What is the average word size

The average word length in English language is 4.7 characters.

Likewise, people ask, how many words is the average essay?

On average, there are 100 to 200 words in a paragraph. And as a general rule with essays, you’ll have an introduction, body, and conclusion. So you can assume that a 1500-word essay will likely to have about 5-8 paragraphs.

Secondly, how can you do a character count in Word?

Open the document in Word that you want to count the characters in. Click the «Review» tab. Click «Word Count» in the Proofing section. The Word Count window opens and displays the numbers of characters in the document with and without spaces.

What is the D word?

‘The DWord is the longest-running online community for professionals in the documentary film industry. The name «DWord» is defined as «industry euphemism for documentary,» as in: «We love your film but we don’t know how to sell it. It’s a dword.» As of 2019 it has over 17,000 members in 130 countries.

What word takes 3 hours to say?

A man has been recorded spending more than three hours to pronounce what is supposedly the longest word in the English language. ‘Methionylthreonylthreonylglutaminylarginyl isoleucine’ is the chemical name of ‘titin’ (also known as ‘connectin’) — the largest known protein. It has 189,819 letters.

Write Your Answer

It depends, because there are two main ways of measuring it.

The first way is to look at every word in the dictionary and take the average length. This will be shorter if you use a small dictionary and longer if you use a thick dictionary, because thick dictionaries include uncommon words, and uncommon words are longer on average.

The second way is to look at a particular text or group of texts. A large collection of texts organized to be used to answer questions about language is called a «corpus.» The average word length in a corpus depends on how that corpus was assembled. Does it include children’s literature? Shorter word length. Chemistry research papers? Longer word length. Dialogue? Shorter. Postmodern theory? Longer.

The first way will always give you a bigger number than the second way, because in real writing, short words like «a» and «the» are repeated many times. In the dictionary, they are only listed once.

But no matter which way you choose, there is a big range depending on how it’s done.

The New York Times averages 4.9 characters

An editor once counseled me to change all the instances of employment in an article to jobs.

Length of a word online

Short and sweet Why write employment when you can write jobs? Save your readers processing time by choosing short words. Image by Serhii Khanas

Great advice. More than 80 years of readability research demonstrate that short, simple, familiar words are easiest to read and understand.

So if short words are better, how long should your words be?

If you’re writing in English, keep them to five characters on average.

This is important. Because short words are the No. 1 predictor of readability.

Write like The Times.

When I recommend to my clients that they limit their word length to five characters per word, they roll their eyes.

But when I tell them I know they can do it, because The New York Times does it every day, they sit up and take note.

We analyzed all stories in one edition of The New York Times. (We skipped the sports pages, thinking they might skew our results.) On that day, Times words:

  • Ranged from an average of 4 characters per word (“Seeing the World Through Its Barbecue Joints“) to an average of 5.6 characters (“Wright Architecture School Sets Fund-raising Goal for Independence”).
  • Averaged 4.9 characters per word.
  • Weighed in at a median of 4.9 characters per word.

And, remember, the Times is covering topics ranging from rocket science and brain surgery.

What’s your average word length? Could you make big stories more accessible to more people if it were shorter?

Benchmark your word length against the best.

Need more evidence?

  • The Wall Street Journal averages 4.8 characters per word. And that’s covering the most complex machinations of business.
  • The BBC averages 4.7 characters per word. And that’s covering global news. (Benchmark your copy against telecomm and other trades.)
  • The average word in the English language is 4.7 characters. Can you keep your average word length to 4.7 characters?
  • Words of more than two syllables are hard words, according to the Gunning-Mueller Fog Index. The SMOG (Simple Measure of Gobbledygook) index agrees. Can you write, on average, in easy words?
  • Of the 235 words in the Gettysburg Address (that’s fewer than the number on the back of a potato chip package today), 174 of them have only one syllable.
  • Winston Churchill entertained himself by writing essays of one-syllable words. (And he’s the only person, as far as I know, who ever slayed Nazis with his words.) The man won a Nobel Laureate for literature for his writing.

Two syllables? One? 4.7 characters?

Why so short?

Why so short?

The words we use most often in the English language are Anglo-Saxon. And most of those are single-syllable words: the, of, and, to, a, in, that, it, is, was, I and so on.

Why so short?

Short and sweet The words we use most often in the English language are one-syllable long.

In fact, the 54 most-used words in the English language have just one syllable, according to WordCount, a website that chronicles how we use language. “About” comes in 55th, followed by even more one-syllable words. These super-short words make it possible to keep your overall average short.

And short words serve you, your audience and your organization better. Why not use them?

How to keep words short.

How do you keep your words within these character limits? Open your Microsoft Word document, and start using the word count tool.

Don’t use Microsoft Word? An online character count tool or other character counter will work just as well to help you find your word and character count.

At this point, you’re not worrying about keyword density, keeping your message under 280 characters or hitting any particular number of words in an article. You’re not counting words, you’re making sure the number of characters per word hits the target.

Once your online word counter tool says you’ve hit the mark, you can move onto online tools for SEO.

Learn other ways to keep words short.

Learn more about writing short words.

  • How long should your message be?

    Would your message be twice as good if it were half as long?

    Yes, the research says. The shorter your message, the more likely readers are to read it, understand it and make good decisions based on it.

    Rev Up Readability — our clear-writing workshop, starting May 26So how long is too long? What’s the right length for your piece? Your paragraphs? Your sentences? Your words?

    Find out at Rev Up Readability — our clear-writing workshop, starting May 26.

    There, you’ll use a cool (free!) tool to analyze your message for 33 readability metrics. You’ll leave with quantifiable targets, tips and techniques for measurably boosting readability.

    Save up to $100 with our group discounts.

    Register for Rev Up Readability — our clear-writing workshop, starting May 26

    Browse all upcoming master classes.

This page was last modified 2007-07-24 11:34:46 by Puchu.Net user Choco. (Show history)

Just recently I had to think about average word length in relationship to text-positioning in a menu. The basic problem is: how many characters do we really need to contain one line of text?

Contents

  • 1 Character-per-line Studies
  • 2 Average Word Length
  • 3 Unrelated Find
  • 4 Conclusion

Character-per-line Studies

If you have worked with a terminal before, or the DOS command prompt, that number is 80. 80 characters-per-line seems like a reasonable number, but I wanted to learn more about this, so I searched on-line and found some studies done about this topic:

  1. http://psychology.wichita.edu/surl/usabilitynews/42/text_length.htm
  2. http://psychology.wichita.edu/surl/usabilitynews/72/LineLength.htm
  3. http://www.psych.utoronto.ca/~muter/Abs1984b.htm

The common theme here seems to be that people read slower when the character-per-line count is low. Higher numbers such as 90 is better. But these studies are done with computer monitors; what about when televisions are used? Font size on a TV screen is often larger, because the display quality can change due to underlying technology, the design of the font and varying viewing environment.

