What is spoken word performance

Here is a colelction of information about the literature of spoken word, from oral traditions to slam poetry, and beyond. Know something that should be here? Please share it in the comments.

What is Spoken Word?

Spoken word is a form of literary art or artistic performance in which lyrics, poetry, or stories are spoken rather than sung. Spoken-word is often done with a musical background, but emphasis is kept on the speaker. One of the most common sorts of spoken word performances is performance poetry, where a poet either reads previously-published poems, or reads poems specifically written to be performed aloud. Another kind that has gained popularity in recent years is political and social commentary, done in such a way that it is, while still prose, somewhat more artistic than a typical speech. Spoken word artists are often poets and musicians. Spoken word gained notoriety in the late 1980s and early 1990s through the emergence of “poetry slams,” where spoken word artists would square off in cabaret-style duels. wikipedia

Three Basic Types of Spoken Word Performances

Lets not get bogged down with taxonomy, but I think it would be fair to acknowledge some various “types” of spoken word poetry. Musicians like to have genres to describe what they do, and to distinguish themselves from others. Spoken Word performers feel similarly. Here are some. There are others, like stand-up comedy, storytelling, and so on, but these are some poetic examples.

Recorded Poetry

What I call “recorded poetry”
is recorded works by the so-called “major poets”, or recordings by poets who are primarily poets-in-print. For better or worse, this kind of spoken word usually sounds to an audience like being read to, because that’s what it is.

“Spoken Word”

Contemporary Spoken Word, generally defined
, is the term given to this visceral, in-your-face style of contemporary poetry of the nineties was spoken word.

Hip-Hop

Hip-hop is unique among the types of spoken word, for its use of rhythm, abundant rhyming, and for its very inventive use of words: made-up words, recycled words, slang words, etc. Some people, notably one random teenager, seem to think that there is
a difference between “hip-hop” and “rap”, the crux of the distinction being the literary merits of the former over the latter
. What are the literary merits of hip-hop? Read an essay entitled
reverse-gentrification of the literary world,
which is the preface of a book byMiles Marshall Lewis.

The term linguistic performance was used by Noam Chomsky in 1960 to describe «the actual use of language in concrete situations».[1] It is used to describe both the production, sometimes called parole, as well as the comprehension of language.[2] Performance is defined in opposition to «competence»; the latter describes the mental knowledge that a speaker or listener has of language.[3]

Part of the motivation for the distinction between performance and competence comes from speech errors: despite having a perfect understanding of the correct forms, a speaker of a language may unintentionally produce incorrect forms. This is because performance occurs in real situations, and so is subject to many non-linguistic influences. For example, distractions or memory limitations can affect lexical retrieval (Chomsky 1965:3), and give rise to errors in both production and perception.[4] Such non-linguistic factors are completely independent of the actual knowledge of language,[5] and establish that speakers’ knowledge of language (their competence) is distinct from their actual use of language (their performance).[6]

BackgroundEdit

Descriptor

Proponent

Explication

Langue/Parole Ferdinand de Saussure (1916)[7] Language is a system of signs. Langue describes the social consensus of how signs are applied. Parole describes the physical manifestation of langue. Emphasizes revealing the structure of langue through the study of parole.
Competence/Performance Noam Chomsky (1965)[8] Introduced in generative grammar theory, competence describes the unconscious and innate knowledge of linguistic rules. Performance describes the observable use of language. Emphasizes the study of competence over performance.
I-Language/E-Language Noam Chomsky (1986)[9] Similar to the performance/competence distinction, I-Language is the internalized innate knowledge of language; E-Language is the externalized observable output. Emphasizes the study of I-Language over E-Language.

Langue versus paroleEdit

Published in 1916, Ferdinand de Saussure’s Course in General Linguistics describes language as «a system of signs that express ideas».[7] de Saussure describes two components of language: langue and parole. Langue consists of the structural relations that define a language, which includes grammar, syntax and phonology. Parole is the physical manifestation of signs; in particular the concrete manifestation of langue as speech or writing. While langue can be viewed strictly as a system of rules, it is not an absolute system such that parole must utterly conform to langue.[10] Drawing an analogy to chess, de Saussure compares langue to the rules of chess that define how the game should be played, and parole to the individual choices of a player given the possible moves allowed within the system of rules.[7]

Competence versus performanceEdit

Proposed in the 1950s by Noam Chomsky, generative grammar is an analysis approach to language as a structural framework of the human mind.[11] Through formal analysis of components such as syntax, morphology, semantics and phonology, a generative grammar seeks to model the implicit linguistic knowledge with which speakers determine grammaticality.

In transformational generative grammar theory, Chomsky distinguishes between two components of language production: competence and performance.[5] Competence describes the mental knowledge of a language, the speaker’s intrinsic understanding of sound-meaning relations as established by linguistic rules. Performance – that is the actual observed use of language – involves more factors than phonetic-semantic understanding. Performance requires extra-linguistic knowledge such as an awareness of the speaker, audience and the context, which crucially determines how speech is constructed and analyzed. It is also governed by principles of cognitive structures not considered aspects of language, such as memory, distractions, attention, and speech errors.

I-Language versus E-LanguageEdit

In 1986, Chomsky proposed a distinction similar to the competence/performance distinction, entertaining the notion of an I-Language (internal language) which is the intrinsic linguistic knowledge within a native speaker and E-Language (external language) which is the observable linguistic output of a speaker. It was I-Language that Chomsky argued should be the focus of inquiry, and not E-Language.[9]

E-language has been used to describe the application of artificial systems, such as in calculus, set theory and with natural language viewed as sets, while performance has been used purely to describe applications of natural language.[12] Between I-Language and competence, I-Language refers to our intrinsic faculty for language, competence is used by Chomsky as an informal, general term, or as term with reference to a specific competency such as «grammatical competence» or «pragmatic competence».[12]

Performance-grammar correspondence hypothesisEdit

John A. Hawkins’s Performance-Grammar Correspondence Hypothesis (PGCH) states that the syntactic structures of grammars are conventionalized based on whether and how much the structures are preferred in performance.[13] Performance preference is related to structure complexity and processing, or comprehension, efficiency. Specifically, a complex structure refers to a structure containing more linguistic elements or words at the end of the structure than at the beginning. It is this structural complexity that results in decreased processing efficiency since more structure requires additional processing.[13] This model seeks to explain word order across languages based on avoidance of unnecessary complexity in favour of increased processing efficiency. Speakers make an automatic calculation of the Immediate Constituent(IC)-to-word order ratio and produce the structure with the highest ratio.[13] Structures with a high IC-to-word order are structures that contain the fewest words required for the listener to parse the structure into constituents which results in more efficient processing.[13]

Head-initial structuresEdit

In head-initial structures, which includes example SVO and VSO word order, the speaker’s goal is to order the sentence constituents from least to most complex.

SVO word orderEdit

SVO word order can be exemplified with English; consider the example sentences in (1). In (1a) three immediate constituents (ICs) are present in the verb phrase, namely VP, PP1 and PP2, and there are four words (went, to, London, in) required to parse the VP into its constituents. Therefore, the IC-to-word ratio is 3/4=75%. In contrast, in (1b) the VP is still composed of three ICs but there are now six words that are required to determine the constituent structure of the VP (went, in, the, late, afternoon, to). Thus, the ratio for (1b) is 3/6 = 50%. Hawkins proposes that speakers prefer to produce (1a) since it has a higher IC-to-word ratio and this leads to faster and more efficient processing.[13]

  1a.  John [VP went [PP1 to London] [[PP2 in the late afternoon]]
  1b.  John [VP went [PP2 in the late afternoon]] [[PP1 to London]]

Hawkins supports the above analysis by providing performance data to demonstrate the preference speakers have for ordering short phrases before long phrases when producing head-initial structures. The table based on English data, below, illustrates that the short prepositional phrase (PP1) is preferentially ordered before the long PP (PP2) and that this preference increases as the size differential between the two PPs increases. For example, 60% of the sentences are ordered short (PP1) to long (PP2) when PP2 was longer than PP1 by 1 word. In contrast, 99% of the sentences are ordered short to long when PP2 is longer than PP1 by 7+ words.

English prepositional phrase orderings by relative weight[13]

n = 323 PP2 > PP1 by 1 word by 2-4 by 5-6 by 7+
[V PP1 PP2] 60% (58) 86% (108) 94% (31) 99% (68)
[V PP2 PP1] 40% (38) 14% (17) 6% (2) 1% (1)

PP2 = longer PP; PP1=shorter PP. Proportion of short-long to long-short as
a percentage; actual numbers of sequences in parentheses. An additional
71 sequences had PPs of equal length (total n=394)

VSO word orderEdit

Hawkins argues that the preference for short followed by long phrases applies to all languages that have head-initial structuring. This includes languages with VSO word order such as from Hungarian. By calculating the IC-to-word ratio for the Hungarian sentences in the same way as was done for the English sentences, 2a. emerges as having a higher ratio than 2b.[13]

 2a.  VP[Döngetik NP[facipöink NP[az utcakat] ]
        batter   wooden shoes-1PL  the streets-ACC
        Our wooden shoes batter the streets
 2b.  VP[Döngetik NP[az utcakat] NP[[ facipöink ] ]

The Hungarian performance data (below) show the same preference pattern as the English data. This study looked at the ordering of two successive noun phrases (NPs) and found that the shorter NP followed by the longer NP is preferred in performance, and that this preference increases as the size differential between NP1 and NP2 increases.

Hungarian noun phrase orderings by relative weight[13]

n = 85 mNP2 > mNP1 by 1 word by 2 by 3+
[V mNP1 mNP2] 85% (50) 96% (27) 100% (8)
[V mNP2 mNP1] 15% (9) 4% (1) 0% (0)

mNP = any NP constructed on its left periphery.
NP2 = longer NP; NP1 = shorter NP. Proportion of short-long
to long-short given as a percentage; actual numbers of sequences
given in parentheses. An additional 21 sequences had NPs of equal length
(total n = 16).

Head-final structuresEdit

Hawkins’ explanation of performance and word order extends to head-final structures. For example, since Japanese is a SOV language the head (V) is at the end of the sentence. This theory predicts that speakers will prefer to order the phrases in head-final sentences from long phrases to short, as opposed to short to long as seen in head-initial languages.[13] This reversal of ordering preference is due to the fact that in head-final sentences it is the long followed by short phrasal ordering that has the higher IC-to-word ratio.

