The word stereotype means

An 18th-century Dutch engraving of the peoples of the world.

Police officers buying doughnuts and coffee, an example of perceived stereotypical behavior[1] in North America.

In social psychology, a stereotype is a generalized belief about a particular category of people.[2] It is an expectation that people might have about every person of a particular group. The type of expectation can vary; it can be, for example, an expectation about the group’s personality, preferences, appearance or ability. Stereotypes are sometimes overgeneralized, inaccurate, and resistant to new information, but can sometimes be accurate.[3]

While such generalizations about groups of people may be useful when making quick decisions, they may be erroneous when applied to particular individuals and are among the reasons for prejudicial attitudes.

Explicit stereotypes[edit]

An explicit stereotype refers to stereotypes that one is aware that one holds, and is aware that one is using to judge people. If person A is making judgments about a particular person B from a group G, and person A has an explicit stereotype for group G, their decision bias can be partially mitigated using conscious control; however, attempts to offset bias due to conscious awareness of a stereotype often fail at being truly impartial, due to either underestimating or overestimating the amount of bias being created by the stereotype.

Implicit stereotypes[edit]

Implicit stereotypes are those that lay on individuals’ subconsciousness, that they have no control or awareness of.[4]
“Implicit stereotypes are built based on two concepts, associative networks in semantic (knowledge) memory and automatic activation”. Implicit stereotypes are automatic and involuntary associations that people make between a social group and a domain or attribute. For example one can have beliefs that women and men are equally capable of becoming successful electricians but at the same time many can associate electricians more with men than women.[5]

In social psychology, a stereotype is any thought widely adopted about specific types of individuals or certain ways of behaving intended to represent the entire group of those individuals or behaviors as a whole.[6] These thoughts or beliefs may or may not accurately reflect reality.[7][8] Within psychology and across other disciplines, different conceptualizations and theories of stereotyping exist, at times sharing commonalities, as well as containing contradictory elements. Even in the social sciences and some sub-disciplines of psychology, stereotypes are occasionally reproduced and can be identified in certain theories, for example, in assumptions about other cultures.[9]

Etymology[edit]

The term stereotype comes from the French adjective stéréotype and derives from the Greek words στερεός (stereos), «firm, solid»[10] and τύπος (typos), impression,[11] hence «solid impression on one or more ideas/theories.»

The term was first used in the printing trade in 1798 by Firmin Didot, to describe a printing plate that duplicated any typography. The duplicate printing plate, or the stereotype, is used for printing instead of the original.

Outside of printing, the first reference to «stereotype» was in 1850, as a noun that meant image perpetuated without change.[12] However, it was not until 1922 that «stereotype» was first used in the modern psychological sense by American journalist Walter Lippmann in his work Public Opinion.[13]

Relationship with other types of intergroup attitudes[edit]

Stereotypes, prejudice, racism, and discrimination[14] are understood as related but different concepts.[15][16][17][18] Stereotypes are regarded as the most cognitive component and often occurs without conscious awareness, whereas prejudice is the affective component of stereotyping and discrimination is one of the behavioral components of prejudicial reactions.[15][16][19] In this tripartite view of intergroup attitudes, stereotypes reflect expectations and beliefs about the characteristics of members of groups perceived as different from one’s own, prejudice represents the emotional response, and discrimination refers to actions.[15][16]

Although related, the three concepts can exist independently of each other.[16][20] According to Daniel Katz and Kenneth Braly, stereotyping leads to racial prejudice when people emotionally react to the name of a group, ascribe characteristics to members of that group, and then evaluate those characteristics.[17]

Possible prejudicial effects of stereotypes[8] are:

  • Justification of ill-founded prejudices or ignorance
  • Unwillingness to rethink one’s attitudes and behavior
  • Preventing some people of stereotyped groups from entering or succeeding in activities or fields[21]

Content[edit]

Stereotype content model, adapted from Fiske et al. (2002): Four types of stereotypes resulting from combinations of perceived warmth and competence.