It seems to me that just looking at just characters isn’t enough. Communication efficiency here is judged often by reading speed, which makes sense to me because the more effective delivery allows readers to read more words per minute. I decided to research on average word length:

  1. http://blogamundo.net/lab/wordlengths/
  2. http://www.askoxford.com/oec/mainpage/oec02/
  3. http://www.w3.org/International/articles/article-text-size
  4. http://www.tug.org/TUGboat/Articles/tb16-3/tb48soj2.pdf

The data from blogamundo.net tells us that average word lengths in English, French, Spanish and German are approximately 5.10, 5.13, 5.22 and 6.26. 5-letters is the unit of calculating typing speed (the word in WPM is calculated in 5-letter units). Adding another character for space, it seems that 80 characters-per-line will fit about 10 words per line, sounds good?

Well, data from askoxford.com shows that just the top 10 most frequently appearing lemmas (words such as the, of, and, to, that, have) account for 25% of the sources in the Oxford English Corpus. Increase that size to the top 100 most frequently appearing lemmas (words such as from, because, go, me, our, well, way) will cover 50% of the sources used by the Oxford dictionary. In other words, the low average word length is heavily influenced by short words frequently used in the English language.

The last data from w3.org and tug.org show that the ratio and average of word length between languages. English has the shortest average length, and other Latin-based languages are at least a few characters longer. These sites also show that the average word length can vary greatly.

Unrelated Find

Not related, but this shows the number of words in various Latin-based languages:

  1. http://arxiv.org/ftp/cs/papers/0006/0006032.pdf

English isn’t an easy language to learn! Just look at the number of words there are, heh.

Conclusion

Well, I did form my own opinion on what I think is the best way of representing the texts on TV, taking into consideration effects of working with different languages. But it isn’t scientifically proven and so I won’t include it here. It isn’t 80. Hopefully there are many links above that you can browse through to form your own opinion, and should you find other interesting links please forward to me. Have a nice day!

Puchu.Net

Document is accessible from
http://www.puchu.net.
© 2002-2010 Sean Yang, Karen Yang, Don Yang and/or respective authors,
all rights reserverd.

This material may contain (biased) opinions,
inappropriate materials for numerous individuals,
work of other authors from the internet,
links that refer to other web documents and resources, or
origial work that cannot be use for personal or commercial purposes.
Please respect the work of original authors.

+ Tools

  • Log in / create account
  • What links here
  • Related changes
  • Please log in to add blog/news entries.

  • Show work in progress →
  • Validate XHTML
  • Hide tools ...

Think word count isn’t important in writing? Well, think again. As a writer, the number of words in a piece matters a great deal; it is the only measure of productivity you have. Therefore, you need to ensure the number is within what’s expected for your genre and category. So how many words should your text have? Let’s take a comprehensive look at our word count list, and find out how it all works.

A good average sentence length

There’s no perfect length, however, a well-written sentence should have 15-20 words on average. Using simple and short sentences will help more people to understand your content. But restrain from using uniform sentence lengths if you don’t want your writings to be choppy and without flow.
Though there’s the possibility of long structured sentences being balanced and readable, comprehension often drops and readers may struggle to understand the whole content. People expect you to get to the point, especially your busiest readers. They don’t have the time to slow down to deconstruct the long, complicated sentences and contemplate the clauses. By making it simple, it shows you respect your content reader’s time and interest.

How many words in a paragraph?

As you can expect, there is no fixed number of words that a paragraph should have. A rule of thumb: the paragraphs are usually about 100 to 200 words long, which is about 6-8 sentences. Nevertheless, it all depends on the ideas, and ideas come in many sizes. So the paragraph can be long enough to reach its end. But we can’t ignore the fact that readers don’t like seeing blocks of paragraphs. And it’s for that reason you should stick to one concept or idea per paragraph to avoid creating very long paragraphs. Simply shift to a new paragraph when you shift to a new concept/idea.

How many words in an essay?

A standard essay must be a minimum of 3-5 paragraphs, but if you’re not instructed to write the given paragraphs, there’s no point sticking to this extremely limiting rule. This is handy in academic writing, where your paragraphs are expected to be longer than most essays. On average, there are 100 to 200 words in a paragraph. And as a general rule with essays, you’ll have an introduction, body, and conclusion. So you can assume that a 1500-word essay will likely to have about 5-8 paragraphs. Remember your essay will be evaluated on the content you present, not the number of paragraphs.

How long should a book be?

Though the length of your book is determined by the details needed to make it good, it is important to adhere to the expected word count in your genre and category. It demonstrates how well you understand your market. And in line with this, here are the industry norms worth knowing.

  • Under 500 words is generally considered flash fiction
  • A short story: 1,000-8,000 words
  • Novella Story: 10,000-40,000 words

Novel

A 40,000-word manuscript can be considered to be a novel, but to be eligible for finisher status, you’re required to write a minimum of 50,000 words. The maximum word is generally 60,000-100,000 words.

Sci-Fi and Fantasy Books

There is no rule on the word limit regarding science and fantasy fiction books. However, most of the books usually fall within the 90,000-120,000 words. The publisher and agents are more lenient on word limits for this genre; therefore, you’re not likely to lose out on a deal if you exceeded the number of words. You may be wondering why the books in this category are an exception. Well, reading epic novels of this genre are interesting and require the audience to take some time to create the fantasy world around them, and then imagine themselves in it.

Adult Novels

The generally recommended words usually fall between 80,000 and 100,000. If your manuscript is over 100,000 words, publishing it could prove to be more expensive.

Children Novels

The word length for children novels is not set in stone, but even so, its likely to be around 20,000 to 40,000 words.

What is the average word count for an email?

According to a study conducted by Email Labs, people spend about 15-20 seconds reading an email. Sending the readers lengthy email messages may make them feel overwhelmed, and might not read through it. Now, you may be tempted to ask, “what’s the right number of words for an email? “ Well, it all depends on the ideas or information you want to deliver. A rule of thumb: the shorter message, the better. Always try to limit your email to around 300 words or less. If your email message needs to be longer, it’s absolutely okay. However, focus on the main message, eliminate the distractions, and keep the content easy to scan.