  3a. Tanaka ga vp[pp[Hanako kara]np[sono hon o] katta]
      Tanaka NOM  Hanako from that book ACC bought
       Tanako bought that book from Hanako 
  3b. Tanaka ga vp[np[sono hon o] pp[Hanako kara] [katta]

The VP and its constituents in 4. are constructed from their heads on the right. This means that the number of words used to calculate the ratio is counted from the head of the first phrase (PP in 3a. and NP in 3b.) to the verb (as indicated in bold above). The IC-to-word ratio for the VP in 3a. is 3/5=60% while the ratio for the VP in 3b. is 3/4=75%. Therefore, 3b. should be preferred by Japanese speakers since it has a higher IC-to-word ratio which leads to faster parsing of sentences by the listener.[13]

The performance preference for long to short phrase ordering in SVO languages is supported by performance data. The table below shows that production of long to short phrases is preferred and that this preference increases as the size of the differential between the two phrases increases. For example, ordering of the longer 2ICm (where ICm is either a direct object NP with an accusative case particle or a PP constructed from the right periphery) before the shorter 1ICm is more frequent, and the frequency increases to 91% if the 2ICm is longer than the 1ICm by 9+ words.

Japanese NPo and PPm orderings by relative weight[13]

n = 153 2ICm > 1ICm by 1-2 words by 3-4 by 5-8 by 9+
[2ICm 1ICm V] 66% (59) 72% (21) 83% (20) 91% (10)
[1ICm 2ICm V] 34% (30) 28% (8) 17% (4) 9% (1)

Npo = direct object NP with accusative case particle. PPm = PP constructed on its right periphery by a P(ostposition). ICm= either NPo or PPm. 2IC=longer IC; 1IC = shorter IC. Proportion of long-to short to short-long orders given as a percentage; actual numbers of sequences in parentheses. an additional 91 sequences had ICs of equal length (total n=244)

Utterance planning hypothesisEdit

Tom Wasow proposes that word order arises as a result of utterance planning benefiting the speaker.[14] He introduces the concepts of early versus late commitment, where commitment is the point in the utterance where it becomes possible to predict subsequent structure.[14] Specifically, early commitment refers to the commitment point present earlier in the utterance and late commitment refers to the commitment point present later in the utterance.[14] He explains that early commitment will favour the listener since early prediction of subsequent structure enables faster processing. Comparatively, late commitment will favour the speaker by postponing decision making, giving the speaker more time to plan the utterance.[14] Wasow illustrates how utterance planning influences syntactic word order by testing early versus late commitment in heavy-NP shifted (HNPS) sentences. The idea is to examine the patterns of HNPS to determine if the performance data show sentences that are structured to favour the speaker or the listener.[14]

Examples of early/late commitment and heavy-NP shiftEdit

The following examples illustrate what is meant by early versus late commitment and how heavy-NP shift applies to these sentences. Wasow looked at two types of verbs:[14]

Vt (transitive verbs): require NP objects.

    4a. Pat VP[brought NP[a box with a ribbon around it] PP[ [to the party] ] 
    4b. Pat VP[brought PP[to the party] NP[ [a box with a ribbon around it] ] 

In 4a. no heavy-NP shift has been applied. The NP is available early but does not provide any additional information about the sentence structure – the «to» appearing late in the sentence is an example of late commitment. In contrast, in 4b., where heavy-NP shift has shifted the NP to the right, as soon as «to» is uttered the listener knows that the VP must contain the NP and a PP. In other words, when «to» is uttered it allows the listener to predict the remaining structure of the sentence early on. Thus for transitive verbs HNPS results in early commitment and favors the listener.

Vp (prepositional verbs): can take an NP object or an immediately following PP with no NP object

    5a. Pat VP[wrote NP[something about Chris] PP[ [on the blackboard]].
    5b. Pat VP[wrote PP[on the blackboard] NP[ [something about Chris.]] 

No HNPS has been applied to 5a. In 5b. the listener needs to hear the word «something» in order to know that the utterance contains a PP and an NP since the object NP is optional but «something» has been shifted to later in the sentence. Thus for prepositional verbs HNPS results in late commitment and favours the speaker.

Predictions and findingsEdit

Based on the above information Wasow predicted that if sentences are constructed from the speaker’s perspective then heavy-NP shift would rarely apply to sentences containing a transitive verb but would apply frequently to sentences containing a prepositional verb. The opposite prediction was made if sentences are constructed from the listener’s perspective.[14]

Speaker’s Perspective Listener’s Perspective
Vt Heavy-NP shift= rare Heavy-NP shift= relatively common
Vp Heavy-NP shift= relatively common Heavy-NP shift =very rare

To test his predictions Wasow analyzed performance data (from corpora data) for the rates of occurrence of HNPS for Vt and Vp and found HNPS occurred twice as frequently in Vp than in Vt, therefore supporting the predictions made from the speaker’s perspective.[14] In contrast, he did not find evidence in support of the predictions made based on the listener’s perspective. In other words, given the data above, when HNPS is applied to sentences containing a transitive verb the result favors the listener. Wasow found that HNPS applied to transitive verb sentences is rare in performance data thus supporting the speaker’s perspective. Additionally, when HNPS is applied to prepositional verb structures the result favors the speaker. In his study of the performance data, Wasow found evidence of HNPS frequently applied to prepositional verb structures further supporting the speaker’s perspective.[14] Based on these findings Wasow concludes that HNPS is correlated with the speaker’s preference for late commitment thereby demonstrating how speaker performance preference can influence word order.

Alternative grammar modelsEdit

While the dominant views of grammar are largely oriented towards competence, many, including Chomsky himself, have argued that a complete model of grammar should be able to account for performance data. But while Chomsky argues that competence should be studied first, thereby allowing further study of performance,[6] some systems, such as constraint grammars are built with performance as a starting point (comprehension, in the case of constraint grammars[15] While traditional models of generative grammar have had a great deal of success in describing the structure of languages, they have been less successful in describing how language is interpreted in real situations. For example, traditional grammar describes a sentence as having an «underlying structure» which is different from the «surface structure» which speakers actually produce. In a real conversation, however, a listener interprets the meaning of a sentence in real time, as the surface structure goes by.[16] This kind of on-line processing, which accounts for phenomena such as finishing another person’s sentence, and starting a sentence without knowing how it is going to finish, is not directly accounted for in traditional generative models of grammar.[16] Several alternative grammar models exist which may be better able to capture this surface-based aspect of linguistic performance, including
Constraint Grammar, Lexical Functional Grammar, and Head-driven phrase structure grammar.

Errors in linguistic performanceEdit

Errors in linguistic performance not only occur in children newly acquiring their native language, second language learners, those with a disability or an acquired brain injury but among competent speakers as well. Types of performance errors that will be of focus here are those that involve errors in syntax, other types of errors can occur in the phonological, semantic features of words, for further information see speech errors. Phonological and semantic errors can be due to the repetition of words, mispronunciations, limitations in verbal working memory, and length of the utterance.[17] Slips of the tongue are most common in spoken languages and occur when the speaker either: says something they did not mean to; produces the incorrect order of sounds or words; or uses the incorrect word.[18] Other instances of errors in linguistic performance are slips of the hand in signed languages, slips of the ear which are errors in comprehension of utterances and slips of the pen which occur while writing. Errors of linguistic performance are perceived by both the speaker and the listener and can therefore have many interpretations depending on the persons judgement and the context in which the sentence was spoken.[19]

It is proposed that there is a close relation between the linguistic units of grammar and the psychological units of speech which implies that there is a relation between linguistic rules and the psychological processes that create utterances.[20] Errors in performance can occur at any level of these psychological processes. Lise Menn proposes that there are five levels of processing in speech production, each with its own possible error that could occur.[18] According to the proposed speech processing structure by Menn an error in the syntactic properties of an utterance occurs at the positional level.

  1. Message Level
  2. Functional Level
  3. Positional Level
  4. Phonological Encoding
  5. Speech Gesture

Another proposal for the levels of speech processing is made by Willem J. M. Levelt to be structured as so:[21]

  1. Conceptualization
  2. Formulation
  3. Articulation
  4. Self-Monitoring

Levelt (1993) states that we as speakers are unaware of most of these levels of performance such as articulation, which includes the movement and placement of the articulators, the formulation of the utterance which includes the words selected and their pronunciation and the rules which must be followed for the utterance to be grammatical. The levels speakers are consciously aware is the intent of the message which occurs at the level of conceptualization and then again at self-monitoring which is when the speaker would become aware of any errors that may have occurred and correct themselves.[21]

Slips of the tongueEdit

One type of slip of the tongue which cause an error in the syntax of the utterance are called transformational errors. Transformational errors are a mental operation proposed by Chomsky in his Transformational Hypothesis and it has three parts which errors in performance can occur. These transformations are applied at the level of the underlying structures and predict the ways in which an error can occur.[20]

  • Structural analysis
  • Structural Change
  • Conditions

Structural Analysis
errors can occur due to the application of (a) the rule misanalyzing the tense marker causing the rule to apply incorrectly, (b) the rule not being applied when it should or (c) a rule being applied when it should not.

This example from Fromkin (1980) demonstrates a rule misanalyzing the tense marker and for subject-auxiliary inversion to be incorrectly applied. The subject-auxiliary inversion is misanalyzed as to which structure it applies, applying without the verb be in the tense as it moves to the C position. This causes «do-support» to occur and the verb to lack tense causing the syntactic error.

    6a. Error: Why do you be an oaf sometimes?
    6b. Target: Why are you an oaf sometimes?

Phrase tree structure of error «Why do you be an oaf sometimes»

Phrase tree structure of target «Why are you an oaf sometimes»

Transformation in Error Error Transformation in Target Target
Underlying Structure [CP[C+q][TP[T’[T PRES][VP[DP you][V’[Vbe][[DP an oaf]][AdvP sometimes][DP why] Underlying Structure [CP[C'[C+q][TP[T'[T PRES][VP[DP you][V'[V be][DP[D an][[NPoaf]] [AdvPsometimes][DP why]
Wh-Movement [CP[DP Why][C'[C+q][TP[T'[T pres][VP[DP you][V'[V be][AP[AP[A'[A an][DP[oaf]]]][AdvP[Adv'[Adv sometimes][DP e] Wh-Movement [CP[DP Why][C'[C+q][TP[T'[T pres][VP[DP you][V'[V be][AP[AP[A'[A an][DP[oaf]]]][AdvP[Adv'[Adv sometimes][DP e]
Subject-Auxiliary Inversion [CP[DP Why][C'[C[T Pres][ [Cq e]][TP[T'[T e][VP[DP you][V'[V be][AP[AP[A'[A an][DP[oaf]]]][AdvP[Adv'[Adv sometimes][DP e] DP Movement [CP[DP Why][C'[C+q][TP[DP you][T'[T PRES][VP[V'[V be][AP[AP[A'[A an][DP[oaf]]]][AdvP[Adv'[Adv sometimes][DP e]
Do-Support [CP[DP Why][C'[C[T[V do][ [T PRES]][ [Cq e]][TP[T'[T e][VP[DP you][V'[V be][AP[AP[A'[A an][DP[oaf]]]][AdvP[Adv'[Adv sometimes][DP e] Subject-Auxiliary Inversion [CP[DP Why][C'[C[T[V be][ [T PRES]]Cq][TP[DP you][T'[T[VP[V'[AP[AP[A'[A an][DP[oaf]]]][AdvP[Adv'[Adv sometimes][DP e]
Morphophonemic Why do you be an oaf sometimes? Morphophonemic Why are you an oaf sometimes?

