31.5 | 31.5 |
61%–70% | 67.1% | 63 | 47.8 | 11.3 | 44.9 | 14.7 | 27.8 | 27.8 |
51%–60% | 54.5% | 55 | 36.0 | 13.0 | 34.5 | 13.2 | 23.7 | 23.7 |
≤50% | 41.1% | 39 | 26.8 | 15.6 | 27.4 | 14.1 | 19.9 | 19.9 |
Table 24. Average drop in performance on eight tests for each level of effort on WMT: scores expressed in terms of standard deviations below the mean for those in the top range of effort
Mean WMT effort | CVLT SD FREE | WRMT faces | WCST categories | Trail Making Aa | Ruff FFT designs | Finger Tap right | PIQ | Immediate story recall | Mean of 8 tests |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
91%–100% | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 | 0 |
81%–90% | −0.8 | −0.4 | −0.5 | −0.7 | −0.5 | −0.4 | −0.5 | −0.5 | −0.54 |
71%–80% | −0.9 | −0.9 | −0.5 | −0.9 | −0.7 | −0.6 | −0.8 | −0.7 | −0.75 |
61%–70% | −1.0 | −1.1 | −0.7 | −1.2 | −0.6 | −0.4 | −0.9 | −0.9 | −0.85 |
51%–60% | −1.5 | −1.9 | −1.3 | −2.4 | −0.7 | −1.2 | −1.4 | −1.3 | −1.46 |
≤50% | −2.0 | −3.1 | −2.1 | −2.8 | −1.1 | −1.6 | −1.6 | −1.2 | −1.94 |
- a
- For Trail Making A, the signs have been reversed to make the table consistent, because longer times imply poorer performance.
When those in the top WMT effort range were compared with those in the second range on all neuropsychological tests shown in the tables, the mean differences were significant at 0.005 or lower in all cases, except for grip strength, grooved pegboard, and finger tip number writing. For example, the mean Trail Making B score was 76 seconds in the top effort range but 105 seconds in the second range (see Table 9). To put that number in perspective, people who had head injuries and up to 6 days of coma took an average of only 89 seconds on Trail Making B in the study of Dikmen and colleagues [5]. Those scoring in the second effort range from the top in Table 1 scored a mean of 8.3 on CVLT short-delayed free recall, whereas the more severe brain injury group discussed earlier scored 9.9 (SD = 3.1). This adds further support to the conclusion that mean WMT scores in the second range imply poor effort, sufficient to affect most other test scores.
It is not surprising that mean scores of 81% to 90% on WMT effort subtests are linked with declines in other test scores when the scores of the following groups are considered: (1) children who had FAS scored a mean of 95.5% (SD = 5.8) on the DR subtest of the WMT [33]; (2) adults who had neurological diseases, selected for having impaired verbal memory, scored a mean of 95% (SD = 5.5) on WMT DR [3]; (3) mentally retarded adults scored a mean of 95% (SD = 5) on the oral form of the WMT [31]; (4) adults who had severe brain injuries and a mean GCS of 5 obtained a mean score of 96.6% (SD = 4) on WMT DR [3].
Read full article
URL:
https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S1047965106000830
Sleep Medicine
Louise M O’Brien PhD, David Gozal MD, in Pediatric Clinics of North America, 2004
Following acute sleep restriction, investigators have failed to find deficits in a word memory task [89], yet such deficits were observed after 38 hours of sleep deprivation in a sample of similar-aged children [88]. Performance of a verbal memory task seemed unaffected by acute sleep restriction [94], and 3 nights of restricted sleep in children aged 10 to 14 years revealed no deficits on a working memory task [95]. In children with SDB, however, memory performance on standardized psychometric tests is significantly reduced compared with control children [46,60], and a dose-response effect may be present, whereby children with higher respiratory disturbance indices show greater memory deficits [60]. In contrast, Owens-Stively et al [93] and O’Brien et al [19,44] failed to identify any differences in memory performance in children with varying degrees of SDB severity and control children. The limited number of studies in this area and the conflicting results require further investigation.
Read full article
URL:
https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0031395503001846
Spanish-language cognitive screening tests: a critical review
S. Torres-Castro, … M.C. Espinel-Bermúdez, in Neurología (English Edition), 2022
Mini-Cog
Table 2 shows the different validation studies of the Mini-Cog. This screening tool for dementia is quick to administer (less than 5 min) and has 2 sections: the first section is a 3-word memory test and the second is a clock-drawing task. The Mini-Cog was introduced in the United States by Borson17 in 2000, with a sample of 249 elderly individuals from different language communities. The test showed greater sensitivity (99%) and specificity (93%) than the MMSE (91% and 92%, respectively) for identifying patients with moderate dementia. Furthermore, the Mini-Cog showed greater screening accuracy than the MMSE (83% vs 81%), even in individuals with very low levels of schooling, and has been shown to be easy to administer to non–English speakers.19 According to a 2008 study by Kaufer et al.,20 the Mini-Cog has high sensitivity for screening for dementia but low sensitivity and specificity (0.50 and 0.73, respectively) for screening for MCI (Table 2).