There’s no set minimum or maximum, regardless of the type of report. If you’d like a guesstimate, just assume the audience will take around 10-20 minutes reading the report. And on average, your readers can read 1 double-spaced page or 250 words in a minute. In 10 minutes they would have read about 2,500-4,500 words. But that’s just an approximation.
Remember a few people read an entire report, so keep it simple and not too long.

How many words should your application have?

Whether you are applying for a job or college, it’s extremely important to keep in mind that there’ll be a long list of applicants. When you present a very long resume, you’re simply scaring off the person who’ll be reading it. Don’t forget he/she has other applications to review. Even if you were to be more qualified, there’s a great possibility of wasting your chances. So if you are not instructed to write within a certain word limit, just make sure your application is short as possible, typically less than 500 words.

How long should your motivational letters be?

While there are no exceptions, your letter of motivation shouldn’t be longer than one page. Make it around 300-500 words. It is crucial to keep it shorter, but make sure you highlight your most relevant qualifications and what you have to offer.

If you want to check your word and character count, improve writing style and word choice online, bookmark our online editor.

Responses. The average word length in English language is 4.7 characters.

How many pages is 90000 words?

180 pages

What does 1 000 words look like?

a page. As you may have already figured out, 1,000 words is approximately 3 pages of double-spaced typed manuscript with one-inch margins on the side. Three pages. That’s all.

Is it possible to write 1000 words in an hour?

The quickest I’ve ever written a 1,000-word article is one hour. In this case, I already knew the subject matter well and didn’t have to back up every fact in the essay with references.

What is 750 words in pages?

Pages by Word Count

Word Count Pages (single spaced) Pages (double spaced)
500 Words 1 Page 2 Pages
600 Words 1⅓ Page 2⅔ Pages
750 Words 1½ Pages 3 Pages
800 Words 1⅗ Pages 3⅕ Pages

What is the shortest novel ever written?

for sale: baby shoes never worn

Is 20 000 words enough for a book?

This means that you cannot fit many words on a printed page and keep it readable. This translates into just 20,000 to 25,000 words for a 100 page book, 30,000 to 42,500 words for a 150-page book, perhaps just 40,000 words for a 200-page book.

What does 20000 words look like?

Answer: 20,000 words is 40 pages single-spaced or 80 pages double-spaced. Typical documents that are 20,000 words include include novels, novellas, and other published books. It will take approximately 67 minutes to read 20,000 words.

How many pages is 300000 words?

600 pages

How many words is a 150 page book?

37,500 words

What size book sells the best?

The most common trim sizes for standard trade fiction and nonfiction books include: 5″ x 8″, 5.5″ x 8.5″, and 6″ x 9″.

How many pages is 40000 words?

80 pages

Is 30000 words enough for a book?

Answer: 30,000 words is 60 pages single-spaced or 120 pages double-spaced. Typical documents that are 30,000 words include include novels, novellas, and other published books. It will take approximately 100 minutes to read 30,000 words.

What is 3000 words in pages?

3,000 words is the equivalent of around 6 pages of A4 single-spaced, and 12 pages of A4 double-spaced. That’s provided you’re using normal margins of 1-inch, and a standard font such as Times New Roman or Arial in a size of 12pt.

How long does it take to write 30000 words?

about 12.5 hours

How long does it take to read 30000 words?

about 1.7 hours

Can you read 200 pages in 4 hours?

Answer: 200 pages will take about 5.6 hours to read for the average reader.

What is considered a fast reader?

A normal rate for learning is 100-200 wpm, and for comprehension it is 200-400 wpm. Speed reading is normally done at a rate of around 400-700 wpm. Anything above 500-600 wpm means sacrificing comprehension, although this varies from person to person.

How many minutes is 1000 words?

6.67 minutes

How long is a 5 7 minute speech?

Although pacing varies, a 5-minute speech is roughly 750 words. Publication coach Daphne Gray-Grant says that the average person speaks about 125-150 words per minute—meaning 5 minutes of talking would entail about 625-750 words. That’s about the typical length of a blog article!

Is it possible to read 1000 words a minute?

“If you understand and appreciate that,” says Elizabeth Schotter, a cognitive psychologist at UC San Diego, “it becomes really obvious that no human being can read 1,000 or 2,000 words per minute and maintain the same levels of comprehension they do at 200 or 400 words per minute.”

1. INTRODUCTION

Every language reflects the context in which it is spoken. While the idea that there is a direct and deterministic link between language structures and thought – the so-called Sapir-Whorf hypothesis – is debatable, there is little doubt that the vocabulary of a language is a reflection of the speaker’s physical and mental world, and consequently, the shifting frequencies in different words also reflect changes in the material world, society, and prevalent concerns. Earlier studies of ‘world views’ as embodied in language were limited to particular concepts reflecting cultural stereotypes or grammatical phenomena perceived as having an influence on speakers’ behaviour. Due to the creation of large, diachronic text corpora and the development of mathematical methods for data processing now we can apply a new approach to the issue of possible connections between a language and the world (societal, physical, mental, etc.) of its speakers. In the present article we study changes in average world length. This is a measurable entity which can be statistically analysed, and, as we will show, there are non-random changes involving word length over time in different languages which promise to shed light on changes in the extra-linguistic world.

Much attention has been given lately to language dynamics, including data-driven or simulation-based investigations into language change viewed in the context of how speakers interact and organize themselves in different networks is (Wichmann 2008; Croft 2000; Hruschka et al. 2009; Trudgill 2004; Kalampokis et al. 2007; Michel et al. 2011; Nettle 1999; Blythe 2012; Fagyal et al. 2010). The present article focuses on such parameters as word length and frequency. Although the word length is significantly correlated with other typological parameters (Wichmann et al. 2011) it also varies within languages (across words belonging to different grammatical categories) and depends on the frequency, with shorter words tending to be used more frequently than longer ones. Indeed, the word frequency and length are fundamental parameters in the study of psychological processes of language acquisition and usage (Sidney et al. 2000; Baddeley and Scott 1971), and the frequency of language units is the basis of usage-based sociolinguistic models of language evolution (Baxter et al. 2009).

One observes some clear general laws of language change including the S-shaped curve of innovation diffusion (Fagyal et al. 2010) and the effect of word frequency on change rates whereby the less frequently a word is used, the greater the probability that it will change (Pagel et al. 2007). As a word drops out of use it may be compensated for by another, whereby frequencies of different words become linked. But the process of word frequency change is not only connected with the process of substitution of one form by another one. Importantly, word frequency can vary under the influence of sociocultural factors and thus, may reflect these factors.

