What is the definition of the word empathy

A small child hugs an older, injured child

Hugging someone who is hurt is a signal of empathy.

Empathy is the capacity to understand or feel what another person is experiencing from within their frame of reference, that is, the capacity to place oneself in another’s position.[1] Definitions of empathy encompass a broad range of social, cognitive, and emotional processes primarily concerned with understanding others (and others’ emotions in particular). Types of empathy include cognitive empathy, emotional (or affective) empathy, somatic empathy, and spiritual empathy.[2][3][4]

Etymology[edit]

Painting of two girls sitting on the ground

Understanding another’s view

The English word empathy is derived from the Ancient Greek ἐμπάθεια (empatheia, meaning «physical affection or passion»).[5] That word derives from ἐν (en, «in, at») and πάθος (pathos, «passion» or «suffering»).[6] Theodor Lipps adapted the German aesthetic term Einfühlung («feeling into») to psychology in 1903,[7] and Edward B. Titchener translated Einfühlung into English as «empathy» in 1909.[8] In modern Greek εμπάθεια may mean, depending on context, prejudice, malevolence, malice, or hatred.[9]

Definitions[edit]

General[edit]

Since its introduction into the English language, empathy has had a wide range of (sometimes conflicting) definitions among both researchers and laypeople.[10][11][12][13] Empathy definitions encompass a broad range of phenomena, including caring for other people and having a desire to help them; experiencing emotions that match another person’s emotions; discerning what another person is thinking or feeling;[14] and making less distinct the differences[clarification needed] between the self and the other.[15]

Since empathy involves understanding the emotional states of other people, the way it is characterized derives from the way emotions are characterized. For example, if emotions are characterized by bodily feelings, then understanding the bodily feelings of another will be considered central to empathy. On the other hand, if emotions are characterized by a combination of beliefs and desires, then understanding those beliefs and desires will be more essential to empathy. The ability to imagine oneself as another person is a sophisticated process. However, the basic capacity to recognize emotions in others may be innate[16] and may be achieved unconsciously. Empirical research supports a variety of interventions to improve empathy.[17][18][19][20][21]

Empathy is not all-or-nothing; rather, a person can be more or less empathic toward another. Paradigmatically, a person exhibits empathy when they communicate an accurate recognition of the significance of another person’s ongoing intentional actions, associated emotional states, and personal characteristics in a manner that seems accurate and tolerable to the recognized person.[22][23]

One’s ability to recognize the bodily feelings of another is related to one’s imitative capacities, and seems to be grounded in an innate capacity to associate the bodily movements and facial expressions one sees in another with the proprioceptive feelings of producing those corresponding movements or expressions oneself.[24]

Distinctions between empathy and related concepts[edit]

Compassion and sympathy are terms associated with empathy. A person feels compassion when they notice others are in need, and this feeling motivates that person to help. Like empathy, compassion has a wide range of definitions and purported facets (which overlap with some definitions of empathy).[25][26] Sympathy is a feeling of care and understanding for someone in need. Some include in sympathy an empathic concern for another person, and the wish to see them better off or happier.[27]

Empathy is also related to pity and emotional contagion.[28][27] One feels pity towards others who might be in trouble or in need of help. This feeling is described as «feeling sorry» for someone. Emotional contagion is when a person (especially an infant or a member of a mob) imitatively «catches» the emotions that others are showing without necessarily recognizing this is happening.[29]

Alexithymia describes a deficiency in understanding, processing, or describing one’s own emotions (unlike empathy which is about someone else’s emotions).[30]

Classification[edit]

Empathy has two major components:[31]

  1. Affective empathy, also called emotional empathy,[32] is the ability to respond with an appropriate emotion to another’s mental states.[31] Our ability to empathize emotionally is based on emotional contagion:[32] being affected by another’s emotional or arousal state.[33] Affective empathy can be subdivided into the following scales:[31][34]
    • Empathic concern: sympathy and compassion for others in response to their suffering.[31][35][36]
    • Personal distress: feelings of discomfort and anxiety in response to another’s suffering.[31][35][36] There is no consensus regarding whether personal distress is a form of empathy or instead is something distinct from empathy.[28][35] There may be a developmental aspect to this subdivision. Infants respond to the distress of others by getting distressed themselves; only when they are two years old do they start to respond in other-oriented ways: trying to help, comfort, and share.[35]
    • Affective mentalizing: uses clues like like body language, facial expressions, knowledge about the other’s beliefs & situation, and context to understand more about what one is empathizing with.[37]
  2. Cognitive empathy is the ability to understand another’s perspective or mental state.[38][31][39] The terms empathic accuracy, social cognition, perspective-taking, theory of mind, and mentalizing are often used synonymously, but due to a lack of studies comparing theory of mind with types of empathy, it is unclear whether these are equivalent.[40] Although measures of cognitive empathy include self-report questionnaires and behavioral measures, a 2019 meta-analysis[41] found only a negligible association between self-report and behavioral measures, suggesting that people are generally not able to accurately assess their own cognitive empathy abilities. Cognitive empathy can be subdivided into the following scales:[31][34]
    • Perspective-taking: the tendency to spontaneously adopt others’ psychological perspectives.[31][42]
    • Fantasy: the tendency to identify with fictional characters.[31]
    • Tactical (or strategic) empathy: the deliberate use of perspective-taking to achieve certain desired ends.[43]
    • Emotion regulation: a damper on the emotional contagion process that allows you to empathize without being overwhelmed by the emotion you are empathizing with.[44]

The scientific community has not coalesced around a precise definition of these constructs, but there is consensus about this distinction.[45][46][47] Affective and cognitive empathy are also independent from one another; someone who strongly empathizes emotionally is not necessarily good in understanding another’s perspective.[48]

Development[edit]

Evolution across species[edit]

Studies in animal behavior and neuroscience indicate that empathy is not restricted to humans (however the interpretation of such research depends in part on how expansive a definition of empathy researchers adopt[28]).

Empathy-like behaviors have been observed in primates, both in captivity and in the wild, and in particular in bonobos, perhaps the most empathic primate.[49]

One study demonstrated prosocial behavior elicited by empathy in rodents.[50] Rodents demonstrate empathy for cagemates (but not strangers) in pain.[51] An influential study on the evolution of empathy by Stephanie Preston and Frans de Waal[52] discusses a neural perception-action mechanism and postulates a bottom-up model of empathy that ties together all levels,[clarification needed] from state matching[clarification needed] to perspective-taking.

University of Chicago neurobiologist Jean Decety agrees that empathy is not exclusive to humans, but that empathy has deep evolutionary, biochemical, and neurological underpinnings, and that even the most advanced forms of empathy in humans are built on more basic forms and remain connected to core mechanisms associated with affective communication, social attachment, and parental care.[53] Neural circuits involved in empathy and caring include the brainstem, the amygdala, hypothalamus, basal ganglia, insula, and orbitofrontal cortex.[54]

Ontogenetic development[edit]

By the age of two, children normally begin to exhibit fundamental behaviors of empathy by having an emotional response that corresponds with another person’s emotional state.[55] Even earlier, at one year of age, infants have some rudiments of empathy; they understand that, as with their own actions, other people’s actions have goals.[56][57] Toddlers sometimes comfort others or show concern for them. During their second year, they play games of falsehood or pretend in an effort to fool others. Such actions require that the child knows what others believe in order that the child can manipulate those beliefs.[58]

According to researchers at the University of Chicago who used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), children between the ages of seven and twelve experience brain activity when seeing others be injured similar to the brain activity that would occur if the child themself had been injured.[59] Their findings are consistent with previous fMRI studies of pain empathy with adults, and previous findings that vicarious experiencing, particularly of others’ distress, is hardwired and present early in life.[59] The research found additional areas of the brain, associated with social and moral cognition, were activated when young people saw another person intentionally hurt by somebody, including regions involved in moral reasoning.[59]

Although children are capable of showing some signs of empathy, including attempting to comfort a crying baby, from as early as 18 months to two years, most do not demonstrate a full theory of mind until around the age of four.[60] Theory of mind involves the ability to understand that other people may have beliefs that are different from one’s own, and is thought to involve the cognitive component of empathy.[38] Children usually can pass false-belief tasks (a test for a theory of mind) around the age of four. It is theorised that people with autism find using a theory of mind to be very difficult (e.g. the Sally–Anne test).[61][62]

Empathic maturity is a cognitive-structural theory developed at the Yale University School of Nursing. It addresses how adults conceive or understand the personhood of patients. The theory, first applied to nurses and since applied to other professions, postulates three levels of cognitive structures. The third and highest level is a meta-ethical theory of the moral structure of care. Adults who operate with level-III understanding synthesize systems of justice and care-based ethics.[63]

Individual differences[edit]

The Empathic Concern scale assesses other-oriented feelings of sympathy and concern and the Personal Distress scale measures self-oriented feelings of personal anxiety and unease.[64] Researchers have used behavioral and neuroimaging data to analyze extraversion and agreeableness (the Warmth-Altruistic personality profile). Both are associated with empathic accuracy and increased brain activity in two brain regions that are important for empathic processing (medial prefrontal cortex and temporoparietal junction).[65]

Sex differences[edit]

On average, females score higher than males on measures of empathy,[66] such as the Empathy Quotient (EQ), while males tend to score higher on the Systemizing Quotient (SQ). Both males and females with autistic spectrum disorders usually score lower on the EQ and higher on SQ (see below for more detail on autism and empathy).[38]

Other studies show no significant sex differences, and instead suggest that gender differences are the result of motivational differences, such as upholding stereotypes.[66][67] Gender stereotypes about men and women can affect how they express emotions. The sex difference is small to moderate, somewhat inconsistent, and is often influenced by the person’s motivations or social environment.[66] Bosson et al. say «physiological measures of emotion and studies that track people in their daily lives find no consistent sex differences in the experience of emotion,» which «suggests that women may amplify certain emotional expressions, or men may suppress them.»[66] However, a 2014 review from Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews reported that there is evidence that «sex differences in empathy have phylogenetic and ontogenetic roots in biology and are not merely cultural byproducts driven by socialization.»[68]

A review published in Neuropsychologia found that females tended to be better at recognizing facial affects, expression processing, and emotions in general.[69] Males tended to be better at recognizing specific behaviors such as anger, aggression, and threatening cues.[69] A 2014 meta-analysis, in Cognition and Emotion, found a small female advantage in non-verbal emotional recognition.[70]

The 2014 Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews analysis found sex differences in empathy from birth, growing larger with age, and consistent and stable across lifespan.[68] Females, on average, had higher empathy than males, while children with higher empathy, regardless of gender, continue to be higher in empathy throughout development.[68] Analysis of brain event-related potentials found that females who saw human suffering tended to have higher ERP waveforms than males.[68] An investigation of N400 amplitudes found, on average, higher N400 in females in response to social situations which positively correlated with self-reported empathy.[68] Structural fMRI studies also found females to have larger grey matter volumes in posterior inferior frontal and anterior inferior parietal cortex areas which are correlated with mirror neurons in fMRI literature.[68] Females also tended to have a stronger link between emotional and cognitive empathy.[68] The researchers believe that the stability of these sex differences in development are unlikely to be explained by environmental influences but rather by human evolution and inheritance.[68] Throughout prehistory, women were the primary nurturers and caretakers of children; so this might have led to an evolved neurological adaptation for women to be more aware and responsive to non-verbal expressions. According to the «Primary Caretaker Hypothesis», prehistoric men did not have such selective pressure as primary caretakers. This might explain modern day sex differences in emotion recognition and empathy.[68]

Environmental influences[edit]

Some research theorizes that environmental factors, such as parenting style and relationships, affect the development of empathy in children. Empathy promotes pro-social relationships[71] and helps mediate aggression.

Caroline Tisot studied how environmental factors like parenting style, parent empathy, and prior social experiences affect the development of empathy in young children. The children studied were asked to complete an effective empathy measure, while the children’s parents completed a questionnaire to assess parenting style and the Balanced Emotional Empathy scale. The study found that certain parenting practices, as opposed to parenting style as a whole, contributed to the development of empathy in children. These practices include encouraging the child to imagine the perspectives of others and teaching the child to reflect on his or her own feelings. The development of empathy varied based on the gender of the child and parent. Paternal warmth was significantly positively related to empathy in children, especially boys. Maternal warmth was negatively related to empathy in children, especially girls.[72]

Empathy may be disrupted due to brain trauma such as stroke. In most cases, empathy is impaired if a lesion or stroke occurs on the right side of the brain.[73] Damage to the frontal lobe, which is primarily responsible for emotional regulation, can profoundly impact a person’s capacity to experience empathy.[74] People with an acquired brain injury also show lower levels of empathy. More than half of those people with a traumatic brain injury self-report a deficit in their empathic capacity.[75]

There is some evidence that empathy is a skill that one can improve in with training.[76]

Empathic anger and distress[edit]

Anger[edit]

Empathic anger is an emotion, a form of empathic distress.[77] Empathic anger is felt in a situation where someone else is being hurt by another person or thing.[78]

Empathic anger affects desires to help and to punish. Two sub-categories of empathic anger are state empathic anger (current empathic anger) and trait empathic anger (tendency or predisposition to experience empathic anger).[79]

The higher a person’s perspective-taking ability, the less angry they are in response to a provocation. Empathic concern does not, however, significantly predict anger response, and higher personal distress is associated with increased anger.[80]

Distress[edit]

Empathic distress is feeling the perceived pain of another person. This feeling can be transformed into empathic anger, feelings of injustice, or guilt. These emotions can be perceived as pro-social; however, views differ as to whether they serve as motives for moral behavior.[77][81]

Influence on helping behavior[edit]

Investigators into the social response to natural disasters researched the characteristics associated with individuals who help victims. Researchers found that cognitive empathy, rather than emotional empathy, predicted helping behavior towards victims.[82] Taking on the perspectives of others (cognitive empathy) may allow these helpers to better empathize with victims without as much discomfort, whereas sharing the emotions of the victims (emotional empathy) can cause emotional distress, helplessness, and victim-blaming, and may lead to avoidance rather than helping.[83]

Individuals who expressed concern for the vulnerable (i.e. affective empathy) were more willing to accept the COVID-19 pandemic lockdown measures that create distress.[84] Knowledge of how empathic feelings evoke altruistic motivation may lead people to adopt strategies for suppressing or avoiding such feelings. Such numbing, or loss of the capacity to feel empathy for clients, is a possible factor in the experience of burnout among case workers in helping professions. People can better cognitively control their actions the more they understand how altruistic behavior emerges, whether it is from minimizing sadness or the arousal of mirror neurons. Empathy-induced altruism may not always produce pro-social effects. For example, it could lead one to exert oneself on behalf of those for whom empathy is felt at the expense of other potential pro-social goals, thus inducing a type of bias. Researchers suggest that individuals are willing to act against the greater collective good or to violate their own moral principles of fairness and justice if doing so will benefit a person for whom empathy is felt.[85]

Empathy-based socialization differs from inhibition of egoistic impulses through shaping, modeling, and internalized guilt. Therapeutic programs to foster altruistic impulses by encouraging perspective-taking and empathic feelings might enable individuals to develop more satisfactory interpersonal relations, especially in the long-term. Empathy-induced altruism can improve attitudes toward stigmatized groups, and to improve racial attitudes, and actions toward people with AIDS, the homeless, and convicts. Such resulting altruism also increases cooperation in competitive situations.[86]

Empathy is good at prompting prosocial behaviors that are informal, unplanned, and directed at someone who is immediately present, but is not as good at prompting more abstractly-considered, long-term prosocial behavior.[87]

Empathy can not only be a precursor to ones own helpful acts, but can also be a way of inviting help from others. If you mimic the posture, facial expressions, and vocal style of someone you are with, you can thereby encourage them to help you and to form a favorable opinion of you.[88]

Genetics[edit]

General[edit]

Measures of empathy show evidence of being genetically influenced.[89] For example, carriers of the deletion variant of ADRA2B show more activation of the amygdala when viewing emotionally arousing images.[90] The gene 5-HTTLPR seems to influence sensitivity to negative emotional information and is also attenuated by the deletion variant of ADRA2b.[91] Carriers of the double G variant of the OXTR gene have better social skills and higher self-esteem.[clarification needed][92] A gene located near LRRN1 on chromosome 3 influences the human ability to read, understand, and respond to emotions in others.[93]

Neuroscientific basis of empathy[edit]

Contemporary neuroscience offers insights into the neural basis of the mind’s ability to understand and process emotion. Studies of mirror neurons attempt to measure the neural basis for human mind-reading and emotion-sharing abilities and thereby to explain the basis of the empathy reaction.[94] People who score high on empathy tests have especially busy mirror neuron systems.[95] Empathy is a spontaneous sharing of affect, provoked by witnessing and sympathizing with another’s emotional state. The empathic person mirrors or mimics the emotional response they would expect to feel if they were in the other person’s place. Unlike personal distress, empathy is not characterized by aversion to another’s emotional response. This distinction is vital because empathy is associated with the moral emotion sympathy, or empathic concern, and consequently also prosocial or altruistic action.[94]

A person empathizes by feeling what they believe to be the emotions of another, which makes empathy both affective and cognitive.[clarification needed][14] For social beings, negotiating interpersonal decisions is as important to survival as being able to navigate the physical landscape.[96]

Meta-analysis studies of functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) studies of empathy confirm that different brain areas are activated during affective-perceptual empathy than during cognitive-evaluative empathy. Affective empathy is correlated with increased activity in the insula while cognitive empathy is correlated with activity in the mid cingulate cortex and adjacent dorsomedial prefrontal cortex.[97] A study with patients who experienced different types of brain damage confirmed the distinction between emotional and cognitive empathy.[32] Specifically, the inferior frontal gyrus appears to be responsible for emotional empathy, and the ventromedial prefrontal gyrus seems to mediate cognitive empathy.[32]

fMRI has been employed to investigate the functional anatomy of empathy.[98] Observing another person’s emotional state activates parts of the neuronal network that are involved in processing that same state in oneself, whether it is disgust,[99] touch,[100] or pain.[101][36]

The study of the neural underpinnings of empathy received increased interest following a paper published by S.D. Preston and Frans de Waal[102] after the discovery of mirror neurons in monkeys that fire both when the creature watches another perform an action as well as when they themselves perform it. Researchers suggest that paying attention to perceiving another individual’s state activates neural representations, and that this activation primes or generates the associated autonomic and somatic responses (perception-action coupling),[103] unless inhibited. This mechanism resembles the common coding theory between perception and action. Another study provides evidence of separate neural pathways activating reciprocal suppression in different regions of the brain associated with the performance of «social» and «mechanical» tasks. These findings suggest that the cognition associated with reasoning about the «state of another person’s mind» and «causal/mechanical properties of inanimate objects» are neurally suppressed from occurring at the same time.[104]

Mirroring-behavior in motor neurons during empathy may help duplicate feelings.[105] Such sympathetic action may afford access to sympathetic feelings and, perhaps, trigger emotions of kindness and forgiveness.[106]

Impairment[edit]

A difference in distribution between affective and cognitive empathy has been observed in various conditions. Psychopathy and narcissism are associated with impairments in affective but not cognitive empathy, whereas bipolar disorder is associated with deficits in cognitive but not affective empathy. People with Borderline personality disorder may suffer from impairments in cognitive empathy as well as fluctuating affective empathy, although this topic is controversial. [45] Autism spectrum disorders are associated with various combinations, including deficits in cognitive empathy as well as deficits in both cognitive and affective empathy.[31][32][45][35][107][108] Schizophrenia, too, is associated with deficits in both types of empathy.[109] However, even in people without conditions such as these, the balance between affective and cognitive empathy varies.[45]

Atypical empathic responses are associated with autism and particular personality disorders such as psychopathy, borderline, narcissistic, and schizoid personality disorders; conduct disorder;[110] schizophrenia; bipolar disorder;[45] and depersonalization.[111] Sex offenders who had been raised in an environment where they were shown a lack of empathy and had endured abuse of the sort they later committed, felt less affective empathy for their victims.[112]

Autism[edit]

The interaction between empathy and autism is a complex and ongoing field of research. Several different factors are proposed to be at play.