The following example from Fromkin (1980) demonstrates how a rule is being applied when it should not. The subject-auxiliary inversion rule is omitted in the error utterance, causing affix-hopping to occur and putting the tense onto the verb «say» creating the syntactic error. In the target the subject-auxiliary rule and then do-support applies creating the grammatically correct structure.

    7a. Error: And what he said?
    7b. Target: And what did he say?

Phrase tree structure for error «And what he said»

Phrase tree structure for target «And what did he say?»

Transformation in Error Error Transformation in Target Target
Underlying Structure [CP[CONJ And][CP[C'[C +q][TP[T'[T PAST][VP[DP he][V'[V say][DP what] Underlying Structure [CP[CONJ And][CP[C'[C +q][TP[T'[T PAST][VP[DP he][V'[V say][DP what]
Wh-Movement [CP[CONJ And][CP[DP what][C'[C +q][TP[T'[T PAST][VP[DP he][V'[V say][DP e] DP & Wh-Movement [CP[CONJ And][CP[DP what][C'[C +q][TP[DP he][T'[T PAST][VP[V'[V say]
Affix Hopping [CP[CONJ And][CP[DP what][C'[C +q][TP[T'[T e][VP[DP he][V'[V say+PAST][DP e] Subject-Auxiliary Inversion + Do Support [CP[CONJ And][CP[DP what][C'[C[T[V do][ [T PAST]][ [Cq]][TP[DP he][T'[T e][VP[DP e][V'[V say][DPe]
Morphophonemic And what he said? Morphophonemic And what did he say?

This example from Fromkin (1980) shows how a rule is being applied when it should not. The subject-auxiliary inversion and do-support has applied to an idiomatic expression causing the insertion of «do» when it should not be applied in the ungrammatical utterance.

    8a. Error: How do we go!!
    8b. Target: How we go!!

Phrase tree structure for error «how do we go?»

Phrase tree structure for target «how we go!»

Transformation in Error Error Transformation in Target Target
Underlying Structure [CP[C'[C +q][TP[T'[T PRES][VP[DP we][V'[V go][DP how] Underlying Structure [CP[C'[C +q][TP[T'[T PRES][VP[DP we][V'[V go][DP how]
Wh-Movement [CP[DP how][C'[C +q][TP[T'[T PRES][VP[DP we][V'[V go][DP e] Wh-Movement [CP[DP how][C'[C +q][TP[T'[T PRES][VP[DP we][V'[V go][DP e]
DP Movement [CP[DP how][C'[C +q][TP[DP we][T'[T PRES][VP[DP e][V'[V go][DP e] DP Movement [CP[DP how][C'[C +q][TP[DP we][T'[T PRES][VP[DP e][V'[V go][DP e]
Subject-Auxiliary Inversion + Do-Support [CP[DP How][C'[C[T[V do][ [T PRES]][ [Cq]][TP[DP we][T'[T e][VP[DP e][V'[V go][DP e] [CP[DP how][C'[C +q][TP[DP we][T'[T PRES][VP[DP e][V'[V go][DP e]
Morphophonemic How do we go! Morphophonemic How we go!

Structural Change
Errors can occur in the carrying out of rules, even though the analysis of the phrase marker is done correctly. This can occur when the analysis requires multiple rules to occur.

The following example from Fromkin (1980) shows the relative clause rule copies the determiner phrase «a boy» within the clause and this causes front attaching to the Wh-marker. Deletion is then skipped, leaving the determiner phrase in the clause in the error utterance causing it to be ungrammatical.

    9a. Error: A boy who I know a boy has hair down to here.
    9b. Target: A boy who I know has hair down to here.

Phrase tree structure for error «A boy who I know a boy who has hair down to here»

Phrase tree structure for target «A boy who I know has hair down to here.»

Transformation in Error Error Transformation in Target Target
Underlying Structure [TP[T'[Te][CP[C'[C +q][TP[T'[T e][VP[VP[DP I][V'[V know][DP a boy]]][VP[DP who][V'[V has][AdvP hair down to here] Underlying Structure [TP[T'[Te][CP[C'[C +q][TP[T'[T e][VP[VP[DP I][V'[V know][DP a boy]]][VP[DP who][V'[V has][AdvP hair down to here]
Wh-Movement [TP[T'[Te][CP[DP who][C'[C +q][TP[T'[T e][VP[VP[DP I][V'[V know][DP a boy]]][VP[DP who][V'[V has][AdvP hair down to here] Wh-Movement [TP[T'[Te][CP[DP who][C'[C +q][TP[T'[T e][VP[VP[DP I][V'[V know][DP a boy]]][VP[DP e][V'[V has][AdvP hair down to here]
DP-Movement [TP[DP a boy]][T'[Te][CP[DP who][C'[C +q][TP[DP I][T'[T e][VP[VP[DP e][V'[V know][DP a boy]]][VP[DP e][V'[V has][AdvP hair down to here] DP-Movement [TP[DP a boy]][T'[Te][CP[DP who][C'[C +q][TP[DP I][T'[T e][VP[VP[DP e][V'[V know][DP e]]][VP[DP e][V'[V has][AdvP hair down to here]
Morphophonemic A boy who I know a boy has hair down to here Morphophonemic A boy who I know has hair down to here

Conditions errors restrict when the rule can and cannot be applied.

This last example from Fromkin (1980) shows that a rule was applied under a certain condition in which it is restricted. The subject-auxiliary inversion rule cannot apply to embedded clauses. In the case of this example it has causing for the syntactic error.

    10a. Error: I know where is a top for it. 
    10b. Target: I know where a top for it is. 

Phrase tree structure for error «I know where is a top for it»

Phrase tree structure for target «I know where a top for it is»

Transformations in Error Error Transformation in Target Target
Underlying Structure [TP[T'[T e][VP[DP I][V'[V know][DP where][CP[C'[C e][TP[T'[T PRES][VP[DP a top][V'[PP for it][V be] Underlying Structure [TP[T'[T e][VP[DP I][V'[V know][DP where][CP[C'[C e][TP[T'[T PRES][VP[DP a top][V'[PP for it][V be]
DP Movement [TP[DP I][T'[T e][VP[DP e][V'[V know][DP where][CP[C'[C e][TP[DP a top][T'[T PRES][VP[DP e][V'[PP for it][V be] DP Movement [TP[DP I][T'[T e][VP[DP e][V'[V know][DP where][CP[C'[C e][TP[DP a top][T'[T PRES][VP[DP e][V'[PP for it][V be]
Subject-Auxiliary Inversion [TP[DP I][T'[T e][VP[DP e][V'[V know][DP where][CP[C'[C[T[V be][ [T PRES]][ [C e]][TP[DP a top][T'[T e][VP[DP e][V'[PP for it][V e] Affix Hopping TP[DP I][T'[T e][VP[DP e][V'[V know][DP where][CP[C'[C e][TP[DP a top][T'[T e][VP[DP e][V'[PP for it][V be+PRES]
Morphophonemic I know where is a top for it Morphophonemic I know where a top for it is

A study of deaf Italians found that the second person singular of indicatives would extend to corresponding forms in imperatives and negative imperatives.[22]

Error Target
«pensi» «pensa»
think-2nd PERS-SG-PRES-IND think-2nd PERS-SG-IMP
«(you) think» «do think»
Error Target
«non fa» «non fare»
not do-2nd PERS-SG-IMP do-inf
«not do» «do not do»

The following is an example taken from Dutch data in which there is verb omission in the embedded clause of the utterance (which is not allowed in Dutch), resulting in a performance error.[22]

Error Target
«dit is de jongen die de tomaat snijdt en dit is de jongen die het brood» «deze jongen snijdt de tomaat en deze jongen het brood»
«this is the boy that cuts the tomato and this is the boy that the bread» «this boy cuts the tomato and this boy the bread»

A study done with Zulu speaking children with a language delay displayed errors in linguistic performance of lacking proper passive verb morphology.[22]

Error Target
«Ulumile ihnashi» «Ulunywe yihnashi»
«U-lum-ile i-hnashi U-luny-w-e y-i-hnashi
sm1-bite-PAST NC5-horse sm1-bite-PASS-PAST COP-NC5-horse
«He bit, the horse did.» «He was bitten by the horse.»
Error Target
«Ulumile ifish» «Ulunywe yifish»
sm1-bite-PAST NC5-fish sm1-bite-PASS-PAST COP-NC5-fish
«He bit, the fish did.» «He was bitten by the fish.»

Slips of the handEdit

The linguistic components of American Sign Language (ASL) can be broken down into four parts; the hand configuration, place of articulation, movement and other minor parameters. Hand configuration is determined by the shape of the hand, fingers and thumbs and is specific to the sign that is being used. It allows the signer to articulate what they are wanting to communicate by extending, flexing, bending or spreading the digits; the position of the thumb to the fingers; or the curvature of the hand. However, there are not an infinite amount of possible hand configurations, there are 19 classes of hand configuration primes as listed by the Dictionary of American Sign Language. Place of articulation is the particular location that the sign is being performed known as the «signing place». The «signing place» can be the whole face or a particular part of it, the eyes, nose, cheek, ear, neck, trunk, any part of the arm, or the neutral area in front of the signers head and body. Movement is the most complex as it can be difficult to analyze. Movement is restricted to directional, rotations of the wrist, local movements of the hand and interactions of the hands. These movements can occur singularly, in sequence, or simultaneously. Minor parameters in ASL include contacting region, orientation and hand arrangement. They are subclasses of hand configuration.