Our article is devoted to the processes of word frequency change under the influence of sociocultural factors. It continues recent work related to the quantitative analysis of cultural trends or ‘culturomics’ (Michel et al. 2011). The subject of our study is regularities of word average length variations and factors which cause these changes.

Data concerning average word length are available for different languages. Thus, for instance, according to the Wolfram Alpha computational knowledge engine (http://www.wolframalpha.com/ input/?i=average+english%20+word+length) the average word length in English is 5.1 letters and that in Russian it is 5.28 (Sharov 2011). Nevertheless, accurate quantitative analyses of the dynamics of this parameter have not been carried out. Average word length can both decrease and increase in the course of time. Over large periods of time a language may change its morphological type (agglutinative, inflective, or isolating) (Croft 2003), and changes in the morphological type of a language are expected to influence average word length. But such changes happen very slowly. It has recently been shown (Wichmann et al. 2013) that the world’s language families tend to have typical average world lengths which differ from family to family. This means that a characteristic average word length can persist for the amount of time that corresponds to the typical age of known language families, which is several thousand years. Language typologists and historical linguists are concerned with the diachronic study of languages over large time scales (Heggarty 2007). Here we are interested in fluctuations of word length over relatively short time spans of a few hundred years, not in the type of language-internal typological changes which happen over a larger time scale of thousands of years.

In particular, we will look at English and Russian. These languages have not changed radically during the two last centuries, so when we still observe fluctuations in average word length then they must be due to the factors other than typological change. We hypothesize that the fluctuations are due to changes in the frequencies of different kinds of words, these changes are, in turn, caused by sociocultural factors.

As described in the following section, studying the dynamics of average word length at relatively short periods of time (decades or centuries) has become possible because of the creation of big diachronic text corpora.

2. METHODS AND DATA

The creation of the digital library Google Books and the word frequency calculation enabled by the Google Books Ngram Viewer (http://books.google.com/ngrams/) has created extraordinary new opportunities for studying the evolution of the lexicon of different languages. As demonstrated in the seminal paper by Michel et al. (2011), these data can fruitfully be applied for analyzing cultural trends.

Some cautionary words concerning the nature of the corpus are in order. It should be taken into account, for instance, that the Ngram Viewer does not offer morphological analyses. Different inflectional forms of one and the same root (for example, English chair and chairs, or various case-forms of a particular Russian noun) are considered to be different words on a par with completely unrelated forms such as English chair and table. Thus, in what follows our definition of ‘word’ also does not take into account that some words are related through shared lexemes while others are not.

Using the total set of n-grams presented on the site, average word length can be counted for each year (using MatLab, for instance). Although the English corpus comprises texts dated back to 1520, great amounts of text for reliable statistic computations are only available for 1800 onwards, according to the recommendation of the creators of the Ngram Viewer.

Average word length can be calculated using the following formula:

, (1)

where is the length of i-th word and is its relative frequency (usage probability). Let us consider that a particular word frequency (k-th word) varies, but that correlations of other words are invariable. According to the normalization requirement we derive

(2)

where is an average word length without considering the k-th word. Suppose that the k-th word usage frequency varies over . As it is seen from the formula (2), the change of average word length is

(3)

Deriving the -value from the second formula (2), we get

(4)

The -value shows the partial contribution of a k-th word to the average word length change. For the majority of words, except the several most frequently used ones, is realized as

(5)

Thus, the increase of the use of words whose length is smaller than the current average word length results in a decrease of average word length and vice versa.

The value derived from the formula (5) enables us to evaluate the contribution of a given word to the average word length change. Correspondingly, we can distinguish the words which contribute most to the change of average word length.

Two different approaches to frequency calculation should be taken into consideration when comparing the further results and diagrams. First, all numbers were excluded. There are also many OCR errors and some onomatopoeia, but their contribution to overall statistical data can generally be neglected. Our corrections were also taken into account when calculating frequencies. The Ngram Viewer shows frequencies normalized by the total number of 1-grams, including numbers, but we normalize them by the number of 1-grams that can be considered true words.

The Ngram Viewer comprises texts written in seven languages, but here we analyse the data for just two of them – English and Russian.

Fig. 1 shows the average word length distribution in the English language over time according to the Ngram Viewer data.

Fig. 1. Average word length over time in the English language. Each curve indicates the results for the total and fiction-only corpus, and also for British and American English separately

Three periods can be discerned in the graph: from 1800 to 1900, from 1901 to 1994, and from 1995 to 2008. In spite of fluctuations due to the relative scarcity of data, average word length is almost constant during the first period; it increases in a linear fashion during the second period, and decreases during the third period.

Words can be divided into two major groups: function words, which indicate a grammatical relationship (articles, conjunctions, particles, prepositions etc.), and content words (nouns, verbs, adjectives, numerals, etc.). Pronouns and auxiliary verbs were included into the first group. Most function words are short, consisting of only 2–3 letters (less than the average word length). Increase in the frequency of these word reduces average word length and vice versa. Almost all content words are long (longer than average word length), having the opposite influence: increase in frequency of these words lengthens average word length and vice versa.

Frequency variability of function and content words is studied separately. Fig. 2 shows changes of average word length of all the words examined and of long and short words separately.

One can see that short words, most of which are function words, almost do not change their length. At the same time, the shape of the curve which shows change of average length of long words, most of which are content words, is similar to the curve which describes word length of all long words.

Fig. 2. Dynamics of average length of English short and long words

Fig. 3. Relative contribution to average word length change in Russian of words of different length for the two time intervals

Fig. 3 shows the relative contribution to average word length change of words of different length in Russian. There are peaks at two- and twelve-letter words. The period after 1939 is considered in order to avoid the influence of the orthography reform of 1918. The biggest value of average word length for the Russian language dates back to 1976. Correspondingly, two time periods are distinguished: from 1939 to 1976 when a tendency to average word length increase is observed, and 1976–2008 where the opposite tendency takes place. As the number of long words grows, their total contribution (approximately 70 per cent) becomes higher than that of short words.

3. THE INFLUENCE OF SOCIAL FACTORS ON CONTENT WORDS: A HYPOTHESIS

It can be assumed that the increase in average length is determined by vocabulary extension as new words emerge. As there are a limited number of short combinations of letters and most of them are used in any language, neologisms such as computer, the Internet, globalization are necessarily long. But there are other factors involved in word length variation. Fig. 4 shows the amount of words used in different periods of time.