A study of high-functioning adults with autistic spectrum disorders found an increased prevalence of alexithymia,[113] a personality construct characterized by the inability to recognize and articulate emotional arousal in oneself or others.[113][114] Some fMRI research indicates that alexithymia contributes to a lack of empathy.[115] The lack of empathic attunement inherent to alexithymic states may reduce quality[116] and satisfaction[117] of relationships. Empathy deficits associated with the autism spectrum may be due to significant comorbidity between alexithymia and autism spectrum conditions rather than a result of social impairment.[118]

Relative to typically developing children, high-functioning autistic children showed reduced mirror neuron activity in the brain’s inferior frontal gyrus (pars opercularis) while imitating and observing emotional expressions in neurotypical children.[119] EEG evidence revealed significantly greater mu suppression in the sensorimotor cortex of autistic individuals. Activity in this area was inversely related to symptom severity in the social domain, suggesting that a dysfunctional mirror neuron system may underlie social and communication deficits observed in autism, including impaired theory of mind and cognitive empathy.[120] The mirror neuron system is essential for emotional empathy.[32]

Studies have suggested that autistic individuals have an impaired theory of mind.[31] Theory of mind relies on structures of the temporal lobe and the pre-frontal cortex; empathy relies on the sensorimotor cortices as well as limbic and para-limbic structures.[121] The lack of clear distinctions between theory of mind and cognitive empathy may have caused an incomplete understanding of the empathic abilities of those with Asperger syndrome; many reports on the empathic deficits of individuals with Asperger syndrome are actually based on impairments in theory of mind.[31][122][123] Although autistic people have difficulties in recognizing and articulating emotions, some studies have reported that while they may lack cognitive empathy (the ability to assume another’s emotions), they have higher than average levels of affective empathy (feeling the emotions that another is feeling, once they are known).[124]

Individuals on the autistic spectrum self-report lower levels of empathic concern, show less or absent comforting responses toward someone who is suffering, and report equal or higher levels of personal distress compared to controls.[35] The combination of reduced empathic concern and increased personal distress may lead to the overall reduction in empathy.[35] Professor Simon Baron-Cohen suggests that those with classic autism often lack both cognitive and affective empathy.[108] However, other research found no evidence of impairment in autistic individuals’ ability to understand other people’s basic intentions or goals; instead, data suggests that impairments are found in understanding more complex social emotions or in considering others’ viewpoints.[125] People with Asperger syndrome may have problems understanding others’ perspectives in terms of theory of mind, but the average person with the condition demonstrates equal empathic concern as, and higher personal distress than, controls.[31] The existence of individuals with heightened personal distress on the autism spectrum is a possible explanation for why some people with autism appear to have heightened emotional empathy.[35][107] Although increased personal distress may be an effect of heightened egocentrism, emotional empathy depends on mirror neuron activity (which, as described previously, has been found to be reduced in those with autism), and empathy in people on the autism spectrum is generally reduced.[32][35] Empathy deficits present in autism spectrum disorders may be more indicative of impairments in the ability to take the perspective of others, while the empathy deficits in psychopathy may be more indicative of impairments in responsiveness to others’ emotions. These «disorders of empathy» further highlight the importance of the ability to empathize, by the way they illustrate some of the consequences of disrupted empathy development.[126]

The empathizing–systemizing theory (E-S) classifies people by testing their capabilities along two independent dimensions—empathizing (E) and systemizing (S)—to establish their Empathy Quotient (EQ) and Systemizing Quotient (SQ). Five «brain types» can be distinguished based on such scores, which are theorized to correlate with differences at the neural level. In E-S theory, autism and Asperger syndrome are associated with below-average empathy and average or above-average systemizing. The E-S theory has been extended into the Extreme Male Brain theory, which suggests that people with an autism spectrum condition are more likely to have an «Extreme Type S» brain type, corresponding with above-average systemizing but challenged empathy.[127]

The extreme male brain (EMB) theory proposes that individuals on the autistic spectrum are characterized by impairments in empathy due to sex differences in the brain: specifically, people with autism spectrum conditions show an exaggerated male profile. Some aspects of autistic neuroanatomy seem to be extrapolations of typical male neuroanatomy, which may be influenced by elevated levels of fetal testosterone rather than gender itself.[127][128]

The double empathy problem theory proposes that prior studies on autism and empathy may have been misinterpreted and that autistic people show the same levels of cognitive empathy towards one another as non-autistic people do.[129]

Psychopathy[edit]

Psychopathy is a personality disorder partly characterized by antisocial and aggressive behaviors, as well as emotional and interpersonal deficits including shallow emotions and a lack of remorse and empathy.[130][131] The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) and International Classification of Diseases (ICD) list antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) and dissocial personality disorder, stating that these have been referred to as or include what is referred to as psychopathy.[132][133][134][135]

Psychopathy is associated with atypical responses to distress cues (e.g. facial and vocal expressions of fear and sadness), including decreased activation of the fusiform and extrastriate cortical regions, which may partly account for impaired recognition of and reduced autonomic responsiveness to expressions of fear, and impairments of empathy.[136][137][138][139] Studies on children with psychopathic tendencies have also shown such associations.[140][141][142] The underlying biological surfaces[clarification needed] for processing expressions of happiness are functionally intact in psychopaths, although less responsive than in those of controls.[139][140][141][142] The neuroimaging literature is unclear as to whether deficits are specific to particular emotions such as fear. Some fMRI studies report that emotion perception deficits in psychopathy are pervasive across emotions (positives and negatives).[143]

One study on psychopaths found that, under certain circumstances, they could willfully empathize with others, and that their empathic reaction initiated the same way it does for controls. Psychopathic criminals were brain-scanned while watching videos of a person harming another individual. The psychopaths’ empathic reaction initiated the same way it did for controls when they were instructed to empathize with the harmed individual, and the area of the brain relating to pain was activated when the psychopaths were asked to imagine how the harmed individual felt. The research suggests psychopaths can switch empathy on at will, which would enable them to be both callous and charming. The team who conducted the study say they do not know how to transform this willful empathy into the spontaneous empathy most people have, though they propose it might be possible to rehabilitate psychopaths by helping them to activate their «empathy switch». Others suggested that it remains unclear whether psychopaths’ experience of empathy was the same as that of controls, and also questioned the possibility of devising therapeutic interventions that would make the empathic reactions more automatic.[144][145]

One problem with the theory that the ability to turn empathy on and off constitutes psychopathy is that such a theory would classify socially sanctioned violence and punishment as psychopathy, as these entail suspending empathy towards certain individuals and/or groups. The attempt to get around this by standardizing tests of psychopathy for cultures with different norms of punishment is criticized in this context for being based on the assumption that people can be classified in discrete cultures while cultural influences are in reality mixed and every person encounters a mosaic of influences. Psychopathy may be an artefact of psychiatry’s standardization along imaginary sharp lines between cultures, as opposed to an actual difference in the brain.[146]

Work conducted by Professor Jean Decety with large samples of incarcerated psychopaths offers additional insights. In one study, psychopaths were scanned while viewing video clips depicting people being intentionally hurt. They were also tested on their responses to seeing short videos of facial expressions of pain. The participants in the high-psychopathy group exhibited significantly less activation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex, amygdala, and periaqueductal gray parts of the brain, but more activity in the striatum and the insula when compared to control participants.[147] In a second study, individuals with psychopathy exhibited a strong response in pain-affective brain regions when taking an imagine-self perspective, but failed to recruit the neural circuits that were activated in controls during an imagine-other perspective—in particular the ventromedial prefrontal cortex and amygdala—which may contribute to their lack of empathic concern.[148]

Researchers have investigated whether people who have high levels of psychopathy have sufficient levels of cognitive empathy but lack the ability to use affective empathy. People who score highly on psychopathy measures are less likely to exhibit affective empathy. There was a strong negative correlation, showing that psychopathy and lack of affective empathy correspond strongly. The DANVA-2[clarification needed] found those who scored highly on the psychopathy scale do not lack in recognising emotion in facial expressions. Therefore, such individuals do not lack in perspective-talking ability but do lack in compassion and the negative incidents that happen to others[clarification needed].[149]

In fact, in an experiment published in March 2007 at the University of Southern California neuroscientist Antonio R. Damasio and his colleagues showed that subjects with damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex lack the ability to empathically feel their way to moral answers, and that when confronted with moral dilemmas, these brain-damaged patients coldly came up with «end-justifies-the-means» answers, leading Damasio to conclude that the point was not that they reached immoral conclusions, but that when they were confronted by a difficult issue – in this case as whether to shoot down a passenger plane hijacked by terrorists before it hits a major city – these patients appear to reach decisions without the anguish that afflicts those with normally functioning brains. According to Adrian Raine, a clinical neuroscientist also at the University of Southern California, one of this study’s implications is that society may have to rethink how it judges immoral people: «Psychopaths often feel no empathy or remorse. Without that awareness, people relying exclusively on reasoning seem to find it harder to sort their way through moral thickets. Does that mean they should be held to different standards of accountability?»[150]

Despite studies suggesting psychopaths have deficits in emotion perception and imagining others in pain, professor Simon Baron-Cohen claims psychopathy is associated with intact cognitive empathy, which would imply an intact ability to read and respond to behaviors, social cues, and what others are feeling. Psychopathy is, however, associated with impairment in the other major component of empathy—affective (emotional) empathy—which includes the ability to feel the suffering and emotions of others (emotional contagion), and those with the condition are therefore not distressed by the suffering of their victims. Such a dissociation of affective and cognitive empathy has been demonstrated for aggressive offenders.[151]

Other conditions[edit]

Atypical empathic responses are also correlated with a variety of other conditions.

Borderline personality disorder is characterized by extensive behavioral and interpersonal difficulties that arise from emotional and cognitive dysfunction.[152] Dysfunctional social and interpersonal behavior plays a role in the emotionally intense way people with borderline personality disorder react.[153] While individuals with borderline personality disorder may show their emotions excessively, their ability to feel empathy is a topic of much dispute with contradictory findings. Some studies assert impairments in cognitive empathy in BPD patients yet no affective empathy impairments, whilst other studies have found impairments in both affective and cognitive empathy. Fluctuating empathy, fluctuating between normal range of empathy, reduced sense of empathy and a lack of empathy has been noted to be present in BPD patients in multiple studies, although more research is needed to determine its prevalence, although it is believed to be at least not uncommon and may be a very common phenomenon. It must be noted that BPD is a very heterogenous disorder, with symptoms including empathy ranging wildly between patients.

One diagnostic criterion of narcissistic personality disorder is a lack of empathy and an unwillingness or inability to recognize or identify with the feelings and needs of others.[154]

Characteristics of schizoid personality disorder include emotional coldness, detachment, and impaired affect corresponding with an inability to be empathic and sensitive towards others.[155][156][157]

A study conducted by Jean Decety and colleagues at the University of Chicago demonstrated that subjects with aggressive conduct disorder demonstrate atypical empathic responses when viewing others in pain.[110] Subjects with conduct disorder were at least as responsive as controls to the pain of others but, unlike controls, subjects with conduct disorder showed strong and specific activation of the amygdala and ventral striatum (areas that enable a general arousing effect of reward), yet impaired activation of the neural regions involved in self-regulation and metacognition (including moral reasoning), in addition to diminished processing between the amygdala and the prefrontal cortex.[110]

Schizophrenia is characterized by impaired affective empathy,[14][45] as well as severe cognitive and empathy impairments as measured by the Empathy Quotient (EQ).[109] These empathy impairments are also associated with impairments in social cognitive tasks.[109]

Bipolar individuals have impaired cognitive empathy and theory of mind, but increased affective empathy.[45][158] Despite cognitive flexibility being impaired, planning behavior is intact. Dysfunctions in the prefrontal cortex could result in the impaired cognitive empathy, since impaired cognitive empathy has been related with neurocognitive task performance involving cognitive flexibility.[158]

Dave Grossman, in his book On Killing, reports on how military training artificially creates depersonalization in soldiers, suppressing empathy and making it easier for them to kill other human beings.[111]

A deadening of empathic response to workmates, customers and the like is one of the three key components of occupational burnout, according to the conceptualisation behind its primary diagnostic instrument, the Maslach Burnout Inventory (MBI).

The term Empathy Deficit Disorder (EDD) has gained popularity online, but it is not a diagnosis under the DSM-5. The term was coined in an article by Douglas LaBier, PhD.[159] In the article, he acknowledges that he «made it up, so you won’t find it listed in the American Psychiatric Association’s Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders» and that his conclusions are derived from personal experience alone.[159] His conclusions have not been validated through clinical studies, nor have studies identified EDD as a separate disorder rather than a symptom associated with previously established diagnoses that do appear in the DSM-5.

In educational contexts[edit]

Another growing focus of investigation is how empathy manifests in education between teachers and learners.[160] Although there is general agreement that empathy is essential in educational settings, research has found that it is difficult to develop empathy in trainee teachers.[161]

Learning by teaching (LbT) is one method used to teach empathy. Students transmit new content to their classmates, so they have to reflect continuously on those classmates’ mental processes. This develops the students’ feeling for group reactions and networking. Carl R. Rogers pioneered research in effective psychotherapy and teaching which espoused that empathy coupled with unconditional positive regard or caring for students and authenticity or congruence were the most important traits for a therapist or teacher to have. Other research and meta-analyses corroborated the importance of these person-centered traits.[162]

In intercultural contexts[edit]

According to one theory, empathy is one of seven components involved in the effectiveness of intercultural communication. This theory also states that empathy is learnable. However, research also shows that people experience more difficulty empathizing with others who are different from them in characteristics such as status, culture, religion, language, skin colour, gender, and age.[161]

To build intercultural empathy in others, psychologists employ empathy training. US researchers William Weeks, Paul Pedersen, et al. state that people who develop intercultural empathy can interpret experiences or perspectives from more than one worldview.[163] Intercultural empathy can also improve self-awareness and critical awareness of one’s own interaction style as conditioned by one’s cultural views[164] and promote a view of self-as-process.[165]
An alternative European approach to intercultural leadership considers four main dimensions: Cognitive Leadership, Affective Leadership, Relational leadership, and Emotional Leadership.[166]

Practical issues[edit]

The capacity to empathize is a revered trait in society.[31] Empathy is considered a motivating factor for unselfish, prosocial behavior,[167] whereas a lack of empathy is related to antisocial behavior.[31][168][169][170]

Apart from the automatic tendency to recognize the emotions of others, one may also deliberately engage in empathic reasoning. Such empathic engagement helps an individual understand and anticipate the behavior of another. Two general methods have been identified: An individual may mentally simulate fictitious versions of the beliefs, desires, character traits, and context of another individual to see what emotional feelings this provokes. Or, an individual may simulate an emotional feeling and then analyze the environment to discover a suitable reason for the emotional feeling to be appropriate for that specific environment.[52]

An empathizer’s own emotional background may affect or distort how they perceive the emotions in others.[171] Societies that promote individualism have lower ability for empathy[clarification needed].[172] The judgments that empathy provides about the emotional states of others are not certain ones. Empathy is a skill that gradually develops throughout life, and which improves the more contact we have with the person with whom one empathizes[clarification needed].