Stereotype content refers to the attributes that people think characterize a group. Studies of stereotype content examine what people think of others, rather than the reasons and mechanisms involved in stereotyping.[22]

Early theories of stereotype content proposed by social psychologists such as Gordon Allport assumed that stereotypes of outgroups reflected uniform antipathy.[23][24] For instance, Katz and Braly argued in their classic 1933 study that ethnic stereotypes were uniformly negative.[22]

By contrast, a newer model of stereotype content theorizes that stereotypes are frequently ambivalent and vary along two dimensions: warmth and competence. Warmth and competence are respectively predicted by lack of competition and status. Groups that do not compete with the in-group for the same resources (e.g., college space) are perceived as warm, whereas high-status (e.g., economically or educationally successful) groups are considered competent. The groups within each of the four combinations of high and low levels of warmth and competence elicit distinct emotions.[25] The model explains the phenomenon that some out-groups are admired but disliked, whereas others are liked but disrespected. This model was empirically tested on a variety of national and international samples and was found to reliably predict stereotype content.[23][26]

Functions[edit]

Early studies suggested that stereotypes were only used by rigid, repressed, and authoritarian people. This idea has been refuted by contemporary studies that suggest the ubiquity of stereotypes and it was suggested to regard stereotypes as collective group beliefs, meaning that people who belong to the same social group share the same set of stereotypes.[20] Modern research asserts that full understanding of stereotypes requires considering them from two complementary perspectives: as shared within a particular culture/subculture and as formed in the mind of an individual person.[27]

Relationship between cognitive and social functions[edit]

Stereotyping can serve cognitive functions on an interpersonal level, and social functions on an intergroup level.[8][20] For stereotyping to function on an intergroup level (see social identity approaches: social identity theory and self-categorization theory), an individual must see themselves as part of a group and being part of that group must also be salient for the individual.[20]

Craig McGarty, Russell Spears, and Vincent Y. Yzerbyt (2002) argued that the cognitive functions of stereotyping are best understood in relation to its social functions, and vice versa.[28]

Cognitive functions[edit]

Stereotypes can help make sense of the world. They are a form of categorization that helps to simplify and systematize information. Thus, information is more easily identified, recalled, predicted, and reacted to.[20] Stereotypes are categories of objects or people. Between stereotypes, objects or people are as different from each other as possible.[6] Within stereotypes, objects or people are as similar to each other as possible.[6]

Gordon Allport has suggested possible answers to why people find it easier to understand categorized information.[29] First, people can consult a category to identify response patterns. Second, categorized information is more specific than non-categorized information, as categorization accentuates properties that are shared by all members of a group. Third, people can readily describe objects in a category because objects in the same category have distinct characteristics. Finally, people can take for granted the characteristics of a particular category because the category itself may be an arbitrary grouping.

A complementary perspective theorizes how stereotypes function as time- and energy-savers that allow people to act more efficiently.[6] Yet another perspective suggests that stereotypes are people’s biased perceptions of their social contexts.[6] In this view, people use stereotypes as shortcuts to make sense of their social contexts, and this makes a person’s task of understanding his or her world less cognitively demanding.[6]

[edit]

[edit]

In the following situations, the overarching purpose of stereotyping is for people to put their collective self (their in-group membership) in a positive light:[30]

  • when stereotypes are used for explaining social events
  • when stereotypes are used for justifying activities of one’s own group (ingroup) to another group (outgroup)
  • when stereotypes are used for differentiating the ingroup as positively distinct from outgroups
Explanation purposes[edit]

An anti-semitic 1873 caricature depicting the stereotypical physical features of a Jewish male.

As mentioned previously, stereotypes can be used to explain social events.[20][30] Henri Tajfel[20] described his observations of how some people found that the anti-Semitic fabricated contents of The Protocols of the Elders of Zion only made sense if Jews have certain characteristics. Therefore, according to Tajfel,[20] Jews were stereotyped as being evil and yearning for world domination to match the anti-Semitic «facts» as presented in The Protocols of the Elders of Zion.

Justification purposes[edit]

People create stereotypes of an outgroup to justify the actions that their in-group has committed (or plans to commit) towards that outgroup.[20][29][30] For example, according to Tajfel,[20] Europeans stereotyped African, Indian, and Chinese people as being incapable of achieving financial advances without European help. This stereotype was used to justify European colonialism in Africa, India, and China.