Fig. 4. The total amount of English words used in different periods (solid black line). The share of words which contributed most to the selected word group is also shown

The diagram shows that the number of words (the total number of words and the number of words which are used most frequently) grows in much the same ways in the nineteenth and twentieth centuries. Thus, this factor cannot explain why word length increases rapidly in the twentieth century, stays constant in the nineteenth century and decreases in the twenty-first century.

As a hypothesis, we suggest the following mechanism of influence of social factors on average word length. Average word length growing systematically over a large period of time reflects an active process of word formation which is connected with a certain idea that dominates in society. The actuality of the new idea, which is verbalized by a particular set of words, leads to a significant growth of the usage of these words. These concepts are usually represented by multi-syllable words. The new concepts are often complicated and the words which define them are formed through the help of affixation, for example socialism, globalization. Besides, as already mentioned, the number of short words is limited in any language and cannot be used to verbalize all notions. All this leads to average word length growth.

On the other hand, decrease in the usage of content words is explained by the loss of actuality of a certain lexical field which, in turn, influences average word length on the whole. The principle of economy in language should also be mentioned as a contributor to the decrease in average word length. According to this principle, speakers will try to transmit as much information as possible using the fewest possible linguistic means. The phonetic and corresponding orthographical structure of a term may become reduced due to contractions in the pronunciation, forms may be truncated, and abbreviations may be introduced – all as a result of the accommodation of words to the needs of speakers.

We hypothesize that during the time when average word length is relatively constant and the change of average word length is sporadic there is a low influx of prevalent new ideas. In other words, a change of dominating concepts will result in a rapid change of the word stock connected with them. Graphically it is expected to incur changes in the slope of a curve or the appearance of small ‘gaps’ reflecting the situation when old concept are not topical anymore and the new one have not yet been formed. If average word length grows close to linearly during significantly long periods of time (e.g., in English from 1900 to 2000), it can be assumed that concepts do not change rapidly at that time but rather evolve slowly.

4. EVOLUTION OF CONTENT WORDS: INVESTIGATING THE HYPOTHESIS EMPIRICALLY

We will start our survey by examining the content words in English during the period from 1990 to 2000. Below there is a list of words which contributed most to average word length increase during this period. The numbers show the level of word contribution to total average length increase. The contribution is calculated according to the formula (5).

1. development 0.00648

2. information 0.00632

3. international 0.00596

4. university 0.00521

5. political 0.00415

6. relationship 0.00413

7. economic 0.00371

8. research 0.00346

9. production 0.00310

10. significant 0.00304

One can see that semantically these words pertain to the key ideas of the twentieth century and reflect the priorities of the English speaking society of that time.

The following approach will help to test the hypothesis that social factors influence the frequencies of content words. The interval from 1800 to 2000 is divided into smaller ones of 25 years each (roughly corresponding to a generation), with the exception of the last interval from 2001 to 2008, which contains data for this eight-year period. The contribution of each word to the change of average word length is calculated for each interval using the formula in (5). The Appendix shows words which contributed most to average word length increase or decrease for each of these periods.

Some remarks are in order: (a) There are a lot of errors in the data during the first interval (1800–1825) due to wrong symbol recognition (OCR errors) caused by low quality of printing and the poor condition of books. For instance, the letters s and t are often recognized as f. This introduces some noise in the data, but not enough to invalidate overall conclusions. (b) The number of short content words is quite small, so they do not contribute much to an average word length. They were disregarded. (c) Words beginning with capital and small letters are merged and are cited in the tables in their typical orthographic forms.

For further analysis, the most ‘influential’ words are distinguished. These are the ten words which contributed most either to word average length decrease or increase during at least two periods. The data are summed up in Tables 1 and 2. The plus sign means that the word belongs to the top ten ‘influential’ words.

Let us analyse these data. One should pay special attention to the fact that the word lists at the beginning of the nineteenth and twenty-first centuries are not similar to word lists of other periods. The language development between 2000 and 2008 will be regarded later. As for the beginning of the nineteenth century, obviously, some interesting and important events took place, but the eighteenth century data are needed to survey changes taking place around the turn of the century. Unfortunately Google Books does not contain enough data, so we cannot deal further with this. The word development is at the top of the list of words which contributed to the average word length over one and a half centuries. This serves as evidence that this is one of the key concepts for English-speaking society. Besides development, the words which are common for different periods of the nineteenth century are one, position, government, American, and conditions. They belong to different semantic groups and it is not clear that they pertain to some single idea or a set of ideas. Alongside with the word ‘development’, the common words for different periods of the twentieth century from Table 1 include: international, production, individual, education, political, university, and information. It seems that these are key words in the modern English-speaking world relating to the global economic and political system based on education and information. There are some common words (marked by the plus sign) for different periods of the twentieth century presented in Table 1, which indicates that the system evolves gradually.

Table 1

Content words which contributed mostly to average word length

Word

1800–1825

1825–1850

1850–1875

1875–1900

1900–1925

1925–1950

1950–1975

1975–2000

2000–2008

one

+

+

position

+

+

government

+

+

+

development

+

+

+

+

+

+

American

+

+

+

conditions

+

+

+

international

+

+

+

production

+

+

individual

+

+

education

+

+

political

+

+

university

+

+

information

+

+

The absolute values of the contribution to average word length are also interesting. During the nineteenth century, the contribution value is 0.003 (except for 1800–1825), and during the twentieth century the contribution value is 0.005 (except for 2000–2008). In other words, the frequency of international, production, individual, education, political, university, and information decreased in the twentieth century more rapidly than that of the one, position, government, American, and conditions in the nineteenth century. This shows that corresponding concepts were more significant for society in the twentieth than in the nineteenth century. It is interesting that the words which contribute most to average word length increase in the twenty-first century do not belong to any single concept.

Thus, the analysis of content words contributed mostly to word frequency increase supports our hypothesis.

Let us consider the words whose frequency decreased rapidly. The data are presented in Table 2.

Table 2

Content words which contributed to average word length decrease

Word

1800–1825

1825–1850

1850–1875

1875–1900

1900–1925

1925–1950

1950–1975

1975–2000

2000–2008

particularly

+

+

immediately

+

+

+

God

+

+

+

circumstances

+

+

+

character

+

+

+

+

+

+

Christian

+

+

practically

+

+

+

Mr

+

+

+

hundred

+

+

constitution

+

+

little

+

+

necessary

+

+

The data for the early nineteenth and twenty-first centuries stand apart. It is interesting that the frequency of word character constantly decreases. On the whole, the words presented in this table belong to different semantic fields and do not reflect any particular idea.