Empathizers report finding it easier to take the perspective of another person in a situation when they have experienced a similar situation,[173] and that they experience greater empathic understanding.[174] Research regarding whether similar past experience makes the empathizer more accurate is mixed.[173][174]

The extent to which a person’s emotions are publicly observable, or mutually recognized as such has significant social consequences. Empathic recognition may or may not be welcomed or socially desirable.[example needed] This is particularly the case when we recognize the emotions that someone has towards us during real time interactions. Based on a metaphorical affinity with touch, philosopher Edith Wyschogrod claims that the proximity entailed by empathy increases the potential vulnerability of either party.[175]

Benefits of empathizing[edit]

People who score more highly on empathy questionnaires also report having more positive relationships with other people. They report «greater life satisfaction, more positive affect, less negative affect, and less depressive symptoms than people who had lower empathy scores.»[176]

Children who exhibit more empathy also have more resilience.[177]

Empathy can be an aesthetic pleasure, «by widening the scope of that which we experience… by providing us with more than one perspective of a situation, thereby multiplying our experience… and… by intensifying that experience.»[178] People can use empathy to borrow joy from the joy of children discovering things or playing make-believe, or to satisfy our curiosity about other people’s lives.[179]

Empathic inaccuracy[edit]

People can severely overestimate how much they understand others.[180] When people empathize with another, they may oversimplify that other person in order to make them more legible.[178] It may improve empathic accuracy for the empathizer to explicitly ask the person empathized with for confirmation of the empathic hypothesis.[181] However, people may be reluctant to abandon their empathic hypotheses even when they are explicitly denied.[178]

Because we oversimplify people in order to make them legible enough to empathize with, we can come to misapprehend how cohesive other people are. We may come to think of ourselves as lacking a strong, integral self in comparison. Fritz Breithaupt calls this the «empathic endowment effect.» Because the empathic person must temporarily dampen their own sense of self in order to empathize with the other, and because the other seems to have a magnified and extra-cohesive sense of self, the empathic person may suffer from this and may «project onto others the self that they are lacking» and envy «that which they must give up in order to be able to feel empathy: a strong self.»[178]

Problems created by too much empathy and empathic bias[edit]

Some research suggests that people are more able and willing to empathize with those most similar to themselves.[182] In particular, empathy increases with similarities in culture and living conditions. Empathy is more likely to occur between individuals whose interaction is more frequent.[183][184] A measure of how well a person can infer the specific content of another person’s thoughts and feelings was developed by William Ickes.[clarification needed][67] In one experiment, researchers gave two groups of men wristbands according to which football team they supported. Each participant received a mild electric shock, then watched another go through the same pain. When the wristbands matched, both brains flared[clarification needed]: with pain, and empathic pain. If they supported opposing teams, the observer was found to have little empathy.[185]

Psychologist Paul Bloom, author of Against Empathy, points out that this bias can result in tribalism and violent responses in the name of helping people of the same «tribe» or social group, for example when empathic bias is exploited by demagogues.[186] He proposes «rational compassion» as an alternative; one example is using effective altruism to decide on charitable donations rationally, rather than by relying on emotional responses to images in the media.[186] Empathy can also be exploited by sympathetic beggars. Bloom points to the example of street children in India, who can get many donations because they are adorable but this results in their enslavement by organized crime. Bloom says that though someone might feel better about themselves and find more meaning[clarification needed] when they give to the person in front of them, in some cases they would do less harm and in many cases do more good in the world by giving to an effective charity through an impersonal website.[186]

Bloom believes improper use of empathy and social intelligence can lead to shortsighted actions and parochialism.[81] He further defies conventional supportive research findings as gremlins from biased standards.[clarification needed]

Bloom says that although psychopaths have low empathy, the correlation between low empathy and violent behavior as documented in scientific studies is «zero».[186] Other measures are much more predictive of violent behavior, such as lack of self-control.[186] People with Asperger syndrome and autism also have low empathy, but are more often the victim of violent attacks than the perpetrators.[186]

Bloom points out that parents who have too much short-term empathy might create long-term problems for their children, by neglecting discipline, helicopter parenting, or deciding not to get their children vaccinated because of the short-term discomfort.[186] People experiencing too much empathy after a disaster may continue to send donations like canned goods or used clothing even after being asked to stop or send cash instead, and this can make the situation worse by creating the need to dispose of useless donations and taking resources away from helpful activities.[186] Bloom also finds empathy can encourage unethical behavior when it causes people to care more about attractive people than ugly people, or people of one’s own race vs. people of a different race.[186] The attractiveness bias can also affect wildlife conservation efforts, increasing the amount of money devoted and laws passed to protect cute and photogenic animals, while taking attention away from species that are more ecologically important.[186]

Empathy and power[edit]

People tend to empathize less when they have more social or political power. For example, people from lower-class backgrounds exhibit better empathic accuracy than those from upper-class backgrounds.[187]

In a variety of «priming» experiments, people who were asked to recall a situation in which they had power over someone else then demonstrated reduced ability to mirror others, to comprehend their viewpoints, or to learn from their perspectives.[188]

Empathic distress fatigue[edit]

Excessive empathy can lead to «empathic distress fatigue», especially if it is associated with pathological altruism. The medical[clarification needed] risks are fatigue, occupational burnout, guilt, shame, anxiety, and depression.[189][190]

Tania Singer says that health care workers and caregivers must be objective regarding the emotions of others. They should not over-invest their own emotions in the other, at the risk of draining away their own resourcefulness.[191] Paul Bloom points out that high-empathy nurses tend to spend less time with their patients, to avoid feeling negative emotions associated with witnessing suffering.[186]

Empathy backfire[edit]

According to a new study, despite empathy being often portrayed as a positive attribute, whether or not the people who express empathy are viewed favorably depends on who they show empathy for. Such is the case in which a third party observes a subject showing empathy for someone of questionable character or generally viewed as unethical, that third party might not like or respect the subject for it, according to the new findings. This is called «empathy backfire».[192]

Disciplinary approaches[edit]

Philosophy[edit]

Ethics[edit]

In the 2007 book The Ethics of Care and Empathy, philosopher Michael Slote introduces a theory of care-based ethics that is grounded in empathy. His claim is that moral motivation does, and should, stem from a basis of empathic response. He claims that our natural reaction to situations of moral significance are explained by empathy. He explains that the limits and obligations of empathy and in turn morality are natural. These natural obligations include a greater empathic and moral obligation to family and friends and to those close to us in time and space. Our moral obligation to such people seems naturally stronger to us than that to strangers at a distance. Slote explains that this is due to the natural process of empathy. He asserts that actions are wrong if and only if they reflect or exhibit a deficiency of fully developed empathic concern for others on the part of the agent.[193]

Phenomenology[edit]

In phenomenology, empathy describes the experience of something from the other’s viewpoint, without confusion between self and other. This draws on[clarification needed] the sense of agency. In the most basic sense, this is the experience of the other’s body as «my body over there». In most other respects, however, what is experienced is experienced as being the other’s experience; in experiencing empathy, what is experienced is not «my» experience, even though I experience it. Empathy is also considered to be the condition of intersubjectivity and, as such, the source of the constitution of objectivity[jargon].[194]

History[edit]

Some postmodern historians such as Keith Jenkins have debated whether or not it is possible to empathize with people from the past. Jenkins argues that empathy only enjoys such a privileged position in the present because it corresponds harmoniously with the dominant liberal discourse of modern society and can be connected to John Stuart Mill’s concept of reciprocal freedom. Jenkins argues the past is a foreign country and as we do not have access to the epistemological conditions of bygone ages we are unable to empathize with those who lived then.[195]

Psychotherapy[edit]

Heinz Kohut introduced the principle of empathy in psychoanalysis. His principle applies to the method of gathering unconscious material.

Business and management[edit]

Because empathy seems to have potential to improve customer relations, employee morale, and personnel management capability, it has been studied in a business context.

In the 2009 book Wired to Care, strategy consultant Dev Patnaik argues that a major flaw in contemporary business practice is a lack of empathy inside large corporations. He states that without empathy people inside companies struggle to make intuitive decisions and often get fooled into believing they understand their business if they have quantitative research to rely upon. He says that companies can create a sense of empathy for customers, pointing to Nike, Harley-Davidson, and IBM as examples of «Open Empathy Organizations». Such companies, he claims, see new opportunities more quickly than competitors, adapt to change more easily, and create workplaces that offer employees a greater sense of mission in their jobs.[196] In the 2011 book The Empathy Factor, organizational consultant Marie Miyashiro similarly argues for bringing empathy to the workplace, and suggests Nonviolent Communication as an effective mechanism for achieving this.[197] In studies by the Management Research Group, empathy was found to be the strongest predictor of ethical leadership behavior out of 22 competencies in its management model, and empathy was one of the three strongest predictors of senior executive effectiveness.[198] A study by the Center for Creative Leadership found empathy to be positively correlated to job performance among employees as well.[199]

The leadership consulting firm Development Dimensions International found in 2016 that 20% of U.S. employers offered empathy training to managers.[200]

Patricia Moore pioneered using empathic techniques to better understand customers. For example, she used makeup and prosthetics to simulate the experience of elderly people, and used the insights from this to inspire friendlier products for that customer segment.[201] Design engineers at Ford Motor Company wore prosthetics to simulate pregnancy and old age, to help them design cars that would work better for such customers.[202] Fidelity Investments trains its telephone customer service employees in a virtual reality app that puts them in a (dramatized) customer’s home so they can experience what it is like to be on the other side of their conversations.[203]

Evolution of cooperation[edit]

Empathic perspective-taking plays important roles in sustaining cooperation in human societies, as studied by evolutionary game theory. In game theoretical models, indirect reciprocity refers to the mechanism of cooperation based on moral reputations that are assigned to individuals based on their perceived adherence a set of moral rules called social norms. It has been shown that if reputations are relative[clarification needed] and individuals disagree on the moral standing of others (for example, because they use different moral evaluation rules or make errors of judgement), then cooperation will not be sustained. However, when individuals have the capacity for empathic perspective-taking, altruistic behavior can once again evolve.[42] Moreover, evolutionary models also revealed that empathic perspective-taking itself can evolve, promoting prosocial behavior in human populations.[204]

Measurement[edit]

Efforts to measure empathy go back to at least the mid-twentieth century.[11][205] Researchers approach the measurement of empathy from a number of perspectives.

Behavioral measures normally involve raters assessing the presence or absence of certain either predetermined or ad hoc[clarification needed] behaviors in the subjects they are monitoring. Both verbal and non-verbal behaviors have been captured on video by experimenters such as Truax.[206] Other experimenters, including Mehrabian and Epstein,[207] required subjects to comment upon their own feelings and behaviors, or those of other people involved in the experiment, as indirect ways of signaling their level of empathic functioning to the raters.

Physiological responses tend to be captured by elaborate electronic equipment that has been physically connected to the subject’s body. Researchers then draw inferences about that person’s empathic reactions from the electronic readings produced.[208]

Bodily or «somatic» measures can be seen as behavioral measures at a micro level. They measure empathy through facial and other non-verbally expressed reactions. Such changes are presumably underpinned by physiological changes brought about by some form of «emotional contagion» or mirroring.[208] These reactions, while they appear to reflect the internal emotional state of the empathizer, could also, if the stimulus incident lasted more than the briefest period, reflect the results of emotional reactions based on cognitions associated with role-taking («if I were him I would feel…»).

Picture or puppet-story indices for empathy have been adopted to enable even very young, pre-school subjects to respond without needing to read questions and write answers.[209] Dependent variables (variables that are monitored for any change by the experimenter) for younger subjects have included self reporting on a seven-point smiley face scale and filmed facial reactions.[210]

In some experiments, subjects are required to watch video scenarios (either staged or authentic) and to make written responses which are then assessed for their levels of empathy;[211] scenarios are sometimes also depicted in printed form.[212]

Self-report measures[edit]

Measures of empathy also frequently require subjects to self-report upon their own ability or capacity for empathy, using Likert-style numerical responses to a printed questionnaire that may have been designed to reveal the affective, cognitive-affective, or largely cognitive substrates of empathic functioning. Some questionnaires claim to reveal both cognitive and affective substrates.[213] However, a 2019 meta analysis questions the validity of self-report measures of cognitive empathy, finding that such self-report measures have negligibly small correlations with corresponding behavioral measures.[41]

Such measures are also vulnerable to measuring not empathy but the difference between a person’s felt empathy and their standards for how much empathy is appropriate. For example, one researcher found that students scored themselves as less empathetic after taking her empathy class. After learning more about empathy, the students became more exacting in how they judged their own feelings and behavior, expected more from themselves, and so rated themselves more severely.[214]

In the field of medicine, a measurement tool for carers is the Jefferson Scale of Physician Empathy, Health Professional Version (JSPE-HP).[215]

The Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) is among the oldest published measurement tools still in frequent use (first published in 1983) that provides a multi-dimensional assessment of empathy. It comprises a self-report questionnaire of 28 items, divided into four 7-item scales covering the subdivisions of affective and cognitive empathy described above.[31][34] More recent self-report tools include The Empathy Quotient (EQ) created by Baron-Cohen and Wheelwright[216] which comprises a self-report questionnaire consisting of 60 items. Another multi-dimensional scale is the Questionnaire of Cognitive and Affective Empathy (QCAE, first published in 2011).[217]

The Empathic Experience Scale is a 30-item questionnaire that measures empathy from a phenomenological perspective on intersubjectivity, which provides a common basis for the perceptual experience (vicarious experience dimension) and a basic cognitive awareness (intuitive understanding dimension) of others’ emotional states.[218]

It is difficult to make comparisons over time using such questionnaires because of how language changes. For example, one study used a single questionnaire to measure 13,737 college students between 1979 and 2009, and found that empathy scores fell substantially over that time.[219] A critic noted these results could be because the wording of the questionnaire had become anachronistically quaint (it used idioms no longer in common use, like “tender feelings”, “ill at ease”, “quite touched”, or “go to pieces” that today’s students might not identify with).[220]

International comparison of country-wide empathy[edit]

In a 2016 study by a US research team, self-report data from the Interreactivity Index (see Measurement) were compared across countries. From the surveyed nations, the nations with the five highest empathy scores were (in descending order): Ecuador, Saudi Arabia, Peru, Denmark, and United Arab Emirates. The lowest scores came from Bulgaria, Poland, Estonia, Venezuela, and Lithuania.[221]

Other animals and empathy between species[edit]

Researchers Zanna Clay and Frans de Waal studied the socio-emotional development of the bonobo chimpanzee.[222] They focused on the interplay of numerous skills such as empathy-related responding, and how different rearing backgrounds of the juvenile bonobo affected their response to stressful events—events related to themselves (e.g. loss of a fight) as well as stressful events of others. They found that bonobos sought out body contact with one another as a coping mechanism. Bonobos sought out more body contact after watching an event distress other bonobos than after their individually experienced stressful event. Mother-reared bonobos sought out more physical contact than orphaned bonobos after a stressful event happened to another. This finding shows the importance of mother-child attachment and bonding in successful socio-emotional development, such as empathic-like behaviors.

Empathic-like behavior has been observed in chimpanzees in different aspects of their natural behaviors. For example, chimpanzees spontaneously contribute comforting behaviors to victims of aggressive behavior in both natural and unnatural settings, a behavior recognized as[who?] consolation. Researchers led by Teresa Romero observed these empathic and sympathetic-like behaviors in chimpanzees in two separate outdoor housed[clarification needed] groups.[223] Acts of consolation were observed in both groups. This behavior is found in humans, particularly in human infants. Another similarity found between chimpanzees and humans is that empathic-like responding was disproportionately provided to kin. Although comforting towards non-family chimpanzees was also observed, as with humans, chimpanzees showed the majority of comfort and concern to close/loved ones. Another similarity between chimpanzee and human expression of empathy is that females provided more comfort than males on average. The only exception to this discovery was that high-ranking males showed as much empathy-like behavior as their female counterparts. This is believed to be because of policing-like behavior and the authoritative status of high-ranking male chimpanzees.

Canines have been hypothesized to share empathic-like responding towards human species. Researchers Custance and Mayer put individual dogs in an enclosure with their owner and a stranger.[224] When the participants were talking or humming, the dog showed no behavioral changes; however when the participants were pretending to cry, the dogs oriented their behavior toward the person in distress whether it be the owner or stranger. The dogs approached the participants when crying in a submissive fashion, by sniffing, licking, and nuzzling the distressed person. The dogs did not approach the participants in the usual form of excitement, tail wagging, or panting. Since the dogs did not direct their empathic-like responses only towards their owner, it is hypothesized that dogs generally seek out humans showing distressing body behavior. Although this could suggest that dogs have the cognitive capacity for empathy, it could also mean that domesticated dogs have learned to comfort distressed humans through generations of being rewarded for that specific behavior.

When witnessing chicks in distress, domesticated hens (Gallus gallus domesticus) show emotional and physiological responding. Researchers Edgar, Paul, and Nicol[225] found that in conditions where the chick was susceptible to danger, the mother hen’s heart rate increased, it sounded vocal alarms, it decreased its personal preening, and its body temperature increased. This responding happened whether or not the chick felt as if it were in danger. Mother hens experienced stress-induced hyperthermia only when the chick’s behavior correlated with the perceived threat. Animal maternal behavior may be perceived as empathy, however, it could be guided by the evolutionary principles of survival and not emotionality[clarification needed].

Humans can empathize with other species. One study of a sample of organisms showed that the strength of human empathic perceptions (and compassionate reactions) toward an organism is negatively correlated with how long ago our species’ had a common ancestor. In other words, the more phylogenetically close a species is to us, the more likely we are to feel empathy and compassion towards it.[226]

In fiction[edit]

Lynn Hunt argued in Inventing Human Rights: A History that the concept of human rights developed how it did and when it did in part as a result of the influence of mid-eighteenth-century European novelists, particularly those whose use of the epistolatory novel form gave readers a more vivid sense that they were gaining access to the candid details of a real life. «The epistolatory novel did not just reflect important cultural and social changes of the time. Novel reading actually helped create new kinds of feelings including a recognition of shared psychological experiences, and these feelings then translated into new cultural and social movements including human rights.»[227]

The power of empathy has become a frequent ability in fiction, specifically in that of superhero media. Users, known as «empaths,» have the ability to sense/feel the emotions and bodily sensations of others and, in some cases, influence or control them.

Although sometimes a specific power held by users such as Marvel Comics character Empath, the power has also been frequently linked to that of telepathy such as in the case of Jean Grey.

The rebooted television series Charmed sees the character Maggie Vera as a witch with the power of empathy. Her powers later expand to allow her to control the emotions of others as well as occasionally concentrate emotion into pure energy. In Season 4 she learns to replicate people’s powers by empathically understanding them.