Performance errors resulting in ungrammatical signs can result due to processes that change the hand configuration, place, movement or other parameter of the sign. These processes can be anticipation, preservation, or metathesis. Anticipation is caused when some characteristic of the next sign is incorporated into the sign that is presently being performed. Preservation is the opposite of anticipation where some characteristic of the preceding sign is carried over into the performance of the next sign. Metathesis occurs when two characteristics of adjacent signs are combined into one in the performance of both signs.[20] Each of these errors will result in an incorrect sign being performed. This could result in either a different sign being performed instead of the intended one, or nonexistent signs which forms are possible and those which forms are not possible due to the structural rules.[20] These are the main types of performance errors in sign language however on the rare occasion there is also the possibility of errors in the order of the signs performed resulting in a different meaning than what the signer intended.[20]

Other types of errorsEdit

Unacceptable Sentences
are ones which, although are grammatical, are not considered proper utterances. They are considered unacceptable due to the lack of our cognitive systems to process them. Speakers and listeners can be aided in the performance and processing of these sentences by eliminating time and memory constraints, increasing motivation to process these utterances and using pen and paper.[17] In English there are three types of sentences that are grammatical but are considered unacceptable by speakers and listeners.[17]

  1. Repeated self-embedded clauses: The cheese that the rat that the cat chased ate is on the table.
  2. Multi Right Branching: This is the cat that caught the rat that ate the cheese that is on the table.
  3. Ambiguity or Garden Path Sentences: The horse raced past the barn fell

When a speaker makes an utterance they must translate their ideas into words, then syntactically proper phrases with proper pronunciation.[23] The speaker must have prior world knowledge and an understanding of the grammatical rules that their language enforces. When learning a second language or with children acquiring their first language, speakers usually have this knowledge before they are able to produce them.[23] Their speech is usually slow and deliberate, using phrases they have already mastered, and with practice their skills increase. Errors of linguistic performance are judged by the listener giving many interpretations if an utterance is well-formed or ungrammatical depending on the individual. As well the context in which an utterance is used can determine if the error would be considered or not.[24] When comparing «Who must telephone her?» and «Who need telephone her?» the former would be considered the ungrammatical phrase. However, when comparing it to «Who want telephone her?» it would be considered the grammatical phrase.[24] The listener may also be the speaker. When repeating sentences with errors if the error is not comprehended then it is performed. As well if the speaker does notice the error in the sentence they are supposed to repeat they are unaware of the difference between their well-formed sentence and the ungrammatical sentence.[20]
An unacceptable utterance can also be performed due to a brain injury. Three types of brain injuries that could cause errors in performance were studied by Fromkin are dysarthria, apraxia and literal paraphasia. Dysarthria is a defect in the neuromuscular connection that involves speech movement. The speech organs involved can be paralyzed or weakened, making it difficult or impossible for the speaker to produce a target utterance. Apraxia is when there is damage to the ability to initiate speech sounds with no paralysis or weakening of the articulators. Literal paraphasia causes disorganization of linguistic properties, resulting in errors of word order of phonemes.[20] Having a brain injury and being unable to perform proper linguistic utterances, some individuals are still able to process complex sentences and formulate syntactically well formed sentences in their mind.[17]Child productions when they are acquiring language are full of errors of linguistic performance. Children must go from imitating adult speech to create new phrases of their own. They will need to use their cognitive operations of the knowledge of their language they are learning to determine the rules and properties of that language.[23] The following are examples of errors in English speaking children’s productions.

  • «I goed»
  • «He runned»

In an elicited production experiment a child, Adam, was prompted to ask questions to an Old Lady[17]

Experimenter Adam, ask the Old Lady what she’ll do next.
Adam Old Lady, what will you do now?
Old Lady I’ll fly to the moon.
Experimenter Adam, ask the Old Lady why she can’t sit down.
Adam Old Lady, why you can’t sit down?
Old Lady You haven’t given me a chair.

Performance measuresEdit

Mean length of utteranceEdit

The most commonly used measure of syntax complexity is the mean length of utterance, also known as MLU.[25] This measure is independent from how often children talk and focuses on the complexity and development of their grammatical systems, including morphological and syntactic development.[26] The number representing a person’s MLU corresponds to the complexity of the syntax being used. In general, as the MLU increases, the syntactic complexity also increases. Typically, the average MLU corresponds to a child’s age due to their increase in working memory, which allows for sentences to be of greater syntactic complexity.[27] For example, the average MLU of a 7-year-old child is 7 words. However, children show more individual variability of syntactic performance with more complex syntax.[26] Complex syntax have a higher number of phrases and clause levels, therefore adding more words to the overall syntactic structure. Seeing as there are more individual differences in MLU and syntactic development as children get older, MLU is particularly used to measure grammatical complexity among school-aged children.[26] Other types of segmentation strategies for discourse are the T-unit and C-unit (communicative unit). If these two measurements are used to account for discourse, the average length of the sentence will be lower than if MLU is used alone. Both the T-units and C-units count each clause as a new unit, hence a lower number of units.

Typical MLU per age group can be found in the following table, according to Roger Brown’s five stages of syntactic and morphological development:[28]

Stage MLU Approximate Age (in months)
1 1.0-2.0 12-26
2 2.0-2.5 27-30
3 2.5-3.0 31-34
4 3.0-3.75 35-40
5 3.75-4.5 41-46
6 4.5+ 47+

Here are the steps for calculating MLU:[27]

  1. Acquire a language sample of about 50-100 utterances
  2. Count the number of morphemes said by the child, then divide by the number of utterances
  3. The investigator can assess what stage of syntactic development the child is at, based on their MLU

Here’s an example of how to calculate MLU:

Example utterance Morpheme and MLU Analysis Total MLU
go home now go (=1) home (=1) now (=1) 3
I live in Billingham I (=1) live (=1) in (=1) Billingham (=1) 4
Mommy kissed my Daddy Mommy (=1) kiss (=1) -ed (=1) my (=1) daddy (=1) 5
I like your dogs I (=1) like (=1) your (=1) dog (=1) -s (=1) 5

In total there are 17 morphemes in this data set. In order to find the MLU, we divide the total number of morphemes (17) by the total number of utterances (4). In this particular data set, the mean length of utterance is 17/4 = 4.25.[29]

Clause densityEdit

Clause density refers to the degree to which utterances contain dependent clauses. This density is calculated as a ratio of the total number of clauses across sentences, divide by the number of sentences in a discourse sample.[25] For example, if the clause density is 2.0, the ratio would indicate that the sentence being analyzed has 2 clauses on average: one main clause and one subordinate clause.

Here is an example of how clause density is measured, using T-units, adapted from Silliman & Wilkinson 2007:[30]

T-unit Number of words Number of clauses Example sentences from a story
1 12 2 When the night was dark I was watching TV in my room
2 5 1 I heard a howling noise
3 3 1 I looked outside

Indices of syntactic performanceEdit

Indices track structures to show a more comprehensive picture of a person’s syntactic complexity. Some examples of indices are Development Sentence Scoring, the Index of Productive Syntax and the Syntactic Complexity Measure.

Developmental sentence scoringEdit

Developmental Sentence Scoring is another method to measure syntactic performance as a clinical tool.[31] In this indice, each consecutive utterance, or sentence, elicited from a child is scored.[32] This is a commonly applied measurement of syntax for first and second language learners, with samples gathered from both elicited and spontaneous oral discourse. Methods for eliciting speech for these samples come in many forms, such having the participant answering questions or re-telling a story. These elicited conversations are commonly tape-recorded for playback during analysis to see how well the person can incorporate syntax among other linguistic cues.[31] For every utterance elicited, the utterance will receive one point if it is a correct form used in adult speech. A score of 1 indicates the least complex syntactic form in the category, whereas a higher score reflects higher level grammaticality.[31] Points are specifically awarded to an utterance based on whether or not it contains any of the eight categories outlined below.[31]

Syntactic categories measured by developmental sentence scoring with examples:

  Indefinite pronouns
  11a. Score of 1: it, this, that
  11b. Score of 6: both, many, several, most, least
  Personal pronouns
  12a. Score of 1: I, me, my, mine, you, your(s)
  12b. Score of 6: Wh-pronouns (i.e. who, which, what, how) and wh-word + infinitive (i.e. I know what to do)
  Main verb
  13a. Score of 1: Uninflected verb (i.e. I "see" you) and copula, is or 's (i.e. It's red)
  13b. Score of 6: Must, shall + verb (i.e. He "must come" or We "shall see"), have + verb + '-en' (i.e. I have eaten)
  Secondary verb
  14a. Score of 1: Infinitival complements (i.e. I wan"na see" = I want to see)
  14b. Score of 6: Gerund (i.e. Swinging is fun)
  Negatives
  15a. Score of 1: it, this or that + copula or auxiliary 'is' or 's + not (i.e. It's "not" mine)
  15b. Score of 5: Uncontracted negative with 'have' (i.e. I have "not" eaten it), auxiliary'have'-negative contraction (i.e. I had"n't" eaten 
       it), pronoun auxiliary 'have' contraction (i.e. I've "not" eaten it)
  Conjunctions
  16a. Score of 1: and 
  16b. Score of 6: where, than, how
  Interrogative reversals
  17a. Score of 1: Reversal of copula (i.e. "Is it" red?)
  17b. Score of 5: Reversal with three auxiliaries (i.e. "Could he" have been going?)
  Wh-questions
  18a. Score of 1: who or what (i.e. "What" do you mean?), what + noun (i.e. "What book" are you reading?)
  18b. Score of 5: whose or which (i.e. "Which" do you want?), which + noun (i.e. "Which book" do you want?)

In particular, those categories that appear the earliest in speech receive a lower score, whereas later-appearing categories receive a higher score. If an entire sentence is correct according to adult-like forms, then the utterance would receive an extra point.[31] The eight categories above are the most commonly used structures in syntactic formation, thus structures such as possessives, articles, plurals, prepositional phrases, adverbs and descriptive adjectives were omitted and not scored.[31] Additionally, the scoring system is arbitrary when applied to certain structures. For example, there is no indication as to why «if» would receive four points rather than five. The scores of all the utterances are totalled in the end of the analysis and then averaged to get a final score. This means that the individual’s final score reflects their entire syntactic complexity level, rather than syntactic level in a specific category.[31] The main advantage of development sentence scoring is that the final score represents the individual’s general syntactic development and allows for easier tracking of changes in language development, making this tool effective for longitudinal studies.[31]

Index of productive syntaxEdit

Similar to Development Sentence Scoring, the Index of Productive Syntax evaluates the grammatical complexity of spontaneous language samples. After age 3, Index of Productive Syntax becomes more widely used than MLU to measure syntactic complexity in children.[33] This is because at around age 3, MLU does not distinguish between children of similar language competency as well as Index of Productive Syntax does. For this reason, MLU is initially used in early childhood development to track syntactic ability, then Index of Productive Syntax is used to maintain validity. Individual utterances in a discourse sample are scored based on the presence of 60 different syntactic forms, placed more generally under four subscales: noun phrase, verb phrase, question/negation and sentence structure forms.[34] After a sample is recorded, a corpus is then formed based on 100 utterance transcriptions with 60 different language structures being measured in each utterance. Not included in the corpus are imitations, self-repetitions and routines, which constitute language that does not represent productive language usage.[35] In each of the four sub-scales previously mentioned, the first two unique occurrences of a form are scored. After this, occurrences of a sub-scale are not scored. However, if a child has mastered a complex syntax structure earlier than expected, they will receive extra points.[35]

Standardized testsEdit

The six main tasks in standardized testing for syntax:[25]

  • What is the level of syntactic complexity?
  • What specific syntactic structures are found? (a syntactic content analysis)
  • Are specific structures representative of what is known about syntactic development within the age range of standardization sample?
  • What are the processing requirements of the test format? (a task analysis)
  • Are processing requirements similar to or different from language processing in more naturalistic contexts?
  • Is syntactic ability in naturalistic language predicted by performance on the test?

Some of the common standardized tests for measuring syntactic performance are the TOLD-2 Intermediate (Test of Language Development), the TOAL-2 (Test of Adolescent Language) and the CELF-R (Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, Revised Screening Test).