Intergroup differentiation[edit]

An assumption is that people want their ingroup to have a positive image relative to outgroups, and so people want to differentiate their ingroup from relevant outgroups in a desirable way.[20] If an outgroup does not affect the ingroup’s image, then from an image preservation point of view, there is no point for the ingroup to be positively distinct from that outgroup.[20]

People can actively create certain images for relevant outgroups by stereotyping. People do so when they see that their ingroup is no longer as clearly and/or as positively differentiated from relevant outgroups, and they want to restore the intergroup differentiation to a state that favours the ingroup.[20][30]

Self-categorization[edit]

Stereotypes can emphasize a person’s group membership in two steps: Stereotypes emphasize the person’s similarities with ingroup members on relevant dimensions, and also the person’s differences from outgroup members on relevant dimensions.[24] People change the stereotype of their ingroups and outgroups to suit context.[24] Once an outgroup treats an ingroup member badly, they are more drawn to the members of their own group.[31] This can be seen as members within a group are able to relate to each other though a stereotype because of identical situations. A person can embrace a stereotype to avoid humiliation such as failing a task and blaming it on a stereotype.[32]

Social influence and consensus[edit]

Stereotypes are an indicator of ingroup consensus.[30] When there are intragroup disagreements over stereotypes of the ingroup and/or outgroups, ingroup members take collective action to prevent other ingroup members from diverging from each other.[30]

John C. Turner proposed in 1987[30] that if ingroup members disagree on an outgroup stereotype, then one of three possible collective actions follow: First, ingroup members may negotiate with each other and conclude that they have different outgroup stereotypes because they are stereotyping different subgroups of an outgroup (e.g., Russian gymnasts versus Russian boxers). Second, ingroup members may negotiate with each other, but conclude that they are disagreeing because of categorical differences amongst themselves. Accordingly, in this context, it is better to categorise ingroup members under different categories (e.g., Democrats versus Republican) than under a shared category (e.g., American). Finally, ingroup members may influence each other to arrive at a common outgroup stereotype.

Formation[edit]

Different disciplines give different accounts of how stereotypes develop: Psychologists may focus on an individual’s experience with groups, patterns of communication about those groups, and intergroup conflict. As for sociologists, they may focus on the relations among different groups in a social structure. They suggest that stereotypes are the result of conflict, poor parenting, and inadequate mental and emotional development. Once stereotypes have formed, there are two main factors that explain their persistence. First, the cognitive effects of schematic processing (see schema) make it so that when a member of a group behaves as we expect, the behavior confirms and even strengthens existing stereotypes. Second, the affective or emotional aspects of prejudice render logical arguments against stereotypes ineffective in countering the power of emotional responses.[33]

Correspondence bias[edit]

Correspondence bias refers to the tendency to ascribe a person’s behavior to disposition or personality, and to underestimate the extent to which situational factors elicited the behavior. Correspondence bias can play an important role in stereotype formation.[34]

For example, in a study by Roguer and Yzerbyt (1999) participants watched a video showing students who were randomly instructed to find arguments either for or against euthanasia. The students that argued in favor of euthanasia came from the same law department or from different departments. Results showed that participants attributed the students’ responses to their attitudes although it had been made clear in the video that students had no choice about their position. Participants reported that group membership, i.e., the department that the students belonged to, affected the students’ opinions about euthanasia. Law students were perceived to be more in favor of euthanasia than students from different departments despite the fact that a pretest had revealed that subjects had no preexisting expectations about attitudes toward euthanasia and the department that students belong to. The attribution error created the new stereotype that law students are more likely to support euthanasia.[35]

Nier et al. (2012) found that people who tend to draw dispositional inferences from behavior and ignore situational constraints are more likely to stereotype low-status groups as incompetent and high-status groups as competent. Participants listened to descriptions of two fictitious groups of Pacific Islanders, one of which was described as being higher in status than the other. In a second study, subjects rated actual groups – the poor and wealthy, women and men – in the United States in terms of their competence. Subjects who scored high on the measure of correspondence bias stereotyped the poor, women, and the fictitious lower-status Pacific Islanders as incompetent whereas they stereotyped the wealthy, men, and the high-status Pacific Islanders as competent. The correspondence bias was a significant predictor of stereotyping even after controlling for other measures that have been linked to beliefs about low status groups, the just-world hypothesis and social dominance orientation.[36]

Based on the anti-public sector bias,[37] Döring and Willems (2021)[38] found that employees in the public sector are considered as less professional compared to employees in the private sector. They build on the assumption that the red-tape and bureaucratic nature of the public sector spills over in the perception that citizens have about the employees working in the sector. With an experimental vignette study, they analyze how citizens process information on employees’ sector affiliation, and integrate non-work role-referencing to test the stereotype confirmation assumption underlying the representativeness heuristic. The results show that sector as well as non-work role-referencing influences perceived employee professionalism but has little effect on the confirmation of particular public sector stereotypes.[39] Moreover, the results do not confirm a congruity effect of consistent stereotypical information: non-work role-referencing does not aggravate the negative effect of sector affiliation on perceived employee professionalism.