See also[edit]

  • Against Empathy: The Case for Rational Compassion (book by Paul Bloom)
  • Artificial empathy
  • Attribution (psychology)
  • Digital empathy
  • Emotional intelligence
  • Emotional literacy
  • Empathic concern
  • Empathizing–systemizing theory
  • Empathy in chickens
  • Empathy in literature
  • Empathy in online communities
  • Empathism
  • Ethnocultural empathy
  • Grounding in communication
  • Highly sensitive person
  • Humanistic coefficient
  • Identification (psychology)
  • Life skills
  • Mimpathy
  • Mirror-touch synesthesia
  • Moral emotions
  • Oxytocin
  • People skills
  • Philip K. Dick’s Do Androids Dream of Electric Sheep?
  • Rapport
  • Schema (psychology)
  • Self-conscious emotions
  • Sensibility
  • Simulation theory of empathy
  • Social emotions
  • Soft skills
  • Theory of mind in animals
  • Vicarious embarrassment

References[edit]

  1. ^ Bellet PS, Maloney MJ (October 1991). «The importance of empathy as an interviewing skill in medicine». JAMA. 266 (13): 1831–2. doi:10.1001/jama.1991.03470130111039. PMID 1909761.
  2. ^ Rothschild, B. (with Rand, M. L.). (2006). Help for the Helper: The psychophysiology of compassion fatigue and vicarious trauma.
  3. ^ Read H (August 22, 2019). «A typology of empathy and its many moral forms». Philosophy Compass. 14 (10). doi:10.1111/phc3.12623. S2CID 202396600.
  4. ^ «The relationship of nursing students’ spiritual care perspectives to their expressions of spiritual empathy» Chism, Lisa Astalos ; Magnan, Morris A. The Journal of nursing education, 2009–11, Vol. 48 (11), p. 597–605; United States
  5. ^ Harper D. «empathy». Online Etymology Dictionary.
  6. ^ ἐμπάθεια. Liddell, Henry George; Scott, Robert; A Greek–English Lexicon at the Perseus Project.
  7. ^ Elizabeth A. Segal, et al. Assessing Empathy (2017), chapter 1
  8. ^ Titchener EB (2014). «Introspection and empath» (PDF). Dialogues in Philosophy, Mental and Neuro Sciences. 7: 25–30. Archived from the original (PDF) on July 26, 2014.
    • Gallese V (2003). «The roots of empathy: the shared manifold hypothesis and the neural basis of intersubjectivity». Psychopathology. 36 (4): 171–80. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.143.2396. doi:10.1159/000072786. PMID 14504450. S2CID 9422028.
    • Koss J (March 2006). «On the Limits of Empathy». The Art Bulletin. 88 (1): 139–157. doi:10.1080/00043079.2006.10786282. JSTOR 25067229. S2CID 194079190.

  9. ^ «εμπάθεια». Glosbe. Glosbe dictionary. Retrieved April 26, 2019.
  10. ^ Batson, C. Daniel (2011). Altruism in Humans.
  11. ^ a b Lanzoni, Susan Marie (2018). Empathy : a history. New Haven. ISBN 978-0-300-22268-5.
  12. ^ Hall, Judith A.; Schwartz, Rachel (May 4, 2019). «Empathy present and future». The Journal of Social Psychology. 159 (3): 225–243. doi:10.1080/00224545.2018.1477442. PMID 29781776. S2CID 29167108.
  13. ^ Hall, Judith A.; Schwartz, Rachel; Duong, Fred (January 2, 2021). «How do laypeople define empathy?». The Journal of Social Psychology. 161 (1): 5–24. doi:10.1080/00224545.2020.1796567. PMID 32870130. S2CID 221405375.
  14. ^ a b c Pijnenborg GH, Spikman JM, Jeronimus BF, Aleman A (June 2013). «Insight in schizophrenia: associations with empathy». European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience. 263 (4): 299–307. doi:10.1007/s00406-012-0373-0. PMID 23076736. S2CID 25194328.
  15. ^ Hodges SD, Klein KJ (September 2001). «Regulating the costs of empathy: the price of being human» (PDF). The Journal of Socio-Economics. 30 (5): 437–52. doi:10.1016/S1053-5357(01)00112-3.
  16. ^ Baird JD, Nadel L (April 2010). Happiness Genes: Unlock the Positive Potential Hidden in Your DNA. New Page Books. ISBN 978-1-60163-105-3.
  17. ^ Singer, Tania; Engert, Veronika (August 2019). «It matters what you practice: differential training effects on subjective experience, behavior, brain and body in the ReSource Project». Current Opinion in Psychology. 28: 151–158. doi:10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.12.005. PMID 30684917. S2CID 59291558.
  18. ^ Weisz, Erika; Ong, Desmond C.; Carlson, Ryan W.; Zaki, Jamil (August 2021). «Building empathy through motivation-based interventions». Emotion. 21 (5): 990–999. doi:10.1037/emo0000929. PMID 33211508. S2CID 227079997.
  19. ^ Teding van Berkhout, Emily; Malouff, John M. (January 2016). «The efficacy of empathy training: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials». Journal of Counseling Psychology. 63 (1): 32–41. doi:10.1037/cou0000093. PMID 26191979.
  20. ^ Rathje, Steve; Hackel, Leor; Zaki, Jamil (July 2021). «Attending live theatre improves empathy, changes attitudes, and leads to pro-social behavior». Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 95: 104138. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2021.104138. S2CID 233549299.
  21. ^ O’Malley WJ (1999). «Teaching Empathy». America. 180 (12): 22–26.
  22. ^ Schwartz W (2002). «From passivity to competence: a conceptualization of knowledge, skill, tolerance, and empathy». Psychiatry. 65 (4): 339–45. doi:10.1521/psyc.65.4.338.20239. PMID 12530337. S2CID 35496086.
  23. ^ Schwartz W (2013). «The parameters of empathy: Core considerations for psychotherapy and supervision». Advances in Descriptive Psychology. 10. doi:10.2139/ssrn.2393689.
  24. ^ Meltzoff AN, Decety J (March 2003). «What imitation tells us about social cognition: a rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience». Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences. 358 (1431): 491–500. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1261. PMC 1351349. PMID 12689375.
  25. ^ Gu, Jenny; Cavanagh, Kate; Baer, Ruth; Strauss, Clara (February 17, 2017). «An empirical examination of the factor structure of compassion». PLOS ONE. 12 (2): e0172471. Bibcode:2017PLoSO..1272471G. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0172471. PMC 5315311. PMID 28212391.
  26. ^ Strauss, Clara; Lever Taylor, Billie; Gu, Jenny; Kuyken, Willem; Baer, Ruth; Jones, Fergal; Cavanagh, Kate (July 2016). «What is compassion and how can we measure it? A review of definitions and measures». Clinical Psychology Review. 47: 15–27. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2016.05.004. PMID 27267346.
  27. ^ a b Batson CD (2009). «These things called empathy: Eight related but distinct phenomena.». In Decety J, Ickes W (eds.). The Social Neuroscience of Empathy. Cambridge: MIT Press. pp. 3–15. ISBN 9780262012973.
  28. ^ a b c Coplan, Amy (September 2011). «Will the real empathy please stand up? A case for a narrow conceptualization». The Southern Journal of Philosophy. 49: 40–65. doi:10.1111/j.2041-6962.2011.00056.x.
  29. ^ Hatfield E, Cacioppo JL, Rapson RL (1993). «Emotional contagion» (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2 (3): 96–99. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.ep10770953. S2CID 220533081. Archived from the original (PDF) on November 19, 2012.
  30. ^ Bar-On RE, Parker JD (2000). The Handbook of Emotional Intelligence: Theory, Development, Assessment, and Application at Home, School, and in the Workplace. San Francisco, California: Jossey-Bass. ISBN 0-7879-4984-1.
  31. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p Rogers K, Dziobek I, Hassenstab J, Wolf OT, Convit A (April 2007). «Who cares? Revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome» (PDF). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 37 (4): 709–15. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0197-8. PMID 16906462. S2CID 13999363. Archived (PDF) from the original on July 16, 2015.
  32. ^ a b c d e f g Shamay-Tsoory SG, Aharon-Peretz J, Perry D (March 2009). «Two systems for empathy: a double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions». Brain. 132 (Pt 3): 617–27. doi:10.1093/brain/awn279. PMID 18971202.
  33. ^ de Waal FB (2008). «Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy» (PDF). Annual Review of Psychology. 59 (1): 279–300. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625. PMID 17550343. Archived (PDF) from the original on April 17, 2012.
  34. ^ a b c Davis M (1983). «Measuring individual differences in empathy: evidence for a multidimensional approach». Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 44 (1): 113–126. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113. hdl:10983/25968.
  35. ^ a b c d e f g h i Minio-Paluello I, Lombardo MV, Chakrabarti B, Wheelwright S, Baron-Cohen S (December 2009). «Response to Smith’s Letter to the Editor «Emotional Empathy in Autism Spectrum Conditions: Weak, Intact, or Heightened?«». Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 39 (12): 1749. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0800-x. S2CID 42834991. Pdf. Archived March 4, 2016, at the Wayback Machine
    See also:

    Smith A (December 2009). «Emotional empathy in autism spectrum conditions: weak, intact, or heightened?». Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 39 (12): 1747–8, author reply 1749–54. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0799-z. PMID 19572192. S2CID 13290717.

  36. ^ a b c Lamm C, Batson CD, Decety J (January 2007). «The neural substrate of human empathy: effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal». Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 19 (1): 42–58. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.511.3950. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.1.42. PMID 17214562. S2CID 2828843.
  37. ^ Segal, Elizabeth A. (2017). Assessing Empathy.
  38. ^ a b c Baron-Cohen S (2003). The Essential Difference: The Truth about the Male and Female Brain. Basic Books. ISBN 9780738208442.
  39. ^ Gerace A, Day A, Casey S, Mohr P (2013). «An exploratory investigation of the process of perspective taking in interpersonal situations». Journal of Relationships Research. 4: e6, 1–12. doi:10.1017/jrr.2013.6.
  40. ^ Rogers K, Dziobek I, Hassenstab J, Wolf OT, Convit A (April 2007). «Who cares? Revisiting empathy in Asperger syndrome» (PDF). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 37 (4): 709–15. doi:10.1007/s10803-006-0197-8. PMID 16906462. S2CID 13999363. Archived (PDF) from the original on July 16, 2015.
  41. ^ a b Murphy BA, Lilienfeld SO (August 2019). «Are self-report cognitive empathy ratings valid proxies for cognitive empathy ability? Negligible meta-analytic relations with behavioral task performance». Psychological Assessment. 31 (8): 1062–1072. doi:10.1037/pas0000732. PMID 31120296. S2CID 162181339.
  42. ^ a b Radzvilavicius AL, Stewart AJ, Plotkin JB (April 2019). «Evolution of empathetic moral evaluation». eLife. 8: e44269. doi:10.7554/eLife.44269. PMC 6488294. PMID 30964002.
  43. ^ «The Tao of Doing Good (SSIR)». ssir.org. Archived from the original on February 13, 2017. Retrieved February 13, 2017.
  44. ^ McLaren, Karla (2013). The Art of Empathy: A Complete Guide to Life’s Most Essential Skill.
  45. ^ a b c d e f g Cox CL, Uddin LQ, Di Martino A, Castellanos FX, Milham MP, Kelly C (August 2012). «The balance between feeling and knowing: affective and cognitive empathy are reflected in the brain’s intrinsic functional dynamics». Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 7 (6): 727–37. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr051. PMC 3427869. PMID 21896497.
  46. ^ Winczewski LA, Bowen JD, Collins NL (March 2016). «Is Empathic Accuracy Enough to Facilitate Responsive Behavior in Dyadic Interaction? Distinguishing Ability From Motivation». Psychological Science. 27 (3): 394–404. doi:10.1177/0956797615624491. PMID 26847609. S2CID 206588127.
  47. ^ Schurz, Matthias; Radua, Joaquim; Tholen, Matthias G.; Maliske, Lara; Margulies, Daniel S.; Mars, Rogier B.; Sallet, Jerome; Kanske, Philipp (March 2021). «Toward a hierarchical model of social cognition: A neuroimaging meta-analysis and integrative review of empathy and theory of mind». Psychological Bulletin. 147 (3): 293–327. doi:10.1037/bul0000303. hdl:2066/226714. ISSN 1939-1455. PMID 33151703. S2CID 226272359.
  48. ^ Kanske P, Böckler A, Trautwein FM, Parianen Lesemann FH, Singer T (September 2016). «Are strong empathizers better mentalizers? Evidence for independence and interaction between the routes of social cognition». Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 11 (9): 1383–92. doi:10.1093/scan/nsw052. PMC 5015801. PMID 27129794.
    • Kanske P, Böckler A, Trautwein FM, Singer T (November 2015). «Dissecting the social brain: Introducing the EmpaToM to reveal distinct neural networks and brain-behavior relations for empathy and Theory of Mind». NeuroImage. 122: 6–19. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.07.082. PMID 26254589. S2CID 20614006.

  49. ^ Sandin J (2007). Bonobos: Encounters in Empathy. Milwaukee: Zoological Society of Milwaukee & The Foundation for Wildlife Conservation, Inc. p. 109. ISBN 978-0-9794151-0-4.
    • de Waal FB (2009). The age of empathy: nature’s lessons for a kinder society. Harmony Books. ISBN 9780307407764.

  50. ^ Ben-Ami Bartal I, Decety J, Mason P (December 2011). «Empathy and pro-social behavior in rats». Science. 334 (6061): 1427–30. Bibcode:2011Sci…334.1427B. doi:10.1126/science.1210789. PMC 3760221. PMID 22158823.
  51. ^ Langford DJ, Crager SE, Shehzad Z, Smith SB, Sotocinal SG, Levenstadt JS, et al. (June 2006). «Social modulation of pain as evidence for empathy in mice». Science. 312 (5782): 1967–70. Bibcode:2006Sci…312.1967L. doi:10.1126/science.1128322. PMID 16809545. S2CID 26027821.
  52. ^ a b de Waal FB (2008). «Putting the altruism back into altruism: the evolution of empathy». Annual Review of Psychology. 59 (1): 279–300. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093625. PMID 17550343.
  53. ^ Decety J (August 2011). «The neuroevolution of empathy». Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1231 (1): 35–45. Bibcode:2011NYASA1231…35D. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06027.x. PMID 21651564. S2CID 9895828.
  54. ^ Decety J, Svetlova M (January 2012). «Putting together phylogenetic and ontogenetic perspectives on empathy». Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. 2 (1): 1–24. doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2011.05.003. PMC 6987713. PMID 22682726.
  55. ^ Hoffman ML (2000). Empathy and Moral Development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 9780521580342.
  56. ^ Decety J, Meyer M (2008). «From emotion resonance to empathic understanding: a social developmental neuroscience account». Development and Psychopathology. 20 (4): 1053–80. doi:10.1017/S0954579408000503. PMID 18838031. S2CID 8508693.
    • Eisenberg N, Spinrad TL, Sadovsky A (2006). «Empathy-related responding in children.». In Killen M, Smetana J (eds.). Handbook of Moral Development. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 517–549. ISBN 9780805847512.

  57. ^ Falck-Ytter T, Gredebäck G, von Hofsten C (July 2006). «Infants predict other people’s action goals». Nature Neuroscience. 9 (7): 878–9. doi:10.1038/nn1729. PMID 16783366. S2CID 2409686.
  58. ^ Zahn-Waxler C, Radke-Yarrow M (1990). «The origins of empathic concern». Motivation and Emotion. 14 (2): 107–130. doi:10.1007/BF00991639. S2CID 143436918.
  59. ^ a b c Decety J, Michalska KJ, Akitsuki Y (September 2008). «Who caused the pain? An fMRI investigation of empathy and intentionality in children». Neuropsychologia. 46 (11): 2607–14. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.05.026. PMID 18573266. S2CID 19428145.
  60. ^ Wimmer H, Perner J (January 1983). «Beliefs about beliefs: representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception». Cognition. 13 (1): 103–28. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(83)90004-5. PMID 6681741. S2CID 17014009.
  61. ^ Baron-Cohen S, Leslie AM, Frith U (October 1985). «Does the autistic child have a «theory of mind»?». Cognition. 21 (1): 37–46. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8. PMID 2934210. S2CID 14955234.
  62. ^ Leslie AM, Frith U (November 1988). «Autistic children’s understanding of seeing, knowing and believing». British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 6 (4): 315–324. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.1988.tb01104.x.
  63. ^ Olsen DP (September 2001). «Empathetic maturity: theory of moral point of view in clinical relations». Advances in Nursing Science. 24 (1): 36–46. doi:10.1097/00012272-200109000-00006. PMID 11554532. Archived from the original on September 7, 2009.
  64. ^ Davis MH (1983). «Measuring Individual Differences in Empathy: Evidence for a Multidimensional Approach». Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 44 (1): 113–26. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113. hdl:10983/25968.
  65. ^ Haas BW, Brook M, Remillard L, Ishak A, Anderson IW, Filkowski MM (2015). «I know how you feel: the warm-altruistic personality profile and the empathic brain». PLOS ONE. 10 (3): e0120639. Bibcode:2015PLoSO..1020639H. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0120639. PMC 4359130. PMID 25769028.
  66. ^ a b c d Bosson, Jennifer K.; Buckner, Camille E.; Vandello, Joseph A. (2021). The Psychology of Sex and Gender. Sage Publications. p. 330. ISBN 978-1-54-439403-9.
  67. ^ a b Ickes W (1997). Empathic accuracy. New York: The Guilford Press.
  68. ^ a b c d e f g h i Christov-Moore L, Simpson EA, Coudé G, Grigaityte K, Iacoboni M, Ferrari PF (October 2014). «Empathy: gender effects in brain and behavior». Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 46 Pt 4 (Pt 4): 604–27. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.001. PMC 5110041. PMID 25236781. Archived from the original on August 14, 2017.
  69. ^ a b Kret ME, De Gelder B (June 2012). «A review on sex differences in processing emotional signals». Neuropsychologia. 50 (7): 1211–21. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.022. PMID 22245006. S2CID 11695245.
  70. ^ Thompson AE, Voyer D (January 1, 2014). «Sex differences in the ability to recognise non-verbal displays of emotion: a meta-analysis». Cognition & Emotion. 28 (7): 1164–95. doi:10.1080/02699931.2013.875889. PMID 24400860. S2CID 5402395.
  71. ^ Decety, Jean; Bartal, Inbal Ben-Ami; Uzefovsky, Florina; Knafo-Noam, Ariel (January 19, 2016). «Empathy as a driver of prosocial behaviour: highly conserved neurobehavioural mechanisms across species». Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 371 (1686): 20150077. doi:10.1098/rstb.2015.0077. ISSN 0962-8436. PMC 4685523. PMID 26644596.
  72. ^ Tisot CM (2003). Environmental contributions to empathy development in young children (PhD thesis). Temple University. OCLC 56772472.
  73. ^ Leigh R, Oishi K, Hsu J, Lindquist M, Gottesman RF, Jarso S, et al. (August 2013). «Acute lesions that impair affective empathy». Brain. 136 (Pt 8): 2539–49. doi:10.1093/brain/awt177. PMC 3722353. PMID 23824490.
  74. ^ de Sousa A, McDonald S, Rushby J (July 1, 2012). «Changes in emotional empathy, affective responsivity, and behavior following severe traumatic brain injury». Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 34 (6): 606–23. doi:10.1080/13803395.2012.667067. PMID 22435955. S2CID 44373955.
  75. ^ de Sousa A, McDonald S, Rushby J, Li S, Dimoska A, James C (October 2010). «Why don’t you feel how I feel? Insight into the absence of empathy after severe traumatic brain injury». Neuropsychologia. 48 (12): 3585–95. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.008. PMID 20713073. S2CID 25275909.
  76. ^ van Berkhout, E. Teding; Malouff, J.M. (2016). «The efficacy of empathy training: A meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials». Journal of Counseling Psychology. 63 (1): 32–41. doi:10.1037/cou0000093. PMID 26191979.
  77. ^ a b Hoffman ML (1990). «Empathy and justice motivation». Motivation and Emotion. 14 (2): 151–172. doi:10.1007/BF00991641. S2CID 143830768.
  78. ^ Hoffman 2000, p. 101.
  79. ^ Vitaglione GD, Barnett MA (December 2003). «Assessing a new dimension of empathy: Empathic anger as a predictor of helping and punishing desires». Motivation and Emotion. 27 (4): 301–25. doi:10.1023/A:1026231622102. S2CID 143276552. Archived from the original on May 14, 2011.
  80. ^ Mohr P, Howells K, Gerace A, Day A, Wharton M (2007). «The role of perspective taking in anger arousal». Personality and Individual Differences. 43 (3): 507–517. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2006.12.019. hdl:2328/36189.
    • Day A, Mohr P, Howells K, Gerace A, Lim L (June 2012). «The role of empathy in anger arousal in violent offenders and university students» (PDF). International Journal of Offender Therapy and Comparative Criminology. 56 (4): 599–613. doi:10.1177/0306624X11431061. hdl:2328/35889. PMID 22158909. S2CID 46542250.