Task being tested TOLD-2 Intermediate TOAL-2 CELF-R
Listening Grammaticality Judgement (hears 1 sentence: judges correct/incorrect) Syntactic Paraphrase (hears 3 sentences; marks 2 with similar meaning)
Speaking Sentence Combining (hears 2-4 sentences, says 1 sentence that combines input sentences) Sentence Imitation (hears 1 sentence, repeats verbatim) Formulating Sentences (hears 1-2 words and sees a picture; makes up a sentence using words), Imitating Sentences (hears 1 sentence, repeats verbatim), Scrambled Sentences (hears/sees/reads sentence components out of order; says 2 different recorded/correct versions)
Reading Syntactic paraphrase (read 5 sentences; marks 2 with similar meaning)
Writing Sentence combining (reads 2-6 sentences; writes 1 sentence that combines input sentences)

See alsoEdit

  • Langue and parole
  • Linguistic competence
  • Generative grammar
  • Transformational grammar
  • Psycholinguistics
  • Syntax

ReferencesEdit

  1. ^ Matthews, P. H. «performance.» Oxford Reference. 30 Oct. 2014. http://www.oxfordreference.com/view/10.1093/acref/9780199202720.001.0001/acref-9780199202720-e-2494.
  2. ^ Reishaan, Abdul-Hussein Kadhim (2008). «The Relationship between Competence and Performance: Towards a Comprehensive TG Grammar». اداب الكـوفة. 1 (2). Archived from the original on 2014-11-29. Retrieved 2014-11-14.
  3. ^ Carlson, Marvin (2013), Performance: A Critical Introduction (revised ed.), Routledge, ISBN 9781136498657
  4. ^ Myers, David G. (December 2011), «8», Psychology (10 ed.), worth publishers, p. 301, ISBN 9781429261784
  5. ^ a b Noam Chomsky.(2006).Language and Mind Third Edition. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 0-521-85819-4
  6. ^ a b Chomsky, Noam (1965), Aspects of the Theory of Syntax, p. 4, ISBN 0-262-53007-4
  7. ^ a b c de Saussure, F. (1986). Course in general linguistics (3rd ed.). (R. Harris, Trans.). Chicago: Open Court Publishing Company. (Original work published 1972). p. 9-10, 15, 102.
  8. ^ Chomsky, Noam (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge: MA: MIT Press.
  9. ^ a b Chomsky, Noam (1986).Knowledge of Language. New York:Praeger. ISBN 0-275-90025-8.
  10. ^ Lacey, Nick (1998). Image and Representation: Key Concepts in Media Studies. Palgrave.
  11. ^ A Chomsky, Noam (1956). «Three Models for the Description of Language». IRE Transactions on Information Theory 2 (2): 113 123.doi:10.1109/TIT.1956.1056813.
  12. ^ a b Smith, Neilson Voyne (1999). Chomsky: Ideas and Ideals. Cambridge University Press. pp. 37–39. ISBN 9780521837880.
  13. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k Hawkins, John A. (2004). Efficiency and Complexity in Grammars. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-199-25268-8.
  14. ^ a b c d e f g h i Wasow, Thomas (2002). Postverbal behavior. Vol. lecture notes, No. 145. Centre for the Study of Language and Information. ISBN 978-1-57586-401-3.
  15. ^ Karlsson, Fred; Voutilainen, Atro; Heikkilae, Juha; Anttila, Arto (January 1995), Constraint Grammar: A Language-Independent System for Parsing Unrestricted Text, Walter de Gruyter, ISBN 9783110882629
  16. ^ a b Sag, I. A. & Wasow, T., 2011. Performance-Compatible Competence Grammar[dead link]. In: R. Borsley & K. Börjars, eds. Non-Transformational Syntax: Formal and Explicit Models of Grammar. s.l.:John Wiley & Sons, pp. 359-377.
  17. ^ a b c d e Stephen Crain; Rosalind Thornton (2000). Investigations in Universal Grammar: A Guide to Experiments on the Acquisition of Syntax and Semantics. MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-53180-1.
  18. ^ a b Lise Menn (2011). Psycholinguistics: Introduction and Applications. Plural Pub. ISBN 978-1-59756-283-6.
  19. ^ Montserrat Sanz; Itziar Laka; Michael K. Tanenhaus (29 August 2013). Language Down the Garden Path: The Cognitive and Biological Basis for Linguistic Structures. Oxford University Press. pp. 2–. ISBN 978-0-19-967713-9.
  20. ^ a b c d e f g Victoria Fromkin (1980). Errors in linguistic performance: slips of the tongue, ear, pen, and hand. Academic Press. ISBN 978-0-12-268980-2.
  21. ^ a b Willem J. M. Levelt (1993). Speaking: From Intention to Articulation. MIT Press. ISBN 978-0-262-62089-5.
  22. ^ a b c Elisabetta Fava (2002). Clinical Linguistics: Theory and Applications in Speech Pathology and Therapy. John Benjamins Publishing. pp. 5–. ISBN 1-58811-223-3.
  23. ^ a b c Michael W. Eysenck; Mark T. Keane (2000). Cognitive Psychology: A Student’s Handbook. Taylor & Francis. ISBN 978-0-86377-550-5.
  24. ^ a b Montserrat Sanz; Itziar Laka; Michael K. Tanenhaus (29 August 2013). Language Down the Garden Path: The Cognitive and Biological Basis for Linguistic Structures. Oxford University Press. pp. 258–. ISBN 978-0-19-967713-9.
  25. ^ a b c Scott, CM & Stokes, SL 1995 ‘Measures of Syntax in School Age Children and Adolescents’, Language, Speech & Hearing Services in Schools, vol.56, pp. 309-320
  26. ^ a b c Huttenlocher, J, Vasilyeva, M,
    Cymerman, E & Levine, S 2002, ‘Language input and child syntax’, Cognitive Psychology, vol. 45, pp. 337–374.
  27. ^ a b Everyday Language Discovering the Hidden Powers of Speech and Language 2014, Morphology and MLU. Available from: <http://everydaylanguage.qwriting.qc.cuny.edu/2014/03/08/morphology-and-mlu/>. [12 November 2014].
  28. ^ Brown, R 1973, A first language: The early stages, George Allen & Unwin, London.
  29. ^ Speech Language Therapy Info 2014, Mean Length of Utterance. Available from: <http://www.sltinfo.com/mean-length-of-utterance/>. [12 November 2014].
  30. ^ Silliman, ER & Wilkinson, LC 2007, Language and Literacy Learning in Schools. The Guilford Press, New York.
  31. ^ a b c d e f g h Rheinhardt, KM 1972, ‘The Developmental Sentence Scoring Procedure’, Independent Studies and Capstones, vol. 314.
  32. ^ Politzer, RL, 1974, ‘Developmental Sentence Scoring as a Method of Measuring Second Language Acquisition’, Modern Language Journal, vol. 58, no. 5/6, pp. 245.
  33. ^ Lavie, A, Sagae, K, MacWhinney, B, ‘Automatic Measurement of Syntactic Development in Child Language’, Proceedings of the 43rd Annual Meeting on Association for Computational Linguistics, pp.197-204.
  34. ^ Springer Reference 2014, Index of Productive Syntax (IPSyn). Available from: <http://www.springerreference.com/docs/html/chapterdbid/333184.html>. [26 October 2014].
  35. ^ a b Moyle, M & Long, S 2013, ‘Index of Productive Syntax (IPSyn)’, Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders, pp. 1566-1568


На основании Вашего запроса эти примеры могут содержать грубую лексику.


На основании Вашего запроса эти примеры могут содержать разговорную лексику.


Hanna also began doing spoken word performances that addressed sexism and violence against women, issues with which she became concerned after volunteering for SafePlace, a domestic violence organization over the next two years.



Ханна также начинает выступать со spoken word шоу на темы сексизма и насилия над женщинами; проблем, с которыми она столкнулась, пока работала волонтером организации по предотвращению домашнего насилия на протяжении двух лет.


His YouTube Channel, Hussain’s House, features his spoken word performances and funny videos.



Его YouTube-канал, Hussain’s House, включает в себя его устные представления и забавные видеоролики.

Другие результаты


And later on the same evening, he will be doing a spoken word performance.


Joan Sutherland Theatre, the Opera House’s second-largest internal venue, is ideal for housing opera, ballet, contemporary dance, music and spoken-word performances.



Театр Джоан Сазерленд, второй по величине внутренний дом Оперы, идеально подходит для проживания оперы, балета, современного танца, музыки и устных выступлений.


The venerable Chicago Theater in the Loop is better-known for its sign then for anything else, but it has rock, jazz, gospel, and spoken-word performances by authors like David Sedaris.



Почтенный Чикагский театр в «Петле» более известен своим знаком, чем чем-либо еще, но в нем есть рок, джаз, евангелие и выступления произнесенных авторов, таких как Дэвид Седарис.


A hundred pairs of eyes were anticipating her spoken word poetry performance.


In 2015, she launched with musician Gaston Gorga PNEUMA, a collaborative project including spoken word, live performance, sound manipulation, improvisation and visuals.



В 2015 году совместно с Гастоном Горджа она запустила проект PNEUMA — сочетающий в себе песни, живой перформанс, эксперименты со звуком, импровизацию, слово и визуальный ряд.


There was an increase in hip-hop groups that merged the dance with theatre, contemporary dance, spoken word and other performance art forms.



Увеличилось число хип-хоп групп, которые объединили танец с театром, современным танцем и другими формами исполнительского искусства.


Spoken-word poetry is the art of performance poetry.


The Kennedy Center’s Hip Hop series is slated to feature five events, including a spoken word summit, dance workshops and live performances.



Планируется 5 хип-хоп шоу от центра Кеннеди, в течение которых будут саммиты, танцевальные семинары и конечно же живые выступления.


Within 24 hours, the performance was made into a digital single featuring the NHS Voices of Care Choir, and Moore’s spoken words.



В течение 24 часов это представление было записано и превращено в цифровой сингл с участием подпевающего Тома Мура, а также хора врачей и волонтёров «Голоса заботы» Национальной службы здравоохранения Великобритании (NHS).


The world-famous Amateur Night at the Apollo Theater returns in February for a new season of performances from singers, dancers, comedians and spoken-word artists.



Всемирно известный аматорский вечер в театре «Аполлон» возвращается в феврале для нового сезона выступлений от певцов, танцоров, комиков и артистов.


In addition, the department programs a 25-show season every year that includes performances ranging from performance art, theater, dance, spoken word and music.



Кроме того, отделение исполнительского искусства ежегодно организует серию из 25 шоу, в которые входят перформансы, театр, танец, литература и музыка.


The former includes all activities which are helpful for storing and retrieving information: photographing, writing, taping, etc. The latter implies skilled performances which are sent by means of physical activity, like a spoken word or a handshake.



Первая включает все виды деятельности, которые помогают сохранять и извлекать информацию: фотография, письмо, запись видео и т. д. Вторая подразумевает выполнение выученных актов путём физических действий, например, произнесённое слово или рукопожатие.


Lamar has been praised and lauded for his deep lyrical content, remarkable live performances, and his profound mix of hip-hop, spoken word, jazz, soul, funk, poetry and African sounds.