Illusory correlation[edit]

Research has shown that stereotypes can develop based on a cognitive mechanism known as illusory correlation – an erroneous inference about the relationship between two events.[6][40][41] If two statistically infrequent events co-occur, observers overestimate the frequency of co-occurrence of these events. The underlying reason is that rare, infrequent events are distinctive and salient and, when paired, become even more so. The heightened salience results in more attention and more effective encoding, which strengthens the belief that the events are correlated.[42][43][44]

In the intergroup context, illusory correlations lead people to misattribute rare behaviors or traits at higher rates to minority group members than to majority groups, even when both display the same proportion of the behaviors or traits. Black people, for instance, are a minority group in the United States and interaction with blacks is a relatively infrequent event for an average white American.[45] Similarly, undesirable behavior (e.g. crime) is statistically less frequent than desirable behavior. Since both events «blackness» and «undesirable behavior» are distinctive in the sense that they are infrequent, the combination of the two leads observers to overestimate the rate of co-occurrence.[42] Similarly, in workplaces where women are underrepresented and negative behaviors such as errors occur less frequently than positive behaviors, women become more strongly associated with mistakes than men.[46]

In a landmark study, David Hamilton and Richard Gifford (1976) examined the role of illusory correlation in stereotype formation. Subjects were instructed to read descriptions of behaviors performed by members of groups A and B. Negative behaviors outnumbered positive actions and group B was smaller than group A, making negative behaviors and membership in group B relatively infrequent and distinctive. Participants were then asked who had performed a set of actions: a person of group A or group B. Results showed that subjects overestimated the frequency with which both distinctive events, membership in group B and negative behavior, co-occurred, and evaluated group B more negatively. This despite the fact the proportion of positive to negative behaviors was equivalent for both groups and that there was no actual correlation between group membership and behaviors.[42] Although Hamilton and Gifford found a similar effect for positive behaviors as the infrequent events, a meta-analytic review of studies showed that illusory correlation effects are stronger when the infrequent, distinctive information is negative.[40]

Hamilton and Gifford’s distinctiveness-based explanation of stereotype formation was subsequently extended.[43] A 1994 study by McConnell, Sherman, and Hamilton found that people formed stereotypes based on information that was not distinctive at the time of presentation, but was considered distinctive at the time of judgement.[47] Once a person judges non-distinctive information in memory to be distinctive, that information is re-encoded and re-represented as if it had been distinctive when it was first processed.[47]

Common environment[edit]

One explanation for why stereotypes are shared is that they are the result of a common environment that stimulates people to react in the same way.[6]

The problem with the ‘common environment’ is that explanation in general is that it does not explain how shared stereotypes can occur without direct stimuli.[6] Research since the 1930s suggested that people are highly similar with each other in how they describe different racial and national groups, although those people have no personal experience with the groups they are describing.[48]

Socialization and upbringing[edit]

Another explanation says that people are socialised to adopt the same stereotypes.[6] Some psychologists believe that although stereotypes can be absorbed at any age, stereotypes are usually acquired in early childhood under the influence of parents, teachers, peers, and the media.