  81. ^ a b Bloom P (January 2017). «Empathy and Its Discontents». Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 21 (1): 24–31. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2016.11.004. PMID 27916513. S2CID 3863278.
  82. ^ Marjanovic Z, Struthers G (August 8, 2011). «Who Helps Natural-Disaster Victims? Assessment of Trait and Situational Predictors» (PDF). Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy. 12 (1): 245–267. doi:10.1111/j.1530-2415.2011.01262.x.
  83. ^ Einolf C (March 13, 2012). «Is Cognitive Empathy More Important than Affective Empathy? A Response to «Who Helps Natural-Disaster Victims?»» (PDF). Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy. 12 (1): 268–271. doi:10.1111/j.1530-2415.2012.01281.x. Retrieved May 30, 2014.
  84. ^ Petrocchi, Serena; Bernardi, Sheila; Malacrida, Roberto; Traber, Rafael; Gabutti, Luca; Grignoli, Nicola (December 2021). «Affective empathy predicts self-isolation behaviour acceptance during coronavirus risk exposure». Scientific Reports. 11 (1): 10153. Bibcode:2021NatSR..1110153P. doi:10.1038/s41598-021-89504-w. PMC 8115029. PMID 33980946.
  85. ^ Batson CD, Moran T (1999). «Empathy-induced altruism in a prisoner’s dilemma». Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 29 (7): 909–924. doi:10.1002/(sici)1099-0992(199911)29:7<909::aid-ejsp965>3.0.co;2-l.
  86. ^ Snyder CR, Lopez SJ, eds. (2009). Oxford Handbook of Positive Psychology (Second ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 243–44. ISBN 978-0-19-518724-3.
  87. ^ Einolf, Christopher J. (2008). «Empathic concern and prosocial behaviors: A test of experimental results using survey data». Social Science Research. 37 (4): 1267–1279. doi:10.1016/j.ssresearch.2007.06.003. PMID 19227702.
  88. ^ van Baaren, R.B.; Holland, R.W.; Kawakami, K.; van Knippenberg, A. (2004). «Mimicry and prosocial behavior». Psychological Science. 15 (1): 71–74. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01501012.x. PMID 14717835. S2CID 3681430.
    • Müller, B.C.N.; Maaskant, A.J.; van Baaren, R.B.; Dijksterhuis, A. (2012). «Prosocial consequences of imitation». Psychological Reports. 110 (3): 891–898. doi:10.2466/07.09.21.PR0.110.3.891-898. PMID 22897091. S2CID 13528009.
    • van Baaren, R.B.; Janssen, R.; Chartrand, T.L.; Dijksterhuis, A. (2009). «Where is the love? The social aspects of mimicry». Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society Biological Sciences. 364 (1528): 2381–2389. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0057. PMC 2865082. PMID 19620109.

  89. ^ Davis MH, Luce C, Kraus SJ (September 1994). «The heritability of characteristics associated with dispositional empathy». Journal of Personality. 62 (3): 369–91. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1994.tb00302.x. PMID 7965564.
  90. ^ Todd RM, Anderson AK (November 2009). «The neurogenetics of remembering emotions past». Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 106 (45): 18881–2. Bibcode:2009PNAS..10618881T. doi:10.1073/pnas.0910755106. PMC 2776429. PMID 19889977.
    • Todd RM, Ehlers MR, Müller DJ, Robertson A, Palombo DJ, Freeman N, et al. (April 2015). «Neurogenetic variations in norepinephrine availability enhance perceptual vividness». The Journal of Neuroscience. 35 (16): 6506–16. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4489-14.2015. PMC 6605217. PMID 25904801.

  91. ^ Naudts KH, Azevedo RT, David AS, van Heeringen K, Gibbs AA (September 2012). «Epistasis between 5-HTTLPR and ADRA2B polymorphisms influences attentional bias for emotional information in healthy volunteers». The International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology. 15 (8): 1027–36. doi:10.1017/S1461145711001295. PMID 21854681. Pdf. Archived October 23, 2017, at the Wayback Machine
  92. ^ Saphire-Bernstein S, Way BM, Kim HS, Sherman DK, Taylor SE (September 2011). «Oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) is related to psychological resources». Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 108 (37): 15118–22. Bibcode:2011PNAS..10815118S. doi:10.1073/pnas.1113137108. PMC 3174632. PMID 21896752.
  93. ^ Warrier V, Grasby KL, Uzefovsky F, Toro R, Smith P, Chakrabarti B, et al. (June 2018). «Genome-wide meta-analysis of cognitive empathy: heritability, and correlates with sex, neuropsychiatric conditions and cognition». Molecular Psychiatry. 23 (6): 1402–1409. doi:10.1038/mp.2017.122. PMC 5656177. PMID 28584286.
  94. ^ a b Keen S (2006). «A Theory of Narrative Empathy». Narrative. 14 (3): 207–36. doi:10.1353/nar.2006.0015. S2CID 52228354.
  95. ^ Gazzola V, Aziz-Zadeh L, Keysers C (September 2006). «Empathy and the somatotopic auditory mirror system in humans». Current Biology. 16 (18): 1824–9. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2006.07.072. PMID 16979560. S2CID 5223812.
  96. ^ Bartlett MY, DeSteno D (April 2006). «Gratitude and prosocial behavior: helping when it costs you». Psychological Science. 17 (4): 319–25. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01705.x. PMID 16623689. S2CID 6491264.
  97. ^ Fan Y, Duncan NW, de Greck M, Northoff G (January 2011). «Is there a core neural network in empathy? An fMRI based quantitative meta-analysis». Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 35 (3): 903–11. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.10.009. PMID 20974173. S2CID 20965340.
    • Eres R, Decety J, Louis WR, Molenberghs P (August 2015). «Individual differences in local gray matter density are associated with differences in affective and cognitive empathy». NeuroImage. 117: 305–10. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.038. PMID 26008886. S2CID 15373798. Archived from the original on September 8, 2017.

  98. ^ Keysers C, Gazzola V (December 2009). «Expanding the mirror: vicarious activity for actions, emotions, and sensations». Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 19 (6): 666–71. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2009.10.006. PMID 19880311. S2CID 2692907.
    • Decety J, Moriguchi Y (November 2007). «The empathic brain and its dysfunction in psychiatric populations: implications for intervention across different clinical conditions». BioPsychoSocial Medicine. 1 (1): 22. doi:10.1186/1751-0759-1-22. PMC 2206036. PMID 18021398.

  99. ^ Wicker B, Keysers C, Plailly J, Royet JP, Gallese V, Rizzolatti G (October 2003). «Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust». Neuron. 40 (3): 655–64. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(03)00679-2. PMID 14642287.
  100. ^ Keysers C, Wicker B, Gazzola V, Anton JL, Fogassi L, Gallese V (April 2004). «A touching sight: SII/PV activation during the observation and experience of touch». Neuron. 42 (2): 335–46. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(04)00156-4. PMID 15091347.
    • Blakemore SJ, Bristow D, Bird G, Frith C, Ward J (July 2005). «Somatosensory activations during the observation of touch and a case of vision-touch synaesthesia». Brain. 128 (Pt 7): 1571–83. doi:10.1093/brain/awh500. PMID 15817510.

  101. ^ Morrison I, Lloyd D, di Pellegrino G, Roberts N (June 2004). «Vicarious responses to pain in anterior cingulate cortex: is empathy a multisensory issue?». Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience. 4 (2): 270–8. doi:10.3758/cabn.4.2.270. PMID 15460933.
    • Jackson PL, Meltzoff AN, Decety J (February 2005). «How do we perceive the pain of others? A window into the neural processes involved in empathy». NeuroImage. 24 (3): 771–9. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.391.8127. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.09.006. PMID 15652312. S2CID 10691796.
    • Singer T, Seymour B, O’Doherty J, Kaube H, Dolan RJ, Frith CD (February 2004). «Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain». Science. 303 (5661): 1157–62. Bibcode:2004Sci…303.1157S. doi:10.1126/science.1093535. hdl:21.11116/0000-0001-A020-5. PMID 14976305. S2CID 14727944.

  102. ^ Preston SD, de Waal FB (February 2002). «Empathy: Its ultimate and proximate bases». The Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 25 (1): 1–20, discussion 20–71. doi:10.1017/s0140525x02000018. PMID 12625087.
  103. ^ Gutsell JN, Inzlicht M (2010). «Empathy constrained: Prejudice predicts reduced mental simulation of actions during observation of outgroups». Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 46 (5): 841–845. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.011.
  104. ^ Jack AI, Dawson AJ, Begany KL, Leckie RL, Barry KP, Ciccia AH, Snyder AZ (February 2013). «fMRI reveals reciprocal inhibition between social and physical cognitive domains». NeuroImage. 66: 385–401. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.10.061. PMC 3602121. PMID 23110882.
    • Case Western Reserve University (October 30, 2012). «Empathy represses analytic thought, and vice versa: Brain physiology limits simultaneous use of both networks». Science Daily. Archived from the original on October 24, 2017.

  105. ^ Thomas B (November 6, 2012). «What’s so special about mirror neurons? (guest blog)». Scientific American. New York. Archived from the original on May 21, 2015.
  106. ^ Marsh J (March 29, 2012). «Do mirror neurons give us empathy?». Greater Good Magazine. Greater Good Science Center. Archived from the original on October 24, 2017.
    See also:

    Ramachandran VS (2011). The tell-tale brain: a neuroscientist’s quest for what makes us human. New York: W.W. Norton. ISBN 9780393077827.

  107. ^ a b Phoebe Caldwell, «Letters», London Times, Dec. 30 2005
  108. ^ a b Baron-Cohen S (2011). Zero Degrees of Empathy: A New Theory of Human Cruelty. Penguin UK. ISBN 9780713997910. Retrieved August 8, 2013.
  109. ^ a b c Bora E, Gökçen S, Veznedaroglu B (July 2008). «Empathic abilities in people with schizophrenia». Psychiatry Research. 160 (1): 23–9. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2007.05.017. PMID 18514324. S2CID 20896840.
  110. ^ a b c Decety J, Michalska KJ, Akitsuki Y, Lahey BB (February 2009). «Atypical empathic responses in adolescents with aggressive conduct disorder: a functional MRI investigation». Biological Psychology. 80 (2): 203–11. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2008.09.004. PMC 2819310. PMID 18940230.
  111. ^ a b Grossman D (1996). On Killing: The Psychological Cost of Learning to Kill in War and Society. Back Bay Books. ISBN 978-0-316-33000-8.
  112. ^ Simons D, Wurtele SK, Heil P (December 1, 2002). «Childhood Victimization and Lack of Empathy as Predictors of Sexual Offending Against Women and Children». Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 17 (12): 1291–1307. doi:10.1177/088626002237857. ISSN 0886-2605. S2CID 145525384.
  113. ^ a b Hill E, Berthoz S, Frith U (April 2004). «Brief report: cognitive processing of own emotions in individuals with autistic spectrum disorder and in their relatives» (PDF). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 34 (2): 229–35. doi:10.1023/B:JADD.0000022613.41399.14. PMID 15162941. S2CID 776386. Archived from the original (PDF) on June 19, 2013.
  114. ^ Taylor, G.J., & Bagby, R.M, & Parker, J.D.A. Disorders of Affect Regulation: Alexithymia in Medical and Psychiatric Illness. (1997) Cambridge Uni. Press.
    • Sifneos PE (1973). «The prevalence of ‘alexithymic’ characteristics in psychosomatic patients». Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. 22 (2): 255–62. doi:10.1159/000286529. PMID 4770536.

  115. ^ Moriguchi Y, Decety J, Ohnishi T, Maeda M, Mori T, Nemoto K, Matsuda H, Komaki G (September 2007). «Empathy and judging other’s pain: an fMRI study of alexithymia». Cerebral Cortex. New York, N.Y. 17 (9): 2223–34. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhl130. PMID 17150987.
  116. ^ Brackett MA, Warner RM, Bosco JS (2005). «Emotional Intelligence and Relationship Quality Among Couples» (PDF). Personal Relationships. 12 (2): 197–212. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.385.3719. doi:10.1111/j.1350-4126.2005.00111.x. Archived from the original (PDF) on September 27, 2007.
  117. ^ Yelsma P, Marrow S (January 2003). «An examination of couples’ difficulties with emotional expressiveness and their marital satisfaction». The Journal of Family Communication. 3 (1): 41–62. doi:10.1207/S15327698JFC0301_03. S2CID 144200365.
  118. ^ Bird G, Silani G, Brindley R, White S, Frith U, Singer T (May 2010). «Empathic brain responses in insula are modulated by levels of alexithymia but not autism». Brain. 133 (Pt 5): 1515–25. doi:10.1093/brain/awq060. PMC 2859151. PMID 20371509.
  119. ^ Dapretto M, Davies MS, Pfeifer JH, Scott AA, Sigman M, Bookheimer SY, Iacoboni M (January 2006). «Understanding emotions in others: mirror neuron dysfunction in children with autism spectrum disorders». Nature Neuroscience. 9 (1): 28–30. doi:10.1038/nn1611. PMC 3713227. PMID 16327784.
  120. ^ Oberman LM, Hubbard EM, McCleery JP, Altschuler EL, Ramachandran VS, Pineda JA (July 2005). «EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism spectrum disorders». Brain Research. Cognitive Brain Research. 24 (2): 190–8. doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.014. PMID 15993757.
  121. ^ Iacoboni, M. (2005). Understanding others: Imitation, language, empathy. Perspectives on imitation: From cognitive neuroscience to social science 1, 77–99.
  122. ^ Gillberg CL (July 1992). «The Emanuel Miller Memorial Lecture 1991. Autism and autistic-like conditions: subclasses among disorders of empathy». Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 33 (5): 813–42. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1992.tb01959.x. PMID 1634591.
  123. ^ Roeyers H, Buysse A, Ponnet K, Pichal B (February 2001). «Advancing advanced mind-reading tests: empathic accuracy in adults with a pervasive developmental disorder». Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 42 (2): 271–8. doi:10.1017/s0021963001006680. PMID 11280423.
  124. ^ «APA PsycNet». psycnet.apa.org. Retrieved April 14, 2022.
  125. ^ Hamilton AF (August 2009). «Goals, intentions and mental states: challenges for theories of autism». Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, and Allied Disciplines. 50 (8): 881–92. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.621.6275. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2009.02098.x. PMID 19508497.
  126. ^ McDonald, Nicole M., and Daniel S. Messinger. «The development of empathy: How, when, and why.» Moral Behavior and Free Will: A Neurobiological and Philosophical Aprroach (2011): 341-368.
  127. ^ a b Baron-Cohen S (March 2009). «Autism: the empathizing-systemizing (E-S) theory». Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 1156 (The Year in Cognitive Neuroscience 2009): 68–80. Bibcode:2009NYASA1156…68B. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04467.x. PMID 19338503. S2CID 1440395.
  128. ^ Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Ashwin E, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G (February 2009). «Fetal testosterone and autistic traits» (PDF). British Journal of Psychology. 100 (Pt 1): 1–22. doi:10.1348/000712608X311731. hdl:20.500.11820/3012e64e-48e9-46fb-b47e-8a8a7853b4de. PMID 18547459. S2CID 6344484. Archived (PDF) from the original on November 22, 2018. Retrieved November 21, 2018. Pdf. Archived August 9, 2017, at the Wayback Machine
  129. ^ Milton, Damian E.M. (October 1, 2012). «On the ontological status of autism: the ‘double empathy problem’«. Disability & Society. 27 (6): 883–887. doi:10.1080/09687599.2012.710008. ISSN 0968-7599. S2CID 54047060.
  130. ^ Cleckly HC (1941). The Mask of Sanity: An Attempt to Reinterpret the So-Called Psychopathic Personality. St. Louis, MO: Mosby.
  131. ^ Hare RD (1991). The Hare Psychopathy Checklist-Revised. Toronto: Multi Health Systems.
  132. ^ Skeem JL, Polaschek DL, Patrick CJ, Lilienfeld SO (December 2011). «Psychopathic Personality: Bridging the Gap Between Scientific Evidence and Public Policy». Psychological Science in the Public Interest. 12 (3): 95–162. doi:10.1177/1529100611426706. PMID 26167886. S2CID 8521465. Archived from the original on February 22, 2016.
  133. ^ Patrick C (2005). Handbook of Psychopathy. Guilford Press. ISBN 978-1-60623-804-2.[page needed]
  134. ^ Andrade J (March 23, 2009). Handbook of Violence Risk Assessment and Treatment: New Approaches for Mental Health Professionals. New York, NY: Springer Publishing Company. ISBN 978-0-8261-9904-1. Retrieved January 5, 2014.
  135. ^
    WHO (2010) ICD-10: Clinical descriptions and diagnostic guidelines: Disorders of adult personality and behavior Archived March 23, 2014, at the Wayback Machine
  136. ^ Decety J, Skelly L (2013). «The neural underpinnings of the experience of empathy: Lessons for psychopathy.». In Ochsner KN, Kosslyn SM (eds.). The Oxford Handbook of Cognitive Neuroscience. Vol. 2. New York: Oxford University Press. pp. 228–243.
    • Kiehl KA (June 2006). «A cognitive neuroscience perspective on psychopathy: evidence for paralimbic system dysfunction». Psychiatry Research. 142 (2–3): 107–28. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2005.09.013. PMC 2765815. PMID 16712954.