Ламара восхваляли и выделили глубокое лирическое содержание его музыки, замечательные живые выступления и необычное сочетание хип-хопа, устного слова, джаза, соула, фанка, поэзии и африканских звуков.


The former includes all activities which are helpful for storing and retrieving information: photographing, writing, taping, etc. The latter implies skilled performances which are sent by means of physical activity, like a spoken word or a handshake.



Первая включает все виды деятельности, которые помогают сохранять и извлекать информацию: фотография, письмо, запись видео и т. д. Вторая подразумевает выполнение выученных актов путём физических действий, например, произнесённое слово или рукопожатие.


What are the current experiments involving spoken word, gesture, voice, sound, music and movement in performance?



Каковы сегодняшние эксперименты с участием слова, жеста, голоса, звука, музыки и движения?


While Samsung will not collect your spoken word, they may still collect associated texts and other usage data so that we can evaluate the performance of the feature and improve it.



Несмотря на то, что компания Samsung не будет собирать произносимые Вами слова, она все же может собирать соответствующую текстовую информацию и другие данные об использовании с целью оценки и улучшения работы службы.


I decided it had to be something spoken word.



Я рассуждал, что это должно было быть какое-то матерное слово.


He had the character to handle the Spoken Word.



У него был отличный характер для того, чтобы обращаться с Изречённым Словом.

Ничего не найдено для этого значения.

Результатов: 1196. Точных совпадений: 2. Затраченное время: 280 мс

Documents

Корпоративные решения

Спряжение

Синонимы

Корректор

Справка и о нас

Индекс слова: 1-300, 301-600, 601-900

Индекс выражения: 1-400, 401-800, 801-1200

Индекс фразы: 1-400, 401-800, 801-1200

Presentation on theme: «Spoken Word Performance Text Production Oral English Studies Melanie Smith.»— Presentation transcript:

1

2

Spoken Word Performance Text Production Oral English Studies Melanie Smith

3

4

5

Subject Outline Oral Text Production A range of forms of presentation. Develop students’ skills in the oral presentation of complex ideas and information in a fluent and expressive manner. An awareness and control of language techniques and stylistic features appropriate to different audiences, purposes, and contexts.

6

Chief Assessor’s Report Creativity and flair. Opportunity to experiment. Demonstrate a wide range of language skills. Offer options to cater for their interests and needs. Skilfully use conventions of the text type that they are trying to create.

7

The key point is that a range of language skills is evident. Avoid simply reading out a script in a monotone manner. Opportunity to showcase voice and performance techniques Tone Volume Pace Pausing Emphasis Emotion Variations of these

8

Where to next? ‘Spoken word poetry tends to demonstrate a heavy use of rhythm, improvisation, free association, rhymes, rich poetic phrases, word play and slang. It is more aggressive and “in your face” than traditional forms of poetry.’

9

A brief history of spoken word and slam poetry. Political urgency that drove it originally. Nature of modern day slam competitions.

10

What to keep in mind? Concrete Language – project vivid images, sounds, actions and other sensations. Repetition – a simple but powerful poetic device. Rhyme – Rhyming use with skill, surprise and moderation. Attitude – “No attitude, no poem!”

11

Persona – spoken word allows you to be anyone you want to be. Performance – practice performing with elements of good stage presence in mind… Posture and stance Eye contact Projection Enunciation Facial expressions Gestures

12

Kyle “Guante” Tran Myhre and Brother Dash Be True To You! Speak from an authentic experience or perspective. Substance over style. It is an opportunity to say something to an audience. Challenge the audience. Don’t tell people what they want to hear—push them out of their comfort zones.

13

Be specific. Don’t write about “war,” write about a specific person in a specific war dealing with a specific problem. Don’t write about “love,” tell a detailed story about a specific moment in your life when you felt loved.

14

Perform to the audience, not at the audience. Rewrite, edit and re-edit your work. Play with the flow, rhythm and beat of the lines, images and choose precise words or phrases.

15

Read your poem out loud. Know how the words feel in your mouth and sound in your ears. Record your voice and listen to it in order to make changes or improvements. Slow Down! OR Speed up! Perform, Perform, Perform!

16

Watch examples for performance elements. A great place to start is: buttonpoetry.com and https://www.youtube.com/user/Butto nPoetry and https://www.youtube.com/user/Butto nPoetry http://www.poetryoutloud.org/poems -and-performance/tips-on-reciting http://www.poetryoutloud.org/poems -and-performance/tips-on-reciting Explore transcripts of these pieces for written elements. Discuss idea of ‘page to stage’

17

After the talking… Javon Johnson — Cuz He’s Black http://thisis2020.com/poetry-cuz-hes-black/ Shane Koyczan — To This Day http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=KOirdcpYKRw Rachel Rostad — To JK Rowling, from Cho Chang http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=iFPWwx96Kew Sarah Kay – If I should have a daughter http://www.ted.com/talks/sarah_kay_if_i_should_have_a_daughter.html Dylan Garity — Friend Zone http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=_xHp5iTtWRc Sarah Jones — Your Revolution http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=xRgIGMwZd2o Inspired by Gil Scott-Heron’s “The Revolution Will Not Be Televised”

18

Taylor Mali — What Teachers Make http://www.google.com/url?sa=t&rct=j&q=&esrc=s&frm=1&source=video&cd=5&cad=rja &uact=8&ved=0CEoQtwIwBA&url=http%3A%2F%2Fwww.youtube.com%2Fwatch%3Fv%3D 3xkk71rhQpo&ei=jJkjU7bCEIqqkgW_toBg&usg=AFQjCNGarMfqKvub5rpsA1IkLJf_B90d4A& bvm=bv.62922401,d.dGI Georgia Me — Full Figure Potential (A fat girl’s blues) http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=GOrI5B0rGZI George Watsky – V for Virgin http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=EhPikRQRX5o Suli — “I WILL NOT LET AN EXAM RESULT DECIDE MY FATE” http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=D-eVF_G_p-Y Neil Hilborn — OCD http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=vnKZ4pdSU-s http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=vi09ALwxIig Katie Makkai — Pretty http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=M6wJl37N9C0 Guante – Ten Responses to the phrase “Man Up” http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=QFoBaTkPgco Pages Matam — Pinatas http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=zgQRkHcEyq8

19

Live audience and recorded for audience Ethan Smith A Letter to the Girl I used to be https://www.youtube.com/wat ch?v=Lkn06Y8prDU https://www.youtube.com/wat ch?v=Lkn06Y8prDU https://www.youtube.com/wat ch?v=1Sc6w4I1Z00 https://www.youtube.com/wat ch?v=1Sc6w4I1Z00

20

If they still look like this…

21

SHARE THE PAIN! This is where I embarrass myself

22

Things that worked Getting them to find something of importance to them. Giving them space to lessen the embarrassment. The option of taping/recording.

23

Say it again Brainstorm several generic phrases and play with how they can be said. You never did, not really, not at all. You never did, not really, not at all. You never did, not really, not at all.

24

The Recording Draft This strategy was surprisingly effective allowing students room to play with their presenting style. Listen back… Feedback on voice, pausing, emphasis, emotion, tone, volume etc.

25

Son to Mother You suffer in silence, never uttering a word as you try to hold back the beastly black dog. Check where emphasis can add emotion You watch in fear as it bites and tears pieces same here which words need stressing? from your only son, bit by bit, limb from limb. Your whole world collapses around you as you realise you cannot be his saviour. watch up inflection… Takes away seriousness You attempt to remain strong for the sake of your son, pause between 2 parts but he hopes and prays, that one day you’ll see, you were never to blame, good emphasis here not for something so surreal, nothing could have prepared you for something so severe. Good pausing between last three words

26

Speaking back to students Inflection example Emotion and emphasis example

27

Topics they covered… Anorexia Depression Body hair Body image Cruelty of words Girl school bitchery Art Perception Refugees Double meanings Justice and judgement Over thinking Difference Gender Life and death Student exchange Addiction and family Words as an escape Finding your way Overcoming adversity Memory Alzheimer’s Being thankful Going first Saying goodbye Anxiety Intelligence vs. interests Choice

28

Show creativity and flair. Opportunity to experiment. Demonstrate a wide range of language skills. Offer options to cater for their interests and needs. Skilfully use conventions of the text type that they are trying to create.

29

The end result A range of forms of presentation. Develop students’ skills in the oral presentation of complex ideas and information in a fluent and expressive manner. An awareness and control of language techniques and stylistic features appropriate to different audiences, purposes, and contexts. A different form of presentation than analysis of a shared text piece, it is an original piece. A focus on a personal complex idea and being expressive in their delivery. The opportunity to demonstrate awareness and control of their own use of devices for the purpose of performance and expression.

30

31

Please email me msmith@eynesbury.sa.edu.au If you would like copies of: This PowerPoint Task Sheet and assessment criteria Assessment Feedback Sheet Student Background/Preparation materials, professional examples, transcripts, and links Student Examples of final pieces

Art

Tricia Christensen

Tricia Christensen

Last Modified Date: April 12, 2023

Tricia Christensen

Tricia Christensen

Last Modified Date: April 12, 2023

Spoken word is an art form that has long been present in human society. Oral traditions in cultures predate the written word, and poems, or a type of spoken singing or stories, sometimes accompanied by music, held captive many an audience far before the first book was ever written. Even when books became available, troubadours and balladeers often spoke stories of adventure, romance, tragedy and comedy to wide audiences, since reading was not a common accomplishment.

Today, spoken word refers to two separate art forms. One is entirely based on recorded spoken word, such as the reading of novels or the recitation of poetry that supplies us with the modern “books on tape.” The art form in its second definition refers to those events when poets, and sometimes comedians or social commentators recite or invent on the spot poems, commentary, jokes, and the like as a performance piece intended for an audience. The latter type arises from the oral traditions long held in numerous cultures.

Spoken word performances are common in hip-hop.

Spoken word performances are common in hip-hop.

Songs did replace speaking stories or poems, but in the Beat Poetry movement, interest in performing poetry and other writings held sway over poetry fans. Typically beat poets, not necessarily the famous ones, would recite their poems with music playing in the background. Emphasis however was more on the words than the music, though the poet might time phrasing to coincide with certain parts of musical performance. Music could be live or recorded depending upon the poet.

This tradition led to a little mockery, and some contend participants and fans took it too seriously. Certainly as with all genres of poetry, there were some bad poems written and performed. There were also some very valuable ones, and poets that grew out of the Beat Generation, include Mayo Angelou, who critics cite as being one of the most skilled at reciting her work, since her rich voice and phrasing lend resonance and depth to her work.

Spoken word had few adherents by the mid 1970s, but interest in the form reemerged in the 1980s. Part of this was caused by the variant of spoken word performance in hip-hop and rap. Much of what rap artists did was perform poetry with a musical background, and with specific types of phrasing and resonance. Music fans and literary analysts alike evaluated and continue to evaluate the poetry of many rap artists, and call a number of these artists intensely important contributors to the world of poetry.