Fig. 5. Dynamics of the top ten influential words of the twentieth century

As for the absolute values of contributions to average word length of words we can make the following observations. Both for the nineteenth and the twentieth centuries, the contribution value is 0.0025 (except for 1800–1825). The value is much larger during 1800–1825 and 2000–2008, where it is 0.01. The words with significantly decreased frequency in the twenty-first century are of special interest. The less frequently used words are the following: development, university, international, political, and information. According to Table 1, the frequency of these words rapidly increased during the previous years. So the dominant ideas of the twentieth century do not prevail any more. Thus, the analysis of content words contributing less to word frequency supports our general hypothesis.

Let us consider the diagrams of the top ten influential words in the twentieth century: development, information, international, university, political, relationship, economic, research, production, and significant. It is clearly seen that these diagrams reflect the common rules of average word length change in the nineteenth – twenty-first centuries. During the nineteenth century, the majority of these words (except for development) do not increase their frequency and even decrease in frequency. Their frequency increases in the twentieth century. In the twenty-first century the frequency of these words decreases. Further, the decrease in frequency of some words (development, production) begins earlier, approximately in 1980.

5. THE EVOLUTION OF FUNCTION WORDS

There are several dozen function words. The function words which we distinguish (see the Appendix) are the following: articles, prepositions, conjunctions, pronouns, auxiliary verbs, particles and some adverbs.

Below is a list of words which contributed most to word length increase during the twentieth century.

1. the 0.04110

2. of 0.03947

3. he 0.02033

4. it 0.01848

5. I 0.01688

6. his 0.01307

7. to 0.01136

8. was 0.00932

9. at 0.00923

10. and 0.00886

This list of function words is dominated by personal pronouns (four items). The dynamics of personal pronouns is rather interesting. It can clearly be seen that the frequency of the majority of these words is stable or decreases rapidly until the end of the twentieth century, after which the frequency of all the pronouns increases rapidly. As all of them are short, the decrease in their frequency contributes to average word length increase and the increase in their frequency in the beginning of the twenty-first century contributes to average word length decrease. This is why this factor has the same influence as the evolution of content words which was described earlier. But it is not clear whether these two factors are mutually connected.

The majority of other function words are used with a near constant frequency. Some of them show the same rules of frequency change as personal pronouns, but this tendency is much weaker.

Fig. 6. Dynamics of personal pronouns in English

6. AVERAGE WORD LENGTH DYNAMICS IN THE RUSSIAN LANGUAGE

We now turn to the Russian words in order to test our hypothesis about the relationship between changes in average word length and societal changes. It is interesting to compare English with Russian because Russian society differs greatly from the English-speaking society and has had completely different views and values for almost a century. The average word length dynamics in Russian is shown in Fig. 7. As can be seen, the overall situation is generally the same as in the English language: word length is near constant over the nineteenth century, grows in the twentieth century, but the decrease starts earlier – around 1975.

Let us consider the most interesting period in the twentieth and the early twenty-first century. It should be noted that the chosen time periods for both languages (1900–1925 etc.) are not relevant for the Russian language since several important events in Russian history as the socialist revolution (1917), World War II and the collapse of the Soviet Union took place in the middle of the intervals, which leads to some vocabulary mixing during the corresponding periods. The results for the Russian languages are, nevertheless, rather clear.

Fig. 7. The black solid line shows an average word length in the Russian language with the account of the orthographical rules of that time, the dotted line shows it excluding the letter ъ at the end of words (neutralizing the effects of one of the main rules of the orthographical reform of 1918)

Unlike what we observe in the English language, the top ten influential words for the chosen periods, that is the ones that contributed most to average word length, do not coincide much. Different concepts are present at each interval. From 1900 to 1925, the concepts related to economic issues dating back to the nineteenth century mix with revolutionary ideas. From 1925 to 1950, the most influential words relate to the ideas of the socialist regime and Soviet power and key issues of the time, such as construction and agriculture. The priorities change in the period between 1950 and 1975, now starting to revolve around manufacturing and management. The leading concepts changed again between 1975 and 2000, now relating to the idea of nationhood. Legal issues dominate between 2000 and 2008. It is typical that between 1975 and 2000, the frequency of words connected with manufacturing and socialism decreased sharply, something which contributed greatly to a decrease of average length of words during this period. The concept changes between 1925–1950 and 1950–1975 can be considered as a small gap on the diagram a bit before 1950.

Fig. 8 shows diagrams for the top ten words. The lines for the Russian language in Fig. 8 are not as smooth as the corresponding ones for English. Many of the words were used widely only after the revolution. The increase in frequency was perturbed by World War II. It should be noted that priorities changed during the 1950s: the problems of effectiveness became acute and the term revolution was left behind. It is interesting that the frequency of influential Russian words started decreasing in 1980 just as in the English.

Fig. 8. Dynamics of the top ten influential Russian words of the 20th century. Translation of the Russian words in the left window: organisations, revolution, socialist, and soviet; in the right window: production, efficiency, Russian, and state

Thus, there is a direct dependency between fluctuations of average word length and the formation of word groups which are semantically connected with a dominating concept.

Fig. 9. Dynamics of personal pronouns in Russian. Translation of the Russian words in the left window: I, we, and you (singular); in the right window: you (plural), he, she, and they

As for function words, Russian personal pronouns generally behave as English personal pronouns (Fig. 6), and a decrease in frequency takes place in 1975. Nevertheless, there is a conspicuous special behavior of the Russian first person pronoun. The rapid decrease in frequency of я (‘I’) in the Russian language after 1917, which continues up to 1975, can be explained by detraction of the role of personality during the time of socialism and the imposition of collectivist ideology.

7. CONCLUSION

The present article introduces data concerning the change of average word length in the English and Russian languages over the last two centuries based on Google Books e-library and the Ngram Viewer. It is shown that the change of word length is a result of change in word frequency. The change in frequency of content words and function words has been analysed separately.

We have shown that a decrease in average word length correlates with two socio-cultural factors: the existence of some fundamental idea or several interconnected ideas and variation of frequency of personal pronouns.

Decrease in average word length correlates with the retreat from ideas which have been dominating (the number of words which verbalize this concept decreases rapidly), and the frequency of personal pronouns increases. When there are no prevailing concepts, average word length tends not to change.