Table 2. Validation studies for the Mini-Cog.
Author | Country | Year | No. items and sections | Duration | Cut-off point | Sensitivity | Specificity | Reliability |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Borson et al.17 | United States | 2000 | 3-item memory test and clock-drawing test | 3.2 (2) min | Mini-Cog: 99% MMSE: 91% CASI: 92% |
Mini-Cog: 93% MMSE: 92% CASI: 96% |
||
Borson et al.19 | United States | 2005 | Mini-Cog: 83% MMSE: 81% |
|||||
Kaufer et al.20 | United States | 2008 | 3-item memory test and clock-drawing test | 0-1 | Dementia: 0.87 MCI: 0.50 |
Dementia: 0.54 MCI: 0.73 |
Dementia: 70.6 MCI: 61.7 |
|
Steenland et al.21 | United States | 2008 | 3-item memory test and clock-drawing test | 3−5 min | Normal FAQ: 0–2; Mini-Cog: 4-6 MCI FAQ: 0–2; Mini-Cog: 0-3 FAQ: 3–9; Mini-Cog: 0-6 FAQ: 10–12; Mini-Cog: 3-6 FAQ ≥ 13; Mini-Cog: 6 Dementia FAQ: 10–12; Mini-Cog: 0-2 FAQ ≥ 13; Mini-Cog: 0-5 |
Mini-Cog + FAQ: 83% MMSE + FAQ: 85% |
||
Dechent30 | Chile | 2013 | Mini-Cog: 82%; MIS: 82% | Mini-Cog: 69%; MIS: 75% |
CASI: Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument; FAQ: Functional Activities Questionnaire; MCI: mild cognitive impairment; MIS: Memory Impairment Screen; MMSE: Mini–Mental State Examination.
Furthermore, the combination of the Mini-Cog and the Functional Activities Questionnaire identifies individuals with dementia or cognitive impairment with 83% accuracy, compared to 85% for the combination of the MMSE and the Functional Activities Questionnaire, with the latter combination taking twice or 3 times as long to administer as the Mini-Cog.
Read full article
URL:
https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2173580820300146
Neuropsychological impairments in panic disorder: A systematic review
Kate. O׳Sullivan, Emily F. Newman, in Journal of Affective Disorders, 2014
3.2 Neuropsychological variables
As many neuropsychological measures can be said to assess a number of cognitive functions, Lezak et al.׳s (2004) categorisation of neuropsychological assessments has been broadly followed when tabulating and discussing the measures used in the reviewed studies.
3.2.1 Verbal memory
The California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) (Delis et al., 1987), two Selective Reminding (SR) tasks (Buschke and Fuld, 1974) and 4 recall of word lists tasks were included in the analysis. The Warrington Recognition Memory task (Words) (Warrington, 1984) which tests the participant׳s ability to recognise a previously presented word when paired with a distractor was also included, as well as the paired associates and logical memory subscales of the Wechsler Memory Scale (WMS; Wechsler, 1987).
3.2.2 Visual memory
The Benton Visual Retention test (BVRT; Benton, 1945), Rey–Osterrieth Complex Figure test recall measure (RCFT; Rey, 1941), Visual Selective Reminding Test and Continuous Visual Memory Test (CVMT; Trahan and Larrabee, 1989) were included in this domain. Visual memory was also assessed using a non-standardised task in which an array of numbers was visually presented followed by immediate recall (Gordeev, 2008) and the visual reproduction subscale of the WMS (Wechsler, 1987). Visual recognition memory was investigated using 3 subtests of the computerised Cambridge Neuropsychological Test Automated Battery (CANTAB; Cambridge Cognition, Cambridge, UK) – Spatial Recognition, Pattern recognition and delayed match to sample and the Warrington Recognition Memory test (Faces) (Warrington, 1984).
3.2.3 Working memory
Working memory abilities were assessed using span and superspan tasks. The digit span task (Wechsler, 1981), the Hebb Digit Recurring task, the Corsi Block Tapping Task (CBTT; Berch, 1998) and the spatial span subtask of the computerised CANTAB (Cambridge Cognition, Cambridge, UK) were included in the analysis. These tasks are also influenced by attention and verbal and visual memory abilities.
3.2.4 Attention
Attention was assessed using the Digit Symbol task from the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R; Wechsler, 1981), a Continuous Performance Test (Rosvold et al., 1956), time to complete the Trail Making Test part A (TMT A; Reitan, 1958), the Digit Vigilance task (Lewis and Rennick, 1979), the Rapid Visual Information Processing subtask of the CANTAB (Cambridge Cognition, Cambridge, UK) and the mental control task of the WMS (Wechsler, 1987).