The beat type spoken word, often set to jazz instead, and sometimes including other performance aspects like slides or pictures, reemerged as a popular form along with rap. Another outgrowth of spoken word was slam poetry, where poets competed against each other, often extemporaneously. Slam poetry has its roots in Japanese contests of this type, such as the beginning and end of waka, a poetic form that existed in the 10th century.

In another sense though, spoken word doesn’t necessarily involve poetry, and has never gone out of style. It can be the recitation of stories, a solo comedic act, or the performance of a storyteller. There are no hard and fast rules for the art form, which is why you will find many different names associated with the form. Robin Williams is as much master of this literary art form as is Allen Ginsberg or Patti Smith.

Tricia Christensen

Tricia has a Literature degree from Sonoma State University and has been a frequent
contributor for many years. She is especially passionate about reading and writing, although her other interests include
medicine, art, film, history, politics, ethics, and religion. Tricia lives in Northern California and is currently
working on her first novel.

Tricia Christensen

Tricia Christensen

Tricia has a Literature degree from Sonoma State University and has been a frequent
contributor for many years. She is especially passionate about reading and writing, although her other interests include
medicine, art, film, history, politics, ethics, and religion. Tricia lives in Northern California and is currently
working on her first novel.

You might also Like

(redirected from Spoken word performance)
Also found in: Thesaurus.

ThesaurusAntonymsRelated WordsSynonymsLegend:

Noun 1. spoken word - a word that is spoken aloudspoken word — a word that is spoken aloud    

word — a unit of language that native speakers can identify; «words are the blocks from which sentences are made»; «he hardly said ten words all morning»

Based on WordNet 3.0, Farlex clipart collection. © 2003-2012 Princeton University, Farlex Inc.

Mentioned in
?

  • accent
  • accented
  • accenting
  • agitate
  • agitated
  • agitatedly
  • agitating
  • agitative
  • agnosia
  • aphaeresis
  • apheresis
  • aphesis
  • auditory agnosia
  • bigram
  • Canada
  • Canadian
  • channel
  • channeler
  • channelling

References in periodicals archive
?

Performance poetry is more entertaining, it’s like poetry and theatre almost.» Jo, who runs the Art Room project for the Grampian Hospital Arts Trust and works with the Silver City Surfers, said she first became interested in spoken word performance while studying at university.

Ptak, poetry and spoken word performance by Elgin Poet Laureate Chasity Gunn, community booths, children’s crafts and games, and drawings for gift baskets.

Other highlights of the event included a presentation by Ifeoma Fafunwa entitled ‘Our stories as an Inspiration to the next generation’, followed by a spoken word performance from her critically acclaimed play ‘Hearword’.

During the event, participants enjoyed an evening of comedy, poetry, music and spoken word performance by students and members of the wider community.

Also joining the fun, Michellan Sarile-Alagao will be rendering a Spoken Word Performance. Lomography Philippines will also be giving away a Lomo’Instant Camera during the event.

The program will feature a spoken word performance by TiKa Wallace of George Mason High School in Falls Church, Virginia, as well as remarks by agency and YoungArts staff.

Abu Dhabi: The second day of Abu Dhabi’s CultureSummit witnessed a captivating spoken word performance by the UAE’s very own Afra Atiq, as she demonstrated the power of words while conveying personal stories about her life and family.

Saleh will deliver a spoken word performance, while Alexander and Kay will produce and record a mass participation dance in Birmingham.

Angelica also guested in Moira’s concert last weekend and was reported by ABS-CBN to have asked «Bakit lagi ako iniiwan?» in a spoken word performance.

The video can be seen at www.youtube.com/watch?v=-9edIBvlrlg Johnny Marr and Maxine Peake Former Smiths guitarist Johnny Marr has enlisted the vocal assistance of actress Maxine Peake to create an album of music and spoken word performance. Their work may appear to be just like any conventional rap track.

In addition to the youth panel, there was a spoken word performance by two members of the Tennessee State University Speech & Debate Team.

Whether it’s a book, a record, or a spoken word performance, genderqueer artist Andrea Gibson gives voice to truth in a world of increasing chaos and noise.

Dictionary browser
?

  • spoil
  • Spoil bank
  • spoilable
  • spoilage
  • spoilation
  • spoiled
  • spoiled ballot
  • spoiled priest
  • spoiler
  • spoilfive
  • spoilful
  • spoiling
  • spoiling attack
  • spoils
  • spoils system
  • spoilsman
  • Spoilsmonger
  • spoilsport
  • spoilt
  • Spokane
  • spoke
  • spoken
  • spoken communication
  • spoken language
  • spoken word
  • Spoken word performance
  • spoken-word
  • spokeshave
  • spokesman
  • spokesmanship
  • spokesmodel
  • spokesperson
  • spokeswoman
  • spokewise
  • spoliate
  • spoliation
  • spoliative
  • spoliator
  • Spoliatory
  • spondaic
  • spondaical
  • spondaise
  • spondaize
  • spondee
  • Spondias
  • Spondias mombin
  • Spondias purpurea
  • spondulicks
  • Spondulics
  • spondulix
  • spondyl

Full browser
?

  • spoken up for
  • spoken up for
  • spoken up for
  • spoken up for her
  • spoken up for him
  • spoken up for me
  • spoken up for someone
  • spoken up for them
  • spoken up for us
  • spoken up for you
  • spoken upon
  • spoken volumes
  • spoken volumes
  • spoken volumes
  • spoken volumes for
  • spoken volumes for it
  • spoken volumes for something
  • spoken well
  • spoken well
  • spoken well
  • spoken with
  • spoken with a forked tongue
  • spoken with a silver tongue
  • spoken word
  • spoken word
  • spoken word
  • Spoken word artist
  • Spoken word artist
  • Spoken word music
  • Spoken word music
  • Spoken word performance
  • Spoken word poetry
  • Spoken Word Publishing Association
  • Spoken word recognition
  • Spoken word recognition
  • Spoken word recognition
  • Spoken words
  • Spoken words
  • Spoken words
  • Spoken words
  • Spoken Words per Minute
  • spoken your language
  • spoken your peace
  • spoken your piece
  • spoken-word
  • spoken-word
  • spokenly
  • spokes
  • spokes
  • spokes
  • Spokes and Hub Design
  • spokes-and-hub design
  • spokeshave
  • spokeshave
  • spokeshaved
  • spokeshaved
  • spokeshaves
  • spokeshaves
  • spokeshaving
  • spokeshaving
  • spokesman

slam poetry

by CJ McDaniel // August 1  

People have different views about slam poetry and spoken words. Some refer to them as poetry, others as a script, and some as a form of rap or hip-hop. Whatever your concept, have it in mind that they have elements of poetry even though they involve performance.

In reality, slam poetry and spoken word are used interchangeably. It’s so because of the similarities between them. Also, the differences between them are so small compared to their similarities.

This article is a comparison between slam poetry and the spoken word. However, it shows what makes up slam poetry and spoken word and how you can write and perform it.

What Is Slam Poetry?

Slam poetry is known as a form of spoken word that is performed in a slam. It’s performed as a competition between poets at the event. Usually, at a slam, judges are picked from among the audience to score each poet’s performance. It also depends on how it engages the audience.

What Is A Spoken Word?

A spoken word refers to a poem written and has to be performed. It refers to poetry that is written for performance on stage. It can be performed anywhere the poet chooses to or wherever he has a platform to perform.

Comparison Between Slam Poetry And Spoken Word
Slam poetry is performed as a form of competition with other poets. While the spoken word is usually not a competition, it can be just a performance to entertain an audience.

Slam poetry occurs in events known as a poetry slam. But spoken word occurs at any gathering or event, even in a classroom.

However, they’re both performance poetry. That involves reading the poem aloud, using body language, gestures, and facial expressions to pass the message well. Also, you’ll require a similar kind of preparation to write and perform them.

Therefore, they have the same elements, procedures, and steps for writing and performance.

Elements Of Spoken Word And Slam Poetry

Concrete language: Spoken word poetry makes use of concrete language. It includes words and phrases that give your audience a clear understanding of the topic. It also makes the poem clear and vivid. Also, it uses a language that appeals to the senses.

Attitude: It consists of how you perform your spoken word. You should perform it in a way that gives off the opinions and feelings behind it. You should be able to play out your poem. It’s the attitude you deliver that makes it really spoken word poetry and not just a recitation.

Repetition: It involves repeating some phrases or words to create emphasis. Such words or phrases easily stick to the minds of your audience. Also, it contributes to making the poem exciting.

Literary devices: Spoken word contains literary devices. They help to make the spoken word more than just simple sentences. So, they show the audience what you mean and express emotions. They include simile, metaphor, personification, etc. Also, it would be best if you don’t overuse them.

Rhythm: This refers to a pattern of sound that comes with stressing different syllables. It gives a kind of flow to the poem. Also, it uses sound to reinforce meaning. You can use some sound devices like onomatopoeia, alliteration, and consonance to create rhythm.

Rhyme: It involves the use of the same sounds at the end of a line. However, you can choose not to use rhymes. It’s according to your style. But use it moderately.

How To Write Spoken Word And Slam Poetry

Watch others perform: To write and perform spoken words and slam poetry effectively, you have to watch others perform. You can attend a poetry slam or watch them online. Note the ones that impress you most and why they did. Also, be attentive to their use of words, rhythm, and flow of the spoken words.

Some people may prefer to take this step before writing their poems. Others prefer doing so after they’re done writing to get inspired on performing it. So watch others perform whenever suits your purpose.

Choose a topic: Before you write your spoken word poem, you have to choose a topic. You have to select a topic that you’re passionate about because the poem is performed with emotions. You can also choose one that’s a special memory.

However, it’s best if it’s not too broad but specific. For instance, a topic on “love” can be narrowed down to “meeting your first love, or how you love your mom.” Also, it can be centered on your experiences, such as “being part of a large family.” It can also be in the form of a question such as,” What is fear?”

The point is that whatever you choose should draw out your passion while you perform.

Write down all your ideas: Once you’ve selected your topic, take out time to write down ideas. When you brainstorm, put down whatever comes to mind. Write down all your thoughts and feelings towards the topic. Also, you can take breaks and come back to it.

Furthermore, it’ll also help if you carry a pen and a little journal in your pocket or purse. That way, you can easily put down random ideas where ever you are. It’s normal for you to go off point when you brainstorm. That doesn’t mean you won’t get it right.

Write your first draft: After you’re done writing your ideas, you have to put down your first draft. Pick out the words or phrases that relate to your topic. Focus on bringing out the best from the mountain of ideas you wrote down initially.

Also, arrange words to suit the message you want to pass. Start with simple phrases till you get the framework for your poem. Once you’ve gotten what tells the story, then you’re good to go.

Use literary devices: Now that you’ve gotten a framework to work with, use literary devices. They give a flow to your spoken word. Add them in the right parts of your poem. Also, choose the best for your spoken word, such as simile, metaphor, etc.

Check for sentences that’ll sound better with a literary device. However, don’t erase your original lines. Indicate the replacement beside it or write another copy using the replacement. It may help because the original line may still be useful.