If stereotypes are defined by social values, then stereotypes only change as per changes in social values.[6] The suggestion that stereotype content depends on social values reflects Walter Lippman’s argument in his 1922 publication that stereotypes are rigid because they cannot be changed at will.[17]

Studies emerging since the 1940s refuted the suggestion that stereotype contents cannot be changed at will. Those studies suggested that one group’s stereotype of another group would become more or less positive depending on whether their intergroup relationship had improved or degraded.[17][49][50] Intergroup events (e.g., World War II, Persian Gulf conflicts) often changed intergroup relationships. For example, after WWII, Black American students held a more negative stereotype of people from countries that were the United States’s WWII enemies.[17] If there are no changes to an intergroup relationship, then relevant stereotypes do not change.[18]

Intergroup relations[edit]

According to a third explanation, shared stereotypes are neither caused by the coincidence of common stimuli, nor by socialisation. This explanation posits that stereotypes are shared because group members are motivated to behave in certain ways, and stereotypes reflect those behaviours.[6] It is important to note from this explanation that stereotypes are the consequence, not the cause, of intergroup relations. This explanation assumes that when it is important for people to acknowledge both their ingroup and outgroup, they will emphasise their difference from outgroup members, and their similarity to ingroup members.[6] International migration creates more opportunities for intergroup relations, but the interactions do not always disconfirm stereotypes. They are also known to form and maintain them.[51]

Activation[edit]

The dual-process model of cognitive processing of stereotypes asserts that automatic activation of stereotypes is followed by a controlled processing stage, during which an individual may choose to disregard or ignore the stereotyped information that has been brought to mind.[19]

A number of studies have found that stereotypes are activated automatically. Patricia Devine (1989), for example, suggested that stereotypes are automatically activated in the presence of a member (or some symbolic equivalent) of a stereotyped group and that the unintentional activation of the stereotype is equally strong for high- and low-prejudice persons. Words related to the cultural stereotype of blacks were presented subliminally. During an ostensibly unrelated impression-formation task, subjects read a paragraph describing a race-unspecified target person’s behaviors and rated the target person on several trait scales. Results showed that participants who received a high proportion of racial words rated the target person in the story as significantly more hostile than participants who were presented with a lower proportion of words related to the stereotype. This effect held true for both high- and low-prejudice subjects (as measured by the Modern Racism Scale). Thus, the racial stereotype was activated even for low-prejudice individuals who did not personally endorse it.[19][52][53] Studies using alternative priming methods have shown that the activation of gender and age stereotypes can also be automatic.[54][55]

Subsequent research suggested that the relation between category activation and stereotype activation was more complex.[53][56] Lepore and Brown (1997), for instance, noted that the words used in Devine’s study were both neutral category labels (e.g., «Blacks») and stereotypic attributes (e.g., «lazy»). They argued that if only the neutral category labels were presented, people high and low in prejudice would respond differently. In a design similar to Devine’s, Lepore and Brown primed the category of African-Americans using labels such as «blacks» and «West Indians» and then assessed the differential activation of the associated stereotype in the subsequent impression-formation task. They found that high-prejudice participants increased their ratings of the target person on the negative stereotypic dimensions and decreased them on the positive dimension whereas low-prejudice subjects tended in the opposite direction. The results suggest that the level of prejudice and stereotype endorsement affects people’s judgements when the category – and not the stereotype per se – is primed.[57]

Research has shown that people can be trained to activate counterstereotypic information and thereby reduce the automatic activation of negative stereotypes. In a study by Kawakami et al. (2000), for example, participants were presented with a category label and taught to respond «No» to stereotypic traits and «Yes» to nonstereotypic traits. After this training period, subjects showed reduced stereotype activation.[58][59] This effect is based on the learning of new and more positive stereotypes rather than the negation of already existing ones.[59]

Automatic behavioral outcomes[edit]

Empirical evidence suggests that stereotype activation can automatically influence social behavior.[60][61][62][63] For example, Bargh, Chen, and Burrows (1996) activated the stereotype of the elderly among half of their participants by administering a scrambled-sentence test where participants saw words related to age stereotypes. Subjects primed with the stereotype walked significantly more slowly than the control group (although the test did not include any words specifically referring to slowness), thus acting in a way that the stereotype suggests that elderly people will act. And the stereotype of the elder will affect the subjective perception of them through depression.[64] In another experiment, Bargh, Chen, and Burrows also found that because the stereotype about blacks includes the notion of aggression, subliminal exposure to black faces increased the likelihood that randomly selected white college students reacted with more aggression and hostility than participants who subconsciously viewed a white face.[65] Similarly, Correll et al. (2002) showed that activated stereotypes about blacks can influence people’s behavior. In a series of experiments, black and white participants played a video game, in which a black or white person was shown holding a gun or a harmless object (e.g., a mobile phone). Participants had to decide as quickly as possible whether to