  137. ^ Blair RJ (October 1995). «A cognitive developmental approach to mortality: investigating the psychopath» (PDF). Cognition. 57 (1): 1–29. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(95)00676-p. PMID 7587017. S2CID 16366546. Archived from the original (PDF) on July 21, 2013.
  138. ^ Blair RJ (January 2003). «Neurobiological basis of psychopathy». The British Journal of Psychiatry. 182: 5–7. doi:10.1192/bjp.182.1.5. PMID 12509310.
  139. ^ a b «Psychopathy» by Quinton 2006
  140. ^ a b Blair RJ, Colledge E, Mitchell DG (December 2001). «Somatic markers and response reversal: is there orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in boys with psychopathic tendencies?». Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology. 29 (6): 499–511. doi:10.1023/A:1012277125119. PMID 11761284. S2CID 1951812.
  141. ^ a b Blair RJ, Mitchell DG, Richell RA, Kelly S, Leonard A, Newman C, Scott SK (November 2002). «Turning a deaf ear to fear: impaired recognition of vocal affect in psychopathic individuals». Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 111 (4): 682–6. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.111.4.682. PMID 12428783.
  142. ^ a b Stevens D, Charman T, Blair RJ (June 2001). «Recognition of emotion in facial expressions and vocal tones in children with psychopathic tendencies». The Journal of Genetic Psychology. 162 (2): 201–11. doi:10.1080/00221320109597961. PMID 11432605. S2CID 42581610.
  143. ^ Decety J, Skelly L, Yoder KJ, Kiehl KA (February 2014). «Neural processing of dynamic emotional facial expressions in psychopaths». Social Neuroscience. 9 (1): 36–49. doi:10.1080/17470919.2013.866905. PMC 3970241. PMID 24359488.
    • Dawel A, O’Kearney R, McKone E, Palermo R (November 2012). «Not just fear and sadness: meta-analytic evidence of pervasive emotion recognition deficits for facial and vocal expressions in psychopathy». Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 36 (10): 2288–304. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.08.006. hdl:1885/19765. PMID 22944264. S2CID 2596760.

  144. ^ Hogenboom M (July 25, 2013). «Psychopathic criminals have empathy switch». BBC News. Archived from the original on July 27, 2013. Retrieved July 28, 2013.
  145. ^ Lewis T (July 24, 2013). «Cold-hearted Psychopaths Feel Empathy Too». Live Science.
  146. ^ Barrett LF (2017). How Emotions are Made: The Secret Life of the Brain.
    • Atkins D (2014). The Role of Culture in Empathy: The Consequences and Explanations of Cultural Differences in Empathy at the Affective and Cognitive Levels.

  147. ^ Decety J, Skelly LR, Kiehl KA (June 2013). «Brain response to empathy-eliciting scenarios involving pain in incarcerated individuals with psychopathy». JAMA Psychiatry. 70 (6): 638–45. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.27. PMC 3914759. PMID 23615636.
  148. ^ Decety J, Chen C, Harenski C, Kiehl KA (2013). «An fMRI study of affective perspective taking in individuals with psychopathy: imagining another in pain does not evoke empathy». Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 7: 489. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2013.00489. PMC 3782696. PMID 24093010.
  149. ^ Mullins-Nelson JL, Salekin RT, Anne-Marie RT, Leistico RL (2006). «Psychopathy, Empathy, and Perspective -Taking Ability in a Community Sample: Implications for the Successful Psychopathy Concept». International Journal of Forensic Mental Health. 5 (2): 133–149. doi:10.1080/14999013.2006.10471238. S2CID 143760402.
  150. ^ Vedantam, Shankar (May 2007). «If It Feels Good to Be Good, It Might Be Only Natural». The Washington Post. Retrieved April 23, 2010.
  151. ^ Winter K, Spengler S, Bermpohl F, Singer T, Kanske P (April 2017). «Social cognition in aggressive offenders: Impaired empathy, but intact theory of mind». Scientific Reports. 7 (1): 670. Bibcode:2017NatSR…7..670W. doi:10.1038/s41598-017-00745-0. PMC 5429629. PMID 28386118.
  152. ^ Minzenberg MJ, Fisher-Irving M, Poole JH, Vinogradov S (February 2006). «Reduced Self-Referential Source Memory Performance is Associated with Interpersonal Dysfunction in Borderline Personality Disorder» (PDF). Journal of Personality Disorders. 20 (1): 42–54. doi:10.1521/pedi.2006.20.1.42. PMID 16563078. Archived (PDF) from the original on May 16, 2013.
  153. ^ Harari H, Shamay-Tsoory SG, Ravid M, Levkovitz Y (February 2010). «Double dissociation between cognitive and affective empathy in borderline personality disorder». Psychiatry Research. 175 (3): 277–9. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2009.03.002. PMID 20045198. S2CID 27303466.
  154. ^
    Narcissistic personality disorder Archived January 18, 2013, at archive.today – Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders Fourth edition Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) American Psychiatric Association (2000)
  155. ^ «Schizoid personality disorder». Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Fourth (DSM-IV-TR) ed.). American Psychiatric Association. 2000. Archived from the original on January 18, 2013.
  156. ^ Guntrip H (1969). Schizoid Phenomena, Object-Relations, and The Self. New York: International Universities Press.
  157. ^ Ralph Klein- pp. 13–23 in Disorders of the Self: New Therapeutic Horizons, Brunner/Mazel (1995).
  158. ^ a b Shamay-Tsoory S, Harari H, Szepsenwol O, Levkovitz Y (2009). «Neuropsychological evidence of impaired cognitive empathy in euthymic bipolar disorder». The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 21 (1): 59–67. doi:10.1176/jnp.2009.21.1.59. PMID 19359453.
  159. ^ a b «Are You Suffering From Empathy Deficit Disorder?». Psychology Today. Retrieved March 18, 2022.
  160. ^ McAlinden M (2014). «Can teachers know learners’ minds? Teacher empathy and learner body language in English language teaching». In Dunworth K, Zhang G (eds.). Critical perspectives on language education: Australia and the Asia Pacific. Cham, Switzerland: Springer. pp. 71–100. ISBN 9783319061856.
  161. ^ a b Tettegah S, Anderson CJ (2007). «Pre-service teachers’ empathy and cognitions: Statistical analysis of text data by graphical models». Contemporary Educational Psychology. 32 (1): 48–82. doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2006.10.010.
  162. ^ Cornelius-White JH, Harbaugh AP (2010). Learner-Centered Instruction. Thousand Oaks, CA, London, New Delhi, Singapore: SAGE Publications.
    • Rogers CR, Lyon Jr HC, Tausch R (2013). On Becoming an Effective Teacher — Person-centered teaching, psychology, philosophy, anddialogues. London: Routledge. ISBN 978-0-415-81698-4.

  163. ^ William Weeks, Paul Pedersen, & Richard Brislin (1979). A Manual of Structured Experiences for Cultural Learning. La Grange Park, IL: Intercultural Network.
  164. ^ Divine World College (2016), Bachelor of Arts in Intercultural Studies program, Epworth, IA.
  165. ^ Sue Brown and Joyce Osland (2016), Developing Cultural Diversity Competency. University of Portland.
  166. ^ «Trevisani, D. 2005. Intercultural Negotiation. Milan, Franco Angeli»
  167. ^ Eisenberg N, Miller PA (January 1987). «The relation of empathy to prosocial and related behaviors». Psychological Bulletin. 101 (1): 91–119. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.101.1.91. PMID 3562705.
  168. ^ Bjorkqvist K, Osterman K, Kaukiainen A (2000). «Social intelligence — empathy = aggression?». Aggression and Violent Behavior. 5 (2): 191–200. doi:10.1016/s1359-1789(98)00029-9.
  169. ^ Geer JH, Estupinan LA, Manguno-Mire GM (2000). «Empathy, social skills, and other relevant cognitive processes in rapists and child molesters». Aggression and Violent Behavior. 5 (1): 99–126. doi:10.1016/s1359-1789(98)00011-1.
  170. ^ Segal SA, Gerdes KE, Lietz CA (2017). Assessing Empathy. Columbia University Press. pp. 79–81. ISBN 978-0-231-54388-0.
  171. ^ Goleman D (2005). Emotional intelligence (in Danish). New York: Bantam Books. ISBN 978-0-553-38371-3. OCLC 61770783.
  172. ^ Weiner IB, Craighead WE (2010). The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology. John Wiley & Sons. p. 810. ISBN 978-0-470-17026-7.
  173. ^ a b Gerace A, Day A, Casey S, Mohr P (2015). «Perspective taking and empathy: Does having similar past experience to another person make it easier to take their perspective?» (PDF). Journal of Relationships Research. 6: e10, 1–14. doi:10.1017/jrr.2015.6. hdl:2328/35813. S2CID 146270695.
  174. ^ a b Hodges SD, Kiel KJ, Kramer AD, Veach D, Villanueva BR (March 2010). «Giving birth to empathy: the effects of similar experience on empathic accuracy, empathic concern, and perceived empathy». Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin. 36 (3): 398–409. doi:10.1177/0146167209350326. PMID 19875825. S2CID 23104368.
  175. ^ Wyschogrod E (February 1981). «Empathy and sympathy as tactile encounter». The Journal of Medicine and Philosophy. 6 (1): 25–43. doi:10.1093/jmp/6.1.25. PMID 7229562.
  176. ^ Grühn, D. (2008). «Empathy across the adult lifespan: Longitudinal and experience-sampling findings». Emotion. 8 (6): 753–765. doi:10.1037/a0014123. PMC 2669929. PMID 19102586.
  177. ^ Bernard, B. (2004). Resiliency: What we have learned.
  178. ^ a b c d Breithaupt, Fritz (2019). The Dark Sides of Empathy.
  179. ^ Bloom, Paul (2016). Against Empathy: The Case for Rational Compassion. p. chapter 6.
  180. ^ Lau, B.K.Y.; Geipel, J.; Wu, Y.; Keysar, B. (2022). «The extreme illusion of understanding». Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 151 (11): 2957–2962. doi:10.1037/xge0001213. PMID 35377705. S2CID 247954809.
  181. ^ McLaren, Karla (2013). The Art of Empathy: A Complete Guide to Life’s Most Essential Skill. p. 32.
  182. ^ Young, S.G.; Hugenberg, K. (2010). «Mere socialization categorization modulates identification of facial expressions of emotion». Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 99 (6): 964–977. doi:10.1037/a0020400. PMID 20919774.
  183. ^ Levenson RW, Ruef AM (1997). «Physiological aspects of emotional knowledge and rapport.». In Ickes WJ (ed.). Empathic Accuracy. New York, NY: The Guilford Press. pp. 44–72. ISBN 978-1-57230-161-0.
  184. ^ Hoffman (2000), p. 62
  185. ^ Hein G, Silani G, Preuschoff K, Batson CD, Singer T (October 2010). «Neural responses to ingroup and outgroup members’ suffering predict individual differences in costly helping». Neuron. 68 (1): 149–60. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2010.09.003. PMID 20920798.
  186. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k «The Case Against Empathy». Think! (Podcast). KERA. January 5, 2017.
  187. ^ Kraus, M.W.; Côté, S.; Keltner, D. (2010). «Social class, contextualism, and empathic accuracy». Psychological Science. 2 (11): 1716–1723. doi:10.1177/0956797610387613. PMID 20974714. S2CID 7306762.
  188. ^ Hogeveen, J.; Inzlicht, M.; Obhi, S.S. (2014). «Power changes how the brain responds to others». Journal of Experimental Psychology. 143 (2): 755–762. doi:10.1037/a0033477. PMID 23815455.
    • Galinksy, A.D. (2006). «Power and perspectives not taken». Psychological Science. 17 (12): 1068–1074. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01824.x. PMID 17201789. S2CID 3524097.
    • Van Kleef, G.A. (2015). «Power gets you high: The powerful are more inspired by themselves than by others» (PDF). Social Psychology and Personality Science. 6: 472–480. doi:10.1177/1948550614566857. S2CID 8686513.

  189. ^ Klimecki O, Singer T (2012). «Empathic distress fatigue rather than compassion fatigue? Integrating findings from empathy research in psychology and social neuroscience» (PDF). In Oakley B, Knafo A, Madhavan G, Wilson DS (eds.). Pathological Altruism. USA: Oxford University Press. pp. 368–383. ISBN 978-0-19-973857-1.
  190. ^ Tone EB, Tully EC (November 2014). «Empathy as a ‘risky strength’: a multilevel examination of empathy and risk for internalizing disorders». Development and Psychopathology. 26 (4 Pt 2): 1547–65. doi:10.1017/S0954579414001199. PMC 4340688. PMID 25422978.
  191. ^ Solon O (July 12, 2012). «Compassion over empathy could help prevent emotional burnout». Wired UK. Archived from the original on May 15, 2016.
  192. ^ «When Empathy Backfires | Psychology Today».
  193. ^ The Ethics of Care and Empathy, Michael Slote, Oxford University Press, 2007
  194. ^ Empathy in the Context of Philosophy, Lou Agosta, Palgrave/Macmillan, 2010
  195. ^ Jenkins, K. (1991) Re-thinking History London: Routledge
  196. ^ «Wired To Care». wiredtocare.com. Archived from the original on December 10, 2008.
  197. ^ Miyashiro MR (2011). The Empathy Factor: Your Competitive Advantage for Personal, Team, and Business Success. Puddledancer Press. p. 256. ISBN 978-1-892005-25-0.
  198. ^ Dowden C (June 21, 2013). «Forget ethics training: Focus on empathy». The National Post. Archived from the original on July 27, 2013.
  199. ^ «The Importance of Empathy in the Workplace». Center for Creative Leadership. Retrieved November 10, 2020.
  200. ^ Phillips, Kaitlin Ugolik (2020). The Future of Feeling: Building Empathy in a Tech-Obsessed World. pp. 101–102.
  201. ^ Krznaric, Roman (2014). Empathy: Why It Matters, and How to Get It. pp. ⅺ–ⅻ.
  202. ^ Adam Waytz “The Limits of Empathy” in Empathy Harvard Business Review Press (2007)
  203. ^ «Fidelity exploits virtual reality for «empathy training»«. Finextra. October 16, 2017.
  204. ^ Radzvilavicius, Arunas L; Stewart, Alexander J; Plotkin, Joshua B (April 9, 2019). Doebeli, Michael; Tautz, Diethard; Masuda, Naoki; Nowak, Martin A (eds.). «Evolution of empathetic moral evaluation». eLife. 8: e44269. doi:10.7554/eLife.44269. ISSN 2050-084X. PMC 6488294. PMID 30964002.
  205. ^ Chlopan, Bruce E.; McCain, Marianne L.; Carbonell, Joyce L.; Hagen, Richard L. (1985). «Empathy: Review of available measures». Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 48 (3): 635–653. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.48.3.635.
  206. ^ Truax, C. B. (1967). «Rating of Accurate Empathy.» The Therapeutic Relationship and its Impact. A Study of Psychotherapy with Schizophrenics. Eds. C. R. Rogers, E. T. Gendlin, D. J. Kiesler and C. B. Truax. Madison, Wisconsin, The University of Wisconsin Press pp. 555–568.
  207. ^ Mehrabian A, Epstein N (December 1972). «A measure of emotional empathy». Journal of Personality. 40 (4): 525–43. doi:10.1111/j.1467-6494.1972.tb00078.x. PMID 4642390.
  208. ^ a b e.g. Levenson RW, Ruef AM (August 1992). «Empathy: a physiological substrate» (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 63 (2): 234–46. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.63.2.234. PMID 1403614. S2CID 12650202. Archived from the original (PDF) on July 30, 2020.
    • Leslie KR, Johnson-Frey SH, Grafton ST (February 2004). «Functional imaging of face and hand imitation: towards a motor theory of empathy». NeuroImage. 21 (2): 601–7. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2003.09.038. PMID 14980562. S2CID 1723495.