It is interesting to observe that there are some similarities in the data for the English and Russian languages in spite of great social, cultural, economic and political differences between their speakers. This shows that the regularities traced are not accidental, and also that they reflect some global, international trends exerting their influence in parallel with national trends. The most conspicuous similarity is a steeper increase in word length for both languages in the twentieth as compared with the nineteenth century (compare Fig. 1 and Fig. 7). We interpret this as an evidence of an overall increase in the speed of change affecting both the Russian- and English-speaking societies throughout the twentieth century, probably due to factors such as industrialization, urbanization, and migration, constantly driving changes in the world as perceived by the speakers whose dynamics of word usage we have studied in this paper.

The Ngram Viewer system enables the study of language evolution at the micro-level through frequency data that ultimately reflect extra-linguistic factors contingent upon cultural and historical factors.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The authors wish to thank Søren Wichmann for help and useful advice which contributed much to the improvement of the quality of the article.

This work was supported by the Russian Foundation for Basic Research through project RFBR № 12-06-00404-а.

REFERENCES

Baddeley, A. D., and Scott, D. 1971. Word Frequency and the Unit Sequence Interference Hypothesis in Short-Term Memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 10: 35–40. DOI: 10.1016/S0022-5371(71)80090-7.

Baxter, G. J., Blythe, R. A., Croft, W., and McKane, A. J. 2009. Modeling Language Change: An Evaluation of Trudgill’s Theory of the Emergence of New Zealand English. Language Variation and Change 21: 257–296.

Blythe, R. A. 2012. Neutral Evolution: A Null Model for Language Dynamics. Advances in Complex Systems 15(3–4): 1150015. DOI: 10.1142/S0219525911003414.

Croft, W. 2000. Explaining Language Change: An Evolutionary Approach. Harlow: Pearson Education.

Croft, W. 2003. Typology and Universals. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Fagyal, Z., Swarup, S., Escobar, A. M. et al. 2010. Centers and Peripheries: Network Roles in Language Change. Lingua 120: 2061–2079.

Heggarty, P. 2007. Linguistics for Archaeologists: Principles, Methods and the Case of the Incas. Cambridge Archaeological Journal 17: 311–340.

Hruschka, D. J., Christiansen, M. H., Blythe, R. A. et al. 2009. Building Social Cognitive Models of Language Change. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 13: 464–469.

Kalampokis, A., Kosmidis, K., and Argyrakis, P. 2007. Evolution of Vocabulary in Scale-Free and Random Networks. Physica A 379: 665–671.

Michel, J.-B., Shen, Y. K., and Aiden, A. P. et al. 2011. Quantitative Analysis of Culture using Millions of Digitized Books. Science 331: 176–182. DOI: 10.1126/science.1199644.

Nettle, D. 1999. Using Social Impact Theory to Simulate Language Change. Lingua 108: 95–117.

Pagel, M., Atkinson, Q. D., and Meade, A. 2007. Frequency of Word-Use Predicts Rates of Lexical Evolution throughout Indo-European History. Nature 449. DOI:10.1038/nature06176.

Segalowitz, S. J., and Lane, K. C. 2000. Lexical Access of Function versus Content Words. Brain and Language 75: 376–389. DOI: 10.1006/brln. 2000.2361.

Sharov, S. A. 2011. The Statistics of the Words in the Russian Language. In Russian (Шаров С. А. Статистика слов в русском языке). URL: http://www.lingvisto.org/artikoloj/ru_stat.html.

Trudgill, P. 2004. New Dialect Formation: The Inevitability of Colonial Englishes. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.

Wichmann, S. 2008. The Emerging Field of Language Dynamics. Language and Linguistics Compass 2.3: 442–455.

Wichmann, S., Rama, T., and Holman, E. W. 2011. Phonological Diversity, Word Length, and Population Sizes across Languages: The ASJP Evidence. Linguistic Typology 15: 177–197.

Wichmann, S., and Holman, E. W. 2013. Languages with Longer Words have More Lexical Change. In Borin, L., and Saxena, A. (eds.), Approaches to Measuring Linguistic Differences (pp. 249–284). Berlin: de Gruyter Mouton.

APPENDIX

In the following tables the numbers indicate a word’s contribution to the variation of the average words length. Positive numbers correspond to an increase of the average words length, negative ones to a decrease of the average words length.

Table 1

Function words in American English with decreasing frequency

1825–1849

1850–1874

1875–1899

1900–1924

‘he’

4.025

‘of’

5.725

‘and’

2.651

‘I’

3.339

‘to’

3.846

‘to’

3.217

‘of’

1.885

‘he’

2.966

‘be’

2.222

‘and’

2.491

‘the’

1.617

‘and’

2.665

‘his’

1.544

‘his’

1.670

‘by’

0.986

‘his’

2.647

‘by’

0.967

‘be’

1.175

‘to’

0.980

‘to’

2.209

‘him’

0.845

‘we’

0.992

‘we’

0.896

‘my’

1.399

‘is’

0.603

‘my’

0.717

‘i’

0.604

‘was’

1.183

‘who’

0.480

‘our’

0.696

‘or’

0.437

‘de’

1.110

‘that’

0.429

‘it’

0.625

‘are’

0.356

‘by’

1.061

‘it’

0.427

‘all’

0.477

‘our’

0.340

‘him’

0.879

1925–1949

1950–1974

1975–1999

2000–2008

‘i’

4.086

‘the’

8.128

‘of’

20.340

‘of’

5.339

‘it’

2.273

‘of’

4.386

‘the’

11.844

‘the’

3.920

‘the’

2.221

‘he’

3.395

‘in’

5.730

‘is’

2.049

‘he’

2.073

‘it’

3.151

‘by’

3.384

‘in’

1.440

‘of’

2.056

‘i’

2.366

‘be’

3.389

‘by’

1.352

‘and’

1.685

‘his’

1.652

‘is’

3.044

‘be’

0.886

‘to’

1.383

‘be’

1.154

‘may’

0.829

‘or’

0.568

‘at’

1.328

‘so’

1.143

‘no’

0.638

‘a’

0.450

‘was’

1.138

‘by’

1.132

‘not’

0.608

‘may’

0.414

‘her’

1.106

‘was’

1.099

‘has’

0.576

‘are’

0.300

Table 2

Function words in American English with increasing frequency

1825–1849

1850–1874

1875–1899

1900–1924

‘her’

–0.430

‘up’

–0.383

‘everything’

0.415

‘per’

–0.569

‘p’

–0.439

‘are’

–0.4204

‘was’

–0.511

‘be’

–0.640

‘in’

–0.644

‘v’

–0.503

‘her’

–0.571

‘on’

–0.666

‘its’

–0.816

‘was’

–0.675

‘she’