Make use of repetitions and wordplay: Adding repetition and wordplay can boost your spoken word. When you repeat some words or lines, it emphasizes them. Also, it keeps replaying in the mind of your audience and becomes memorable.

Furthermore, wordplay creates a mixture of feelings for your audience. It also improves the creativity of your poem.

Improve the sound: You’re writing your spoken word to read it aloud, so make sure it sounds good. You don’t need a rhyme scheme, but you can use rhyme to improve the sound. Also, the rhythm of your spoken word can improve when you use poetic devices. They include alliteration, onomatopoeia, and so on.

Revise your poem: After you’re through improving your poem, all that’s left is to revise it. It should help you finalize your poem. First, take a break (very important!). It’ll help you revise your piece with fresh eyes.

When you revise it, you’re likely to see what you could have written in a better way. Read it out loud, first to yourself and then a friend. Try to do it several times. Also, effect any corrections you notice it.

Additionally, your piece should have an image. It should be able to pass the right message in the right way to your audience.

How To Improve Your Performance For Spoken Word And Slam Poetry 

After you write your poem, you have to memorize it. That makes performing it easier. But you’ll need to practice a lot to give a great performance.

Posture: Practice the correct posture for performing. You’ll have to stand upright, with your head held high. Bear in mind that the right posture shows your confidence. So your audience will be more eager to see what you have to offer. That’s why you should practice, because you may be nervous, especially if it’s your first time.

However, you can use any posture that explains your spoken word when you’re performing.

Eye contact: Make eye contact with your audience from time to time. It’ll get their attention and will also help to emphasize that part. Don’t make the mistake of going to the stage with a piece of paper. Even if you hide it, your audience will notice that you’re looking at something else.

Volume and pitch: Your volume and pitch should make your spoken words clear. They can also emphasize some words or lines. Your voice should be loud enough for your audience to hear you. But it would help if you aren’t too fast for your pitch.

Pauses: Pause when it’s needed. Don’t just say the words from start to end with no pauses. A short pause each time a dot separates the lines is essential. However, you can use pauses according to your discretion to drive your point home.

Facial expression: Your facial expression should reflect the mood of the spoken word. If there are different moods at various parts, your facial expression should easily switch to reflect them.

Gesture: You can use various gestures to express more clearly, what a line is saying. It also reflects what the poem entails. The way you move your body or use your hands says a lot about the poem and emphasizes a point.

Conclusion

In essence, the main difference between slam poetry and spoken word is in the events that they’re performed. Apart from that, you prepare for both performances in the same way. Therefore, they have the same elements and framework when writing them.

So, you can use the poem you performed as a spoken word to compete at a poetry slam. Now, you can easily write your piece, memorize, practice, and finally, perform it as a spoken word or slam poetry.

Author Image

CJ grew up admiring books. His family owned a small bookstore throughout his early childhood, and he would spend weekends flipping through book after book, always sure to read the ones that looked the most interesting. Not much has changed since then, except now some of those interesting books he picks off the shelf were designed by his company!

Regional Conference

«Since Martin Luther King, Jr.’s death, several persons, especially those seeking data for an article or book, have asked me whether I knew in what way I was influencing his life. The answer is an unqualified ‘No.’ There is no way one can know the degree of influence one has upon another.” ~ Dr. Benjamin E. Mays, in Born to Rebel

Aligning with the overall theme of our MMUF Regional Conference, we would like to invite you to use performance and the aesthetics of word play to speak to commonalities and differences observed and experienced throughout your life. Your observations or narration of experiences can emerge from and engage multiple domains of thinking: social, political, personal, sacred, environmental, economic, psychological, aesthetic, cultural, historical, linguistic, etc.  Choose one or weave several together thematically

What is Spoken Word?

There are many definitions of Spoken Word, and you may have a preferred description. For the sake of brevity, let’s provisionally define it as an aesthetic expression of emotional truth emerging from our complex negotiation of the world (your “life thesis”).

Please submit your work for consideration by October 9th.  Performers for the MMUF Western Regional Conference will be notified by October 16th.

Guidelines for the performance:

  • You may read or recite your work
  • Please limit your Spoken Word to three minutes maximum
  • Provide a brief, written Bio (50 words) explaining how the conference theme «Intersections of Identity» impacts your academic life and/or situates, informs, or intensifies your research
  • Provide a “selfie” or another representative image of yourself

(We may choose to publish your “MMUF Regional Conference Spoken Word” which would include your written piece, your bio, and your photo)

Although we will only have time for a few performances, we welcome ALL written submissions from Mellon Fellows for our informal/online publication (writers will be consulted regarding any editing)  

Select examples of “Spoken Word” performances; please “google” the following:image

  • Afro-Latina by Elizabeth Acevedo
  • When you Say My Name by Zachary Caballero
  • Accents by Denise Frohman
  • Brief History by Jose Soto
  • Descendancy by Mayda de Valle
  • On Realizing I am Blk by Gabriel Ramirez

Questions?  Submissions?  Help and Feedback?  Please contact Winona Wynn at the following email address:  Wynn_w@heritage.edu 


Asked by: Sydni D’Amore

Score: 4.4/5
(23 votes)

A broad designation for poetry intended for performance. Spoken word can encompass or contain elements of rap, hip-hop, storytelling, theater, and jazz, rock, blues, and folk music. …

Where does spoken word come from?

Where has it come from? Spoken word first kicked off with the American Beat Poet movement that took place in the 1940s and 1950s. This saw a group of authors in New York using their work to exploring and influence the American culture of the time.

How do you describe spoken word poetry?

Spoken word poetry is poetry that is written on a page but performed for an audience. Because it is performed, this poetry tends to demonstrate a heavy use of rhythm, improvisation, free association, rhymes, rich poetic phrases, word play and slang.

What’s the difference between spoken word and poetry?

“How is spoken word different from page poetry?” This question seems to have a pretty basic answer: one is written with the intention of being performed, or spoken aloud, while the other is written specifically for the page.

What is the difference between spoken word and rap?

spoken word is delivery based on message emphasis. you deliver your message in a way that will put more power behind your words. rap is delivery based on beat. you deliver your message based on complimenting the beat/music (while emphasizing the message like spoken word.

44 related questions found

Why rap is not a poem?

Rap Is Not Poetry

From linguistic point of view rap music is not poetry. This is because even though it looks like poetry, it takes the ill formed nature of poetry. That is, rappers usually manipulate their wordings to form a metrical pattern (Mel 43).

What is spoken word in rap?

Spoken word is a «catchall» term that includes any kind of poetry recited aloud, including poetry readings, poetry slams, jazz poetry, and hip hop music, and can include comedy routines and prose monologues.

What defines spoken word?

A broad designation for poetry intended for performance. Though some spoken word poetry may also be published on the page, the genre has its roots in oral traditions and performance. Spoken word can encompass or contain elements of rap, hip-hop, storytelling, theater, and jazz, rock, blues, and folk music.

Can spoken word rhyme?

Spoken word poetry is a form of poetry that doesn’t have to rhyme, but certain parts can be rhymed to emphasize an image or give it a lyrical quality. Spoken word poems will sometimes contain elements of hip-hop, folk music, or jazz to enhance the rhythmic presentation.

Is spoken word Slam Poetry?

A spoken word refers to a poem written and has to be performed. It refers to poetry that is written for performance on stage. … Slam poetry is performed as a form of competition with other poets. While the spoken word is usually not a competition, it can be just a performance to entertain an audience.

Who is the best spoken word artist?

12 Powerful Spoken Word Artists You Need To Add To Your Playlist

  • Alok Vaid-Menon (Preferred pronouns: they/them) …
  • Uppa Tsuyo Bantawa (Preferred pronouns: he/him and she/her) …
  • Andrea Gibson (Preferred pronouns: they/them) …
  • Dr Abhijit Khandkar (Preferred pronouns: he/him) …
  • Safia Elhillo (Preferred pronouns: she/her)

What was the first spoken word?

Also according to Wiki answers,the first word ever uttered was “Aa,” which meant “Hey!” This was said by an australopithecine in Ethiopia more than a million years ago.

Why is spoken word so powerful?

It encourages cathartic expression and emotional processing that ultimately contributes to a more holistic pedagogical space. It fosters a culture of active listening. Just as important as the opportunity to speak, spoken word also provides young people a place to listen.

What are the benefits of spoken word poetry?

The teaching of Spoken Word Poetry is important because it addresses students’ critical thinking, democratic engagement, and empowers their voices through verse. Spoken Word Poetry allows students to weave their primary discourse into their secondary discourse.

Why does all slam poetry sound the same?

It’s these two factors, the standardization imposed by competition and the tyranny of the Youtube algorithm, which make Slam Poet Voice so ubiquitous. As popular works in a genre (see above) are widely consumed, they become widely imitated, and those imitations get their own imitations.

Is haiku a form of literature?

haiku, unrhymed poetic form consisting of 17 syllables arranged in three lines of 5, 7, and 5 syllables respectively. The haiku first emerged in Japanese literature during the 17th century, as a terse reaction to elaborate poetic traditions, though it did not become known by the name haiku until the 19th century.

Does poetry have to rhyme?

There’s a common misconception that poems have to rhyme. … A lot of modern poetry doesn’t rhyme, and it still works just fine. If you force your poem to rhyme, the reader/listener will be able to tell. The important thing in poetry isn’t whether or not it rhymes, it’s whether or not it resonates.

How do you recite spoken word poetry?

Tips:

  1. Present yourself well and be attentive. Use good posture. Be confident and make a direct connection with the audience.
  2. Nervous gestures and lack of confidence will detract from your score.
  3. Relax and be natural. Enjoy your poem—the judges will notice.

What is the spoken word of God?

Rhema is the word which the Lord has spoken, and now He speaks it again.»

How do you analyze spoken words?

Check out these six ways to analyze a poem.

  1. Step One: Read. Have your students read the poem once to themselves and then aloud, all the way through, at LEAST twice. …
  2. Step Two: Title. Think about the title and how it relates to the poem. …
  3. Step Three: Speaker. …
  4. Step Four: Mood and Tone. …
  5. Step Five: Paraphrase. …
  6. Step Six: Theme.

Is rap just spoken word?

Rap music can be defined as music that consists of rapping, the same way a song contains singing. … A common difference between spoken word and rap is that rap has a beat while spoken word doesn’t.

Is rap singing or speaking?

Difference Between Rapping and Singing

Rapping is often described as speaking but it is much beyond that. Rapping is rhyming skills and making words rhyme to the given upbeat tune. Singing is often seen as the creation of musical sound with variations in pitch, rhythm, and beat.

Why is hip hop poetry?

Hip hop inspires attentive listeners to reconsider the pleasures and opportunities rhyme offers and it inspires a new generation to reintroduce conspicuous patterned rhyme back into our poetry.

Понравилась статья? Поделить с друзьями:
  • What is spanish word for food
  • What is range object in excel vba
  • What is spanish word for english
  • What is range name in excel
  • What is sorting in excel