3.2.5 Perception
Tests included in this section were classed primarily as tests of perception by Lezak et al. (2004) as they measure visual field perception, visual searching and facial recognition but also include selected and divided attention. The Benton Facial Recognition test (BFRT; Benton 1983) was used to test facial recognition in the absense of a memory condition. The Visual Field Neglect task from the ‘Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprufung’ (TAP; Zimmermann and Finn, 1993) was used to test divided attention and the Signal Detection test from the Weiner Test System (Schuhfried, 1999), however information on psychometric properties could not be found for either of these measures. The Munsterberg test was also used to test selective attention. A Schulte tables task of sustained attention and a digit cancellation test were also used. The Munsterberg test requires participants to find words in a random set of letters within a limited time. Psychometric properties for the Munsterberg task and Schulte tables were not found in a search of the literature. Some literature was found indicating that Schulte tables do not have well established psychometric properties (Ennok, 2010).
3.2.6 Visuospatial ability
Visuospatial ability was assessed using the Block Design task from the WAIS-R (Wechsler, 1981), the copy trial of the RCFT (Rey, 1941), and an unstandardised Virtual Water Maze analog testing spatial orientation and learning (Jacobs et al., 1997). The ability to perform spatial rotations mentally was investigated using the mental rotation test (MRT; Vandenberg and Kuse, 1978).
3.2.7 Executive function
The category of executive functioning was expanded from that of Lezak et al. (2004) in line with Burgess (2003) to incorporate tests of inhibition, coordinated dual tasks (e.g. the Trail Making Task) and verbal fluency. In the context of the reviewed studies, such measures were used with the purpose of assessing executive functioning. Results were considered under the headings of planning and organising, set shifting, verbal fluency and decision making. The Wisconsin Card Sort Task (WCST; Heaton et al., 1993), time to complete the Trail Making Test part B (TMT B; Reitan, 1958), the Intradimensional–Extradimensional shift and the Spatial Working Memory subtasks of the computerised CANTAB (Cambridge Cognition, Cambridge, UK) were used to assess set shifting. The Tower of London subtask of the computerised CANTAB (Cambridge Cognition, Cambridge, UK) and the organisation trial of the RCFT (Rey, 1941) were used to assess planning and organising ability. Verbal fluency was assessed using the FAS letter fluency task (Benton and Hamsher, 1983), a category fluency task (Goodglass and Kaplan, 1983) and the Benton Controlled Oral Word Association Test (COWAT; Benton, 1989). Decision Making ability was examined by Cavedini et al. (2002) using the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT; Bechara et al., 1994), by Kaplan et al. (2006) using the Cambridge Gambling task (Rogers et al., 1999) and by Ludewig et al. (2003) using a two-choice prediction task (Paulus, 1997).
Read full article
URL:
https://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S0165032714003851
Dr. Green’s Word Memory Test (WMT), Medical Symptom Validity Test (MSVT) and Nonverbal-MSVT (NV-MSVT) are computerized memory tests with multiple subtests measuring verbal and nonverbal memory. They contain hidden measures, which serve to check the validity of the patient’s test scores.
The original WMT was invented by Dr. Paul Green as an oral test (Green and Astner, 1995) and then sold for a limited period on his behalf as a DOS computer program (Green, Allen and Astner, 1996). The WMT Windows program (2003) consists of exactly the same test stimuli as the original WMT oral form and the now obsolete DOS program. The vast majority of research on the WMT is with the computer version. The oral form is an adjunct for use in limited circumstances.
The MSVT (Green, 2004) is a shorter, modified and easier version of the WMT. The NV-MSVT (Green, 2008) is a nonverbal equivalent to the MSVT.
The WMT, MSVT and the NV-MSVT are now available to qualified professionals as MS Windows programs only by direct order from Green’s Publishing. Green’s WMT, MSVT and NV-MSVT for Windows are very quick and cost effective. They offer computerized administration, scoring, data storage and automated data spreadsheet creation for analyzing data gathered.
Easy-to-use graphs and tables let you compare your client’s WMT scores with those from any of more than 90 comparison groups (Total N is well over 3,000 cases).
To inspect the WMT, MSVT or NV-MSVT programs just call Matt or John at +1 (236) 420-4351 and see for yourself (Qualified professionals only: You will be asked to state profession, position, qualifications, address and phone number).
SIMULATOR STUDIES: The WMT has been consistently found to be close to or actually 100% accurate in classifying simulators versus good effort volunteers.
More recently, Green’s MSVT has been found to be equally accurate in two German studies (Dr. Thomas Merten and Nina Blaskewitz, Berlin, Germany). A very large ongoing Brazilian study by Dr. John Courtney (New Orleans & Rio de Janeiro) is providing strong support for the accuracy of Green’s MSVT Windows in discriminating between simulators versus good effort volunteers tested in Portuguese (both adults and children).
The MSVT test manual contains appendices summarizing (a) the Brazilian study; (b) normative data from children in grades two to five, given Green’s MSVT Windows and (c) data from people with soft tissue injuries tested with the MSVT in medical clinics in Toronto run by Dr. Jack Richman, Occupational Health Physician. The latter data are the subject of a paper published in the Journal of Occupational and Environmental Medicine. This clinic now has a sample size of more than 3,000 consecutive cases tested with the MSVT.