–0.664

‘of’

–0.770

‘we’

–0.921

‘the’

–0.877

‘for’

–0.701

‘or’

–0.864

‘on’

–1.038

‘at’

–1.002

‘he’

–0.790

‘are’

–0.974

‘and’

–1.381

‘in’

–1.105

‘you’

–0.949

‘for’

–1.331

‘of’

–2.774

‘is’

–1.519

‘de’

–1.010

‘a’

–1.755

‘the’

–3.040

‘i’

–1.763

‘in’

–1.412

‘in’

–2.260

‘a’

–3.886

‘a’

–1.768

‘a’

–2.791

‘is’

–2.705

1925–1949

1950–1974

1975–1999

2000–2008

‘n’

–0.443

‘pp’

–0.267

‘up’

–0.970

‘me’

–0.570

‘l’

–0.480

‘c’

–0.280

‘can’

–0.995

‘we’

–0.589

‘c’

–0.500

‘p’

–0.296

‘me’

–1.016

‘he’

–0.670

‘b’

–0.540

‘l’

–0.302

‘my’

–1.530

‘my’

–0.692

‘v’

–0.561

‘on’

–0.367

‘her’

–1.906

‘she’

–0.895

‘p’

–0.588

‘al’

–0.410

‘she’

–1.927

‘her’

–0.951

‘in’

–0.640

‘j’

–0.509

‘to’

–1.983

‘and’

–0.953

‘for’

–0.655

‘in’

–0.605

‘a’

–2.561

‘you’

–1.485

‘f’

–0.770

‘an’

–0.673

‘you’

–4.072

‘to’

–1.789

‘m’

–0.777

‘b’

–0.815

‘i’

–6.011

‘i’

–3.537

Table 3

Content words in American English with increasing frequency

1825–1849

1850–1874

1875–1899

‘one’

–0.527

‘mr’

–0.517

‘conditions’

0.494

‘beautiful’

0.359

‘president’

0.486

‘monsieur’

0.482

‘intellectual’

0.356

‘general’

0.329

‘american’

0.411

‘inhabitants’

0.287

‘massachusetts’

0.295

‘university’

0.380

‘connection’

0.285

‘conditions’

0.253

‘government’

0.358

‘influence’

0.271

‘development’

0.252

‘literature’

0.321

‘neighborhood’

0.261

‘railroad’

0.224

‘mademoiselle’

0.318

‘constantinople’

0.255

‘signified’

0.223

‘development’

0.305

‘spiritual’

0.251

‘position’

0.222

‘political’

0.258

‘development’

0.243

‘especially’

0.213

‘temperature’

0.258

1900–1924

1925–1949

1950–1974

‘organization’

0.842

‘international’

0.526

‘development’

1.390

‘conditions’

0.701

‘production’

0.485

‘information’

1.177

‘department’

0.633

‘administration’

0.435

‘university’

1.013

‘individual’

0.577

‘research’

0.435

‘relationship’

0.845

‘school’

0.567

‘government’

0.431

‘behavior’

0.759

‘development’

0.562

‘temperature’

0.428

‘patients’

0.721

‘production’

0.550

‘individual’

0.408

‘environmental’

0.698

‘business’

0.540

‘development’

0.403

‘children’

0.683

‘american’

0.537

‘relationship’

0.397

‘research’

0.671

‘education’

0.531

‘problems’

0.391

‘significant’

0.649

1975–1999

2000–2008

‘information’

1.223

‘international’

0.581

‘management’

0.617

‘students’

0.502

‘understanding’

0.418

‘learning’

0.416

‘technology’

0.409

‘research’

0.410

‘internet’

0.378

‘university’

0.265

‘assessment’

0.376

‘political’

0.265

‘including’

0.351

‘understanding’

0.261

‘performance’

0.335

‘chapter’

0.260

‘perspective’

0.334

‘knowledge’

0.239

‘understand’

0.310

‘organizations’

0.219

Table 4

Content words in American English with decreasing frequency

1825–1849

1850–1874

1875–1899

‘mentioned’

–0.328

‘country’

–0.264

‘heaven’

–0.221

‘concerning’

–0.328

‘particular’

–0.269

‘inhabitants’

–0.225

‘celebrated’

–0.330

‘happiness’

–0.275

‘circumstances’

–0.244

‘immediately’

–0.334

‘knowledge’

–0.321

‘received’

–0.244

‘necessary’

–0.340

‘christian’

–0.347

‘signifies’

–0.258

‘distinguished’

–0.374

‘character’

–0.349

‘general’

–0.275

‘considered’

–0.389

‘happiness’

–0.275

‘signified’

–0.283

‘particularly’

–0.413

‘circumstances’

–0.426

‘spiritual’

–0.295

‘scriptures’

–0.449

‘god’

0.580

‘according’

–0.298

‘god’

0.669

‘inhabitants’

–0.835

‘immediately’

–0.312

1900–1924

1925–1949

1950–1974

‘replied’

–0.215

‘hundred’

–0.195

‘satisfactory’

–0.225

‘hundred’

–0.216

‘interesting’

–0.208

‘character’

–0.232

‘constitution’

–0.236

‘character’

–0.219

‘company’

–0.233

‘england’

–0.243

‘country’

–0.223

‘corporation’

–0.262

‘circumstances’

–0.250

‘temperance’

–0.234

‘consideration’

–0.273

‘character’

–0.257

‘beautiful’

–0.235

‘corresponding’

–0.278

‘christian’

–0.257

‘intelligence’

–0.255

‘advertising’

–0.289

‘mademoiselle’

–0.340

‘practically’

–0.258

‘business’

–0.297

‘thousand’

–0.364

‘business’

–0.268

‘constitution’

–0.325

‘monsieur’

–0.479

‘little’

–0.345

‘temperature’

–0.374

1975–1999

2000–2008

‘considerable’

–0.387

‘elizabeth’

–0.118

‘distribution’

–0.391

‘family’

–0.126

‘individual’

–0.398

‘treatment’

–0.130

‘behavior’

–0.430

‘problems’

–0.132

‘population’

–0.431

‘william’

–0.157

‘general’

–0.454

‘married’

–0.165

‘development’

–0.487

‘william’

–0.157

‘administration’

–0.490

‘temperature’

–0.197

‘education’

–0.543

‘said’

–0.233

‘government’

–0.630

‘children’

–0.262

Понравилась статья? Поделить с друзьями:
  • What is the contraction word for it has
  • What is the average function in excel
  • What is the content type for excel
  • What is the archaic word
  • What is the computer program excel