  209. ^ Denham SA, McKinley M, Couchoud EA, Holt R (August 1990). «Emotional and behavioral predictors of preschool peer ratings». Child Development. JSTOR. 61 (4): 1145–52. doi:10.2307/1130882. JSTOR 1130882. PMID 2209184.
  210. ^ Barnett MA (1984). «Similarity of experience and empathy in preschoolers». Journal of Genetic Psychology. 145 (2): 241–250. doi:10.1080/00221325.1984.10532271.
  211. ^ e.g. Geher G, Warner RM, Brown AS (2001). «Predictive validity of the emotional accuracy research scale». Intelligence. Elsevier BV. 29 (5): 373–388. doi:10.1016/s0160-2896(00)00045-3. ISSN 0160-2896.
  212. ^ e.g. Mehrabian & Epstein (1972)
  213. ^ e.g. Davis MH (1980). «A multidimensional approach to individual differences in empathy». JSAS Catalogue of Selected Documents in Psychology. 10 (4): 1–17.
  214. ^ Segal, Elizabeth A. (2017). Assessing Empathy.
  215. ^ Chen D, Lew R, Hershman W, Orlander J (October 2007). «A cross-sectional measurement of medical student empathy». Journal of General Internal Medicine. 22 (10): 1434–8. doi:10.1007/s11606-007-0298-x. PMC 2305857. PMID 17653807.
  216. ^ Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S (April 2004). «The empathy quotient: an investigation of adults with Asperger syndrome or high functioning autism, and normal sex differences» (PDF). Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. 34 (2): 163–75. doi:10.1023/B:JADD.0000022607.19833.00. PMID 15162935. S2CID 2663853. Archived from the original (PDF) on March 4, 2015.
  217. ^ Reniers RL, Corcoran R, Drake R, Shryane NM, Völlm BA (January 2011). «The QCAE: a Questionnaire of Cognitive and Affective Empathy». Journal of Personality Assessment. 93 (1): 84–95. doi:10.1080/00223891.2010.528484. PMID 21184334. S2CID 3035172.
  218. ^ Innamorati M, Ebisch SJ, Gallese V, Saggino A (April 29, 2019). «A bidimensional measure of empathy: Empathic Experience Scale». PLOS ONE. 14 (4): e0216164. Bibcode:2019PLoSO..1416164I. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0216164. PMC 6488069. PMID 31034510.
  219. ^ Konrath, Sara H.; O’Brien, Edward H.; Hsing, Courtney (2011). «Changes in Dispositional Empathy in American College Students Over Time: A Meta-Analysis». Personality and Social Psychology Review. 15 (2): 180–198. doi:10.1177/1088868310377395. PMID 20688954. S2CID 7645344.
  220. ^ Breithaupt, Fritz (2019). The Dark Sides of Empathy.
  221. ^ Chopik WJ, O’Brien E, Konrath SH (2017). «Differences in Empathic Concern and Perspective Taking Across 63 Countries». Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 48 (1). Supplementary Table 1. doi:10.1177/0022022116673910. hdl:1805/14139. ISSN 0022-0221. S2CID 149314942.
  222. ^ Clay Z, de Waal FB (November 2013). «Development of socio-emotional competence in bonobos». Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 110 (45): 18121–6. Bibcode:2013PNAS..11018121C. doi:10.1073/pnas.1316449110. PMC 3831480. PMID 24127600.
  223. ^ Romero T, Castellanos MA, de Waal FB (July 2010). «Consolation as possible expression of sympathetic concern among chimpanzees». Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 107 (27): 12110–5. Bibcode:2010PNAS..10712110R. doi:10.1073/pnas.1006991107. PMC 2901437. PMID 20547864.
  224. ^ Custance D, Mayer J (September 2012). «Empathic-like responding by domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) to distress in humans: an exploratory study» (PDF). Animal Cognition. 15 (5): 851–9. doi:10.1007/s10071-012-0510-1. PMID 22644113. S2CID 15153091.
  225. ^ Edgar JL, Paul ES, Nicol CJ (August 2013). «Protective Mother Hens: Cognitive influences on the avian maternal response». British Journal of Animal Behaviour. 86 (2): 223–229. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.05.004. S2CID 53179718.
  226. ^ Miralles A, Raymond M, Lecointre G (December 2019). «Empathy and compassion toward other species decrease with evolutionary divergence time». Scientific Reports. 9 (1): 19555. Bibcode:2019NatSR…919555M. doi:10.1038/s41598-019-56006-9. PMC 6925286. PMID 31862944.
  227. ^ Hunt, Lynn (2007). Inventing Human Rights: A History.
    • See also: Lynn Hunt «Inventing Human Rights» (lecture, March 2008)

Further reading[edit]

  • Buber, Martin (2020). I and thou. New York. ISBN 978-1-945186-88-2. OCLC 1129703024.

External links[edit]

1

: the action of understanding, being aware of, being sensitive to, and vicariously experiencing the feelings, thoughts, and experience of another of either the past or present without having the feelings, thoughts, and experience fully communicated in an objectively explicit manner

also

: the capacity for this

2

: the imaginative projection of a subjective state into an object so that the object appears to be infused with it

Did you know?

Sympathy and empathy both refer to a caring response to the emotional state of another person, but a distinction between them is typically made: while sympathy is a feeling of sincere concern for someone who is experiencing something difficult or painful, empathy involves actively sharing in the emotional experience of the other person.

Sympathy has been in use since the 16th century, and its greater age is reflected in its wider breadth of meanings, including “a feeling of loyalty” and “unity or harmony in action or effect.” It comes ultimately from the Greek sympathēs, meaning “having common feelings, sympathetic,” which was formed from syn- (“with, together with”) and páthos, “experience, misfortune, emotion, condition.” Empathy was modeled on sympathy; it was coined in the early 20th century as a translation of the German Einfühlung (“feeling-in” or “feeling into”). First applied in contexts of philosophy, aesthetics, and psychology, empathy continues to have technical use in those fields that sympathy does not.

Did you know?

Compassion and empathy both refer to a caring response to someone else’s distress. While empathy refers to an active sharing in the emotional experience of the other person, compassion adds to that emotional experience a desire to alleviate the person’s distress.

… the story of Nellie Bly, the first female investigative reporter, who not only demanded justice from powerful institutions, but also insisted on dignity and compassion for the most vulnerable citizens. — The Christian Science Monitor, 17 Aug. 2022

Blonde clearly wants us to feel for Norma Jeane, but it dwells on her pain so obsessively … that the movie’s empathy feels like another form of exploitation. — Justin Chang, NPR, 23 Sept. 2022

The distinction between compassion and empathy is frequently a topic of exploration.

By empathy I mean feeling the feelings of other people. So if you’re in pain and I feel your pain—I am feeling empathy toward you. If you’re being anxious, I pick up your anxiety. If you’re sad and I pick up your sadness, I’m being empathetic. And that’s different from compassion. Compassion means I give your concern weight, I value it. I care about you, but I don’t necessarily pick up your feelings. … [I]f I feel compassion for you, I’ll be invigorated. I’ll be happy and I’ll try to make your life better. — Paul Bloom, quoted in Vox, 16 Jan. 2019

Compassion is a much older word; it’s been part of the language since the 14th century, and comes ultimately from Latin com- and pati, meaning “to bear, suffer.” Empathy is a 20th century coinage modeled on sympathy as a translation of the German Einfühlung (“feeling-in” or “feeling into”). It was first applied in contexts of philosophy, aesthetics, and psychology and continues to have technical use in those fields.

Example Sentences

Poetic empathy understandably seeks a strategy of identification with victims …


Helen Vendler, New Republic, 5 May 2003


This is tough love with a vengeance, but what a gruesome view of God’s saints bereft of all empathy.


Sidney Callahan, Commonweal, 19 Apr. 2002


Enter a new inmate … a giant black man with a gift of preternatural empathy; he can literally suck the pain out of people.


Richard Corliss, Time, 13 Dec. 1999


But in all those years of young womanhood, my Do-Unto-Others empathy never extended beyond sharing a trolley seat.


Lois Mark Stalvey, The Education of a WASP, 1989



He felt great empathy with the poor.



His months spent researching prison life gave him greater empathy towards convicts.

See More

Recent Examples on the Web

Another misconception is that autistic people don’t have empathy.


Clare Mulroy, USA TODAY, 4 Apr. 2023





Scorpio, a fixed sign, is known for its passion, loyalty, and determination, while Pisces, a mutable sign, is known for its empathy, intuition, and creativity.


Naydeline Mejia, Women’s Health, 31 Mar. 2023





Now, in a study published last week in the journal Science, researchers say the same mechanism responsible for human empathy might also be at play among fish.


Sarah Kuta, Smithsonian Magazine, 28 Mar. 2023





The new study shows that fish can detect fear in other fish, and then become afraid too – and that this ability is regulated by oxytocin, the same brain chemical that underlies the capacity for empathy in humans.


Christina Larson, Fortune, 23 Mar. 2023





So, that is, a) empathy, and b) keeping an open mind to what people are saying.


Marlow Stern, Rolling Stone, 20 Mar. 2023





Kamali said Mandana represents the strength and equality women strive for today, while Cyrus represents the global need for empathy and human rights.


Sydney Carruth, The Arizona Republic, 19 Mar. 2023





The activity is part of a larger effort to build empathy, inclusion and support for young Muslim students who are fasting during the school day.


oregonlive, 19 Mar. 2023





This failure of human imagination, or empathy, mars some classic experiments, Root-Gutteridge and coauthors noted in a 2022 paper focused on animal welfare issues.


Elizabeth Preston, Discover Magazine, 18 Mar. 2023



See More

These examples are programmatically compiled from various online sources to illustrate current usage of the word ’empathy.’ Any opinions expressed in the examples do not represent those of Merriam-Webster or its editors. Send us feedback about these examples.

Word History

Etymology

Greek empatheia, literally, passion, from empathēs emotional, from em- + pathos feelings, emotion — more at pathos

First Known Use

1909, in the meaning defined at sense 2

Time Traveler

The first known use of empathy was
in 1909

Dictionary Entries Near empathy

Cite this Entry

“Empathy.” Merriam-Webster.com Dictionary, Merriam-Webster, https://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/empathy. Accessed 14 Apr. 2023.

Share

More from Merriam-Webster on empathy

Last Updated:
12 Apr 2023
— Updated example sentences

Subscribe to America’s largest dictionary and get thousands more definitions and advanced search—ad free!

Merriam-Webster unabridged

Empathy is the ability to emotionally understand what other people feel, see things from their point of view, and imagine yourself in their place. Essentially, it is putting yourself in someone else’s position and feeling what they are feeling.

Empathy means that when you see another person suffering, such as after they’ve lost a loved one, you are able to instantly envision yourself going through that same experience and feel what they are going through.

While people can be well-attuned to their own feelings and emotions, getting into someone else’s head can be a bit more difficult. The ability to feel empathy allows people to «walk a mile in another’s shoes,» so to speak. It permits people to understand the emotions that others are feeling.

Press Play for Advice on Empathy

Hosted by Editor-in-Chief and therapist Amy Morin, LCSW, this episode of The Verywell Mind Podcast, featuring empathy expert Dr. Kelsey Crowe, shares how you can show empathy to someone who is going through a hard time. Click below to listen now.

Follow Now : Apple Podcasts / Spotify / Google Podcasts

Signs of Empathy

For many, seeing another person in pain and responding with indifference or even outright hostility seems utterly incomprehensible. But the fact that some people do respond in such a way clearly demonstrates that empathy is not necessarily a universal response to the suffering of others.

If you are wondering whether you are an empathetic person, here are some signs that show that you have this tendency:

  • You are good at really listening to what others have to say.
  • People often tell you about their problems.
  • You are good at picking up on how other people are feeling.
  • You often think about how other people feel.
  • Other people come to you for advice.
  • You often feel overwhelmed by tragic events.
  • You try to help others who are suffering.
  • You are good at telling when people aren’t being honest.
  • You sometimes feel drained or overwhelmed in social situations.
  • You care deeply about other people.
  • You find it difficult to set boundaries in your relationships.

Types of Empathy

There are several types of empathy that a person may experience. The three types of empathy are:

  • Affective empathy involves the ability to understand another person’s emotions and respond appropriately. Such emotional understanding may lead to someone feeling concerned for another person’s well-being, or it may lead to feelings of personal distress.
  • Somatic empathy involves having a physical reaction in response to what someone else is experiencing. People sometimes physically experience what another person is feeling. When you see someone else feeling embarrassed, for example, you might start to blush or have an upset stomach.
  • Cognitive empathy involves being able to understand another person’s mental state and what they might be thinking in response to the situation. This is related to what psychologists refer to as the theory of mind or thinking about what other people are thinking.

Empathy vs. Sympathy vs. Compassion

While sympathy and compassion are related to empathy, there are important differences. Compassion and sympathy are often thought to be more of a passive connection, while empathy generally involves a much more active attempt to understand another person.

Uses for Empathy

Being able to experience empathy has many beneficial uses.

  • Empathy allows you to build social connections with others. By understanding what people are thinking and feeling, you are able to respond appropriately in social situations. Research has shown that having social connections is important for both physical and psychological well-being.
  • Empathizing with others helps you learn to regulate your own emotions. Emotional regulation is important in that it allows you to manage what you are feeling, even in times of great stress, without becoming overwhelmed.
  • Empathy promotes helping behaviors. Not only are you more likely to engage in helpful behaviors when you feel empathy for other people, but other people are also more likely to help you when they experience empathy.

Potential Pitfalls of Empathy

Having a great deal of empathy makes you concerned for the well-being and happiness of others. It also means, however, that you can sometimes get overwhelmed, burned out, or even overstimulated from always thinking about other people’s emotions. This can lead to empathy fatigue.

Empathy fatigue refers to the exhaustion you might feel both emotionally and physically after repeatedly being exposed to stressful or traumatic events. You might also feel numb or powerless, isolate yourself, and have a lack of energy.

Empathy fatigue is a concern in certain situations, such as when acting as a caregiver. Studies also show that if healthcare workers can’t balance their feelings of empathy (affective empathy, in particular), it can result in compassion fatigue as well.

Other research has linked higher levels of empathy with a tendency toward emotional negativity, potentially increasing your risk of empathic distress. It can even affect your judgment, causing you to go against your morals based on the empathy you feel for someone else.

Impact of Empathy

Your ability to experience empathy can impact your relationships. Studies involving siblings have found that when empathy is high, siblings have less conflict and more warmth toward each other. In romantic relationships, having empathy increases your ability to extend forgiveness.

Not everyone experiences empathy in every situation. Some people may be more naturally empathetic in general, but people also tend to feel more empathetic toward some people and less so toward others. Some of the factors that play a role in this tendency include:

  • How you perceive the other person
  • How you attribute the other individual’s behaviors
  • What you blame for the other person’s predicament
  • Your past experiences and expectations

Research has found that there are gender differences in the experience and expression of empathy, although these findings are somewhat mixed. Women score higher on empathy tests, and studies suggest that women tend to feel more cognitive empathy than men.

At the most basic level, there appear to be two main factors that contribute to the ability to experience empathy: genetics and socialization. Essentially, it boils down to the age-old relative contributions of nature and nurture.

Parents pass down genes that contribute to overall personality, including the propensity toward sympathy, empathy, and compassion. On the other hand, people are also socialized by their parents, peers, communities, and society. How people treat others, as well as how they feel about others, is often a reflection of the beliefs and values that were instilled at a very young age. 

Barriers to Empathy

Some people lack empathy and, therefore, aren’t able to understand what another person may be experiencing or feeling. This can result in behaviors that seem uncaring or sometimes even hurtful. For instance, people with low affective empathy have higher rates of cyberbullying.

A lack of empathy is also one of the defining characteristics of narcissistic personality disorder. Though, it is unclear whether this is due to a person with this disorder having no empathy at all or having more of a dysfunctional response to others.

A few reasons why people sometimes lack empathy include cognitive biases, dehumanization, and victim-blaming.

Cognitive Biases

Sometimes the way people perceive the world around them is influenced by cognitive biases. For example, people often attribute other people’s failures to internal characteristics, while blaming their own shortcomings on external factors.

These biases can make it difficult to see all the factors that contribute to a situation. They also make it less likely that people will be able to see a situation from the perspective of another.

Dehumanization

Many also fall victim to the trap of thinking that people who are different from them don’t feel and behave the same as they do. This is particularly common in cases when other people are physically distant.

For example, when they watch reports of a disaster or conflict in a foreign land, people might be less likely to feel empathy if they think that those who are suffering are fundamentally different from themselves.

Victim Blaming

Sometimes, when another person has suffered a terrible experience, people make the mistake of blaming the victim for their circumstances. This is the reason that victims of crimes are often asked what they might have done differently to prevent the crime.

This tendency stems from the need to believe that the world is a fair and just place. It is the desire to believe that people get what they deserve and deserve what they get—and it can fool you into thinking that such terrible things could never happen to you.

Causes of Empathy

Human beings are certainly capable of selfish, even cruel, behavior. A quick scan of the news quickly reveals numerous unkind, selfish, and heinous actions. The question, then, is why don’t we all engage in such self-serving behavior all the time? What is it that causes us to feel another’s pain and respond with kindness?

The term empathy was first introduced in 1909 by psychologist Edward B. Titchener as a translation of the German term einfühlung (meaning «feeling into»). Several different theories have been proposed to explain empathy.

Neuroscientific Explanations

Studies have shown that specific areas of the brain play a role in how empathy is experienced. More recent approaches focus on the cognitive and neurological processes that lie behind empathy. Researchers have found that different regions of the brain play an important role in empathy, including the anterior cingulate cortex and the anterior insula.

Research suggests that there are important neurobiological components to the experience of empathy. The activation of mirror neurons in the brain plays a part in the ability to mirror and mimic the emotional responses that people would feel if they were in similar situations.

Functional MRI research also indicates that an area of the brain known as the inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) plays a critical role in the experience of empathy. Studies have found that people who have damage to this area of the brain often have difficulty recognizing emotions conveyed through facial expressions. 

Emotional Explanations

Some of the earliest explorations into the topic of empathy centered on how feeling what others feel allows people to have a variety of emotional experiences. The philosopher Adam Smith suggested that it allows us to experience things that we might never otherwise be able to fully feel.

This can involve feeling empathy for both real people and imaginary characters. Experiencing empathy for fictional characters, for example, allows people to have a range of emotional experiences that might otherwise be impossible.

Prosocial Explanations

Sociologist Herbert Spencer proposed that empathy served an adaptive function and aided in the survival of the species. Empathy leads to helping behavior, which benefits social relationships. Humans are naturally social creatures. Things that aid in our relationships with other people benefit us as well.

When people experience empathy, they are more likely to engage in prosocial behaviors that benefit other people. Things such as altruism and heroism are also connected to feeling empathy for others.

Tips for Practicing Empathy

Fortunately, empathy is a skill that you can learn and strengthen. If you would like to build your empathy skills, there are a few things that you can do:

  • Work on listening to people without interrupting
  • Pay attention to body language and other types of nonverbal communication
  • Try to understand people, even when you don’t agree with them
  • Ask people questions to learn more about them and their lives
  • Imagine yourself in another person’s shoes
  • Strengthen your connection with others to learn more about how they feel
  • Seek to identify biases you may have and how they affect your empathy for others
  • Look for ways in which you are similar to others versus focusing on differences
  • Be willing to be vulnerable, opening up about how you feel
  • Engage in new experiences, giving you better insight into how others in that situation may feel
  • Get involved in organizations that push for social change

A Word From Verywell

While empathy might be lacking in some, most people are able to empathize with others in a variety of situations. This ability to see things from another person’s perspective and empathize with another’s emotions plays an important role in our social lives. Empathy allows us to understand others and, quite often, compels us to take action to relieve another person’s suffering.

  • Top Definitions
  • Quiz
  • Related Content
  • More About Empathy
  • Examples
  • British
  • Cultural

This shows grade level based on the word’s complexity.

[ em-puh-thee ]

/ ˈɛm pə θi /

This shows grade level based on the word’s complexity.


noun

the psychological identification with or vicarious experiencing of the emotions, thoughts, or attitudes of another: She put an arm around her friend’s shoulders and stood by her in silent empathy.

the imaginative ascribing to an object, as a natural object or work of art, feelings or attitudes present in oneself: By means of empathy, a great painting becomes a mirror of the self.

VIDEO FOR EMPATHY

What Is The Real Difference Between «Empathy» And «Sympathy»?

Empathy and sympathy both describe feelings, especially toward another person. But what is the real difference between them?

MORE VIDEOS FROM DICTIONARY.COM

QUIZ

CAN YOU ANSWER THESE COMMON GRAMMAR DEBATES?

There are grammar debates that never die; and the ones highlighted in the questions in this quiz are sure to rile everyone up once again. Do you know how to answer the questions that cause some of the greatest grammar debates?

Which sentence is correct?

Origin of empathy

First recorded in 1900–05; from Greek empátheia “affection,” equivalent to em- “in, within” (see em-2) + path- (base of páschein “to suffer”) + -eia noun suffix (see -ia); its present meaning translates German Einfühlung

synonym study for empathy

WORDS THAT MAY BE CONFUSED WITH empathy

empathy , sympathy (see synonym study at sympathy)

Words nearby empathy

empath, empathetic, empathetically, empathic, empathize, empathy, Empedocles, empennage, emperor, emperor butterfly, emperor moth

Dictionary.com Unabridged
Based on the Random House Unabridged Dictionary, © Random House, Inc. 2023

MORE ABOUT EMPATHY

What does empathy mean?

Empathy is the ability or practice of imagining or trying to deeply understand what someone else is feeling or what it’s like to be in their situation.

Empathy is often described as the ability to feel what others are feeling as if you are feeling it yourself. To feel empathy for someone is to empathize. People who do this are described as empathetic.

Some people use the word empathy interchangeably or in overlapping ways with the word sympathy, which generally means the sharing of emotions with someone else, especially sadness. However, others distinguish the two terms by emphasizing the importance of having empathy for others (feeling their pain) as opposed to having sympathy for them (feeling sorry for them).

Example: Having faced many of the same challenges, Nyala has empathy for immigrants and what it feels like to go through those challenges.

Where does empathy come from?

The first records of the word empathy come from the late 1800s from the context of psychology. The word comes from a translation of the German term Einfühlung, which literally means “a feeling in.” It ultimately derives from the Greek empátheia, meaning “affection” or “passion,” from em-, meaning “in,” and path-, the base of a verb meaning “to suffer.” In contrast, the sym- in sympathy means “with” or “together.”

While having sympathy for someone often means pitying them or feeling bad for them, having empathy is feeling or attempting to feel and understand exactly how a person feels and what it’s like to be them. When you have empathy for someone, you identify with them—as if you were them. In other words, empathy is the ability to feel and understand what it’s like to be “in someone else’s shoes.” Empathy usually involves showing kindness and having compassion—the desire to do something to help a person and reduce their pain. People described as empathetic or empathic due to being very sensitive to the emotions of others are sometimes called empathists or empaths.

A less common and more specific sense of empathy refers to the process of projecting one’s feelings onto an object. This is especially used in the context of art to refer to artists embedding their emotions in their work.

Did you know … ?

How is empathy used in real life?

Try using empathy!

Which of the following actions is an example of having empathy?

A. Feeling sorry for someone
B. Ignoring someone
C. Imagining how someone feels
D. Complimenting someone

Words related to empathy

affinity, appreciation, compassion, insight, pity, rapport, sympathy, warmth, communion, comprehension, concord, recognition, responsiveness, soul, picking up on

How to use empathy in a sentence

  • It’s understandable that people would lack the empathy or the foresight to realize parents have a particular set of challenges.

  • I hope this drive towards human empathy continues well beyond this moment in time.

  • Instead, she proposed approaching anti-maskers with empathy.

  • The post-pandemic focus on employee safety wasn’t just because of a wave of CEO empathy.

  • That has a lot to do with the company’s strong sense of empathy.

  • You write a lot about celebrities and with a lot of empathy.

  • Men’s Rights Activist «I have a lot of empathy for men, and the pressures that they go through.»

  • The book thus has an attractive double “empathy,” a word that appears in all four parts.

  • Scenes elicited intimate comments from the cast and crew about whose perspective solicited more empathy or felt more realistic.

  • But studies show white people simply have less empathy for black people.

  • So-called ‘born’ mechanics, maybe, whose understanding of machinery is a form of empathy we’ve never suspected.

  • Beyond those simple things lay telepathy, telekinesis, empathy….

  • But I won the Twenties too, remember, also without knowing a thing about empathy at the time.

  • Some of the settlers had empathy with the dolphins to a high degree, but Ross’s own powers of contact were relatively feeble.

  • He thought of Geria, of what that dream empathy had suggested.

British Dictionary definitions for empathy


noun

the power of understanding and imaginatively entering into another person’s feelingsSee also identification (def. 3b)

the attribution to an object, such as a work of art, of one’s own emotional or intellectual feelings about it

Derived forms of empathy

empathist, noun

Word Origin for empathy

C20: from Greek empatheia affection, passion, intended as a rendering of German Einfühlung, literally: a feeling in; see en- ², -pathy

Collins English Dictionary — Complete & Unabridged 2012 Digital Edition
© William Collins Sons & Co. Ltd. 1979, 1986 © HarperCollins
Publishers 1998, 2000, 2003, 2005, 2006, 2007, 2009, 2012

Cultural definitions for empathy


Identifying oneself completely with an object or person, sometimes even to the point of responding physically, as when, watching a baseball player swing at a pitch, one feels one’s own muscles flex.

The New Dictionary of Cultural Literacy, Third Edition
Copyright © 2005 by Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company. Published by Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company. All rights reserved.

We know empathy as our ability to feel the emotions of another person. But it is also an ability to imagine what the other person might be thinking or feeling. And it is also an attitude. David Brooks calls it a “social emotion.”

Why is empathy important, as Stephen Hawking told us before passing away?

Here’s the simplest way to explain empathy:

To have empathy is “to sense the hurt or the pleasure of another as he senses it.” Empathy is our ability to recognize and feel another’s emotions and understand their thoughts and views on a situation. It also enables us to use that insight to respond helpfully and support them through challenging times.

Empathy | Definition

Empathy is the ability to imagine and understand what someone else might be thinking or feeling. It is also an ability to experience another person’s emotion (painful or pleasant) as they feel it. According to Carl Rogers, to have empathy is “to sense the hurt or the pleasure of another as he senses it.”

  • Bruce D. Perry: “The essence of empathy is the ability to stand in another’s shoes, to feel what it’s like there. Your primary feelings are more related to the other person’s situation than your own.”
  • Carl Rogers: “Empathy is the listener’s effort to hear the other person deeply, accurately, and non-judgmentally.”
  • Maya Angelou: “I’ve learned that people will forget what you said, people will forget what you did, but people will never forget how you made them feel.”

Empathy | What is the origin story of empathy?

1. Lexical Origin

(OK, excuse the “lexical”—we only wanted to rhyme it with biological! It means relating to vocabulary).

The idea of empathy has existed for centuries, but its entry as a word in English is relatively recent. As a term, empathy has its earliest origins in the Greek word “empatheia,” composed of “en” (in) and “pathos” (feeling), meaning passion.

The modern concept of empathy was first introduced by the mid-19th century estheticians (those philosophers who deal with the nature of beauty, in things like art). To explain the emotional “feeling in” with a work of art, they brought in the German word Einfühlung”.

Robert Vischer, a German philosopher, was the first to use the word “Einfühlung” in 1873. Later, another German philosopher Theodor Lipps, in his Aesthetik, expanded it to mean “feeling one’s way into the experience of another.” The English psychologist Edward Titchener coined the word “empathy” as we know it today, in 1909, as a translated version of the German word einfühlung.

So you see, as a word, empathy has a history of merely 140-150 years. In comparison, the word sympathy is an older word—it has existed for almost 300 years before the first written record of the word empathy.

2. Biological Origin

Scientists tell us there is evidence of genetic roots of empathy, meaning we inherit some of our empathy from our parents.

Empathy has roots in our evolutionary past. The survival of our species depends on helping each other through troubled times. Mutual aid reduces our sufferings. Without the vital ability to empathize, humans would have acted solely to dominate others and not have responded to their calls of distress.

According to the Greater Good Science Center (GGSC), a research institute that studies the psychology, sociology, and neuroscience of well-being:

Empathy probably evolved in the context of the parental care that characterizes all mammals. Signaling their state through smiling and crying, human infants urge their caregiver to take action… females who responded to their offspring’s needs out-reproduced those who were cold and distant,. This may explain gender differences in human empathy.

– Frans De Waal, GGSC

Empathy seems to occur in animals too. We find it in many of our primate relatives, and even in rats.

Empathy | What Does Empathy Mean?

Scientifically, much of our modern understanding of empathy is based on the works of Carl Rogers, the American psychologist. In the past, empathy was considered an inborn trait that could not be taught. It was Rogers who told us we can learn to be empathic. That was revolutionary!

Empathy builds an emotional bridge between people and encourages prosocial behaviors, like sharing of experiences, needs, and desires. Imagine empathy as a bridge between minds where people walk up and share their opinions, beliefs, fears, and anxieties.

When we can sense the suffering of others, it allows us to understand their pain and resonate with their distress. And the distress we experience on seeing them in pain then motivates us to respond with kindness and compassion.

Now, the crucial part: Empathy is imagining and experiencing another person’s feelings and thoughts from their position. You stand in their shoes and feel where the shoe pinches their feet, and understand how it torments them. It’s not how you would have felt if wearing the same shoes, but how they are feeling in those shoes.

Another vital fact: Empathizing is understanding their pain exactly as they feel. A feeling of empathy may not always prod us into going out to help every person who seeks our aid. You can feel empathy for all in pain, but you simply can’t possibly help all who want your help. So, you can be empathetic without wanting to, or being able to, help them.

(Strange fact: Some people have this strange behavior called pathological altruism. It is when a person goes out of their way to seemingly improve another person’s condition, even at a risk or cost to themselves. We see it commonly as codependency, in which a person makes every effort to help their partner’s addiction or any other destructive habit, without being able to leave the relationship.)

Some animals show emotional contagion or social mimicry to another animal in pain. But it is not true empathy, because empathy involves self-awareness, while mimicry does not. Human babies can also show this social mimicry; they can smile at you without understanding why you’re smiling!

So, what does self-awareness mean? Being self-aware means the empathetic person can always distinguish the self from the other. In empathy, one feels they are with someone, but they do not confuse themselves with the other person. That is, one always knows the emotions they are resonating with are the emotions of another person.

In 1975, Carl Rogers wrote Empathic – An Unappreciated Way of Being. In that, he proposed human empathy is a process and not a state. This meant empathy does not come in a fixed amount in each of us, and that all of us can change it for the better or worse. With training, the healthcare workers tend to feel less empathy, especially of the compassionate type. But those caregivers who feel hyperempathy can burn themselves out easily.

Finally, having empathy does not mean you agree with the person. You can empathize, but it doesn’t mean you have to give them your approval or consensus. To sound a little sciencey, this is what Robert Hogan, an international authority on personality assessment and leadership, says: “[Empathy is] the intellectual or imaginative apprehension of another’s condition or state of mind.” Now, Hogan doesn’t mention giving them a nod when in empathy.

So, when in empathy, you can say, “I can understand you, but I don’t agree with you.”

Empathy | What Are The 3 Types of Empathy?

Paul Ekman, an American psychologist and professor emeritus at the University of California, San Francisco, and a pioneer in the field of emotions and micro-expressions, classifies empathy into three types:

1. Cognitive empathy or perspective-taking

The recognizing and understanding of another’s thoughts and feelings. Cognitive empathy is feeling by thinking. It is the form of empathy that psychopaths have lots of—they understand what causes you the most pain and torture you with exactly that—with zero sympathies towards you. So, a psychopath can feel empathy, but not sympathy (we have a video by Brené Brown explaining the difference later in this post). People who score higher on cognitive empathy have more grey matter in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex area of the brain.

2. Emotional empathy or affective empathy

The vicarious sharing of feelings after an emotional interaction. In this, you understand as well as feel how the shoe pinches the other person. The people in the medical care profession usually have this type of empathy; they fully understand and feel your pain. This type of empathic response is called empathic distress or personal distress. People who score higher on affective empathy have more grey matter in an area of the brain called the anterior insula.

3. Compassionate empathy or empathic concern

The action part of the previous two types, or an impulse to act after understanding and feeling another’s experience. In this, after you can understand and feel the other person’s woe, you take action to resolve it for them. Compassion is a tender response to another’s suffering. Compassion comes with a wish to act. In contrast to empathy, compassion does not mean sharing the suffering of the other.

Cognitive empathy is a well-known forerunner of compassionate empathy. Compassion cannot exist without empathy.

[Some of you might be interested in this: According to some experts, empathy is different from the Theory of Mind (ToM). The ToM is our intellectual ability to assume and presume other peoples’ beliefs, intentions, and thoughts. Empathy, on the other hand, is the capacity to understand and share the emotional experiences of others (Gallese, 2003). While the ToM can be seen as “cognitive” perspective-taking, empathy is known as “emotional” perspective-taking.]

Empathy | Where does it live in our brains?

Studies show “I feel your pain” is much more than a figure of speech. We actually do feel the pain of others.

Empathy is a hardwired capacity in our brains. Brain research shows there is a “neural relay mechanism” that allows empathic people to subconsciously mimic the postures, mannerisms, and facial expressions of others, as compared to unempathic persons.

Neuroscientists have seen this mirroring capacity even at the level of single muscle fibers. So, if a person’s hand muscle is poked by a needle, the same brain areas are activated in the person who is watching it from a distance. Even simply watching or copying an emotional expression stimulates a similar network in the brain of the observer.

We feel another person’s pain, but only in a lessened form. This lessening of the sensation makes it possible for us to empathize but also not get crushed by another’s distress.

From brain scan studies, scientists have zeroed in on a region called the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) which becomes active when one experiences pain, and could also become active when one watched others in pain.

Interestingly, the same studies also showed when observing another person’s pain, this region is more active in people with high levels of empathy (and less active in psychopaths).

The Danish research team led by Giacomo Rizzolatti discovered a group of nerve cells in the ACC that become active when a rat sees another rat in pain. These are called “mirror neurons”. Rizzolatti says these cells form the biological basis of empathy and compassion.

Empathy | Why is empathy important in society

Empathy is … a key ingredient of successful relationships because it helps us understand the perspectives, needs, and intentions of others.

Look at the picture below. It’s easy to guess these two ladies are listening and talking to each other with empathy. Empathic communication means listening to a person while reflecting, clarifying, and amplifying their experience, without forcing one’s own words into the conversation.

empathy-is-a-social-emotion

Empathy is understanding another person’s feelings without passing judgment on them. It is a “social emotion” without which we might even become a threat to society. We must have empathy to have good, authentic relationships.

To be empathic is about listening to understand the reality and meaning behind the spoken words. It is listening without judging, trying to change the other person, or thinking up what should our answers be. It is listening with unconditional respect.

Can a relationship last without empathy? Can a society thrive without empathy? No. Empathy was the key tool that got us this far in the Natural Selection game. Without empathy, we wouldn’t have helped each other survive through crises.

1. How did empathy help build early human societies?

Time for a great story: A student once asked the famous anthropologist Margaret Mead, “What is the earliest sign of civilization?”

The student expected her to say the wheel, a grinding stone, or a weapon. But Mead replied, “A healed femur.”

Mead said the first evidence of civilization was a 15,000 years old healed femur (thighbone) found in an archaeological site. She explained, in the animal kingdom, if you break your leg, you die. You cannot run from danger, get to the river for a drink, or hunt for food. You are meat for the prowling predators. You get eaten. No animal survives a broken leg long enough for the bone to heal.

A broken femur that has healed is the evidence that someone has taken time to stay with the one who fell, has bound up the wound, has carried the person to safety, and has tended the person through recovery. A healed femur indicates that someone has helped a fellow human, rather than abandoning them to save their own life.

Helping someone else through difficulty is where civilization started. And that help could come only after empathy.

2. How to increase empathy in relationships?

Having empathy is a key relationship skill. And like any other skill, we can get better at it through practice. Rick Hanson, a psychologist, suggests empathy can be practiced better by expressing these four basic skills:

  1. Paying attention – listening with full attention, without interrupting, to what they are saying, keeping the focus on their experience.
  2. Inquiring – asking open-ended questions relating to what they are talking about (e.g., How do you feel about him/her? or What do you think they’ll do?).
  3. Digging down – trying to find more about their story, the deeper emotions behind their expressed feelings, imagining how the person might be suffering from inside, wondering how their experiences have shaped them thus.
  4. Double-checking – repeating to them what they are saying to keep it clear what they are saying (e.g., “Let me say back what I hear you saying. Are you saying that …?”)

3. Does empathy has any negative sides?

Empathy has a dark side too. By the dictates of evolution, nature wired us to empathize most with those who are alike. We don’t seem to care much for others who are socially and culturally different from us. Emotional empathy, or emotional sharing, most easily occurs among members of the same “tribe.”

We have maximum empathy for those who look like or act like us. We feel the most for those who have suffered like us or with whom we share a common goal. This results in biases in various communities and can take the extreme form of racism. Learn more about the five hurtful ways of empathy.

Meanwhile, the poets explain empathy in relationships as only they can:

Empathy vs Sympathy | Video

In this lovingly animated short video by RSA, Brené Brown reminds us we can create a genuine empathic connection only when we are brave enough to get in touch with our frailties.

Brené Brown on Empathy

Empathy | Books

  • Emotions Revealed – Paul Ekman
  • The Empathy Effect – Helen Riess
  • Against Empathy – Paul Bloom
  • The Art of Empathy – Karla McLaren
  • Empathy: A History – Susan Lanzoni
  • Intellectual Empathy – Maureen Linker

Final Words

Empathy is our ability to sense a person’s emotions and imagine what they might think or feel. We can have empathy as shared happiness or shared suffering—the positive or negative nature of their emotional state does not matter.

• • •

Empathy can also hurt you.

• • •

Author Bio: Written and reviewed by Sandip Roy — medical doctor, psychology writer, and happiness researcher. Founder and Chief Editor of The Happiness Blog. Writes on mental well-being, happiness, positive psychology, and philosophy (especially Stoicism).


Our Happiness Story!


If you liked it, please spread the word.

Понравилась статья? Поделить с друзьями:
  • What is the definition of the word culture
  • What is the definition of the word content
  • What is the definition of the word compound
  • What is the definition of the word civil rights
  • What is the definition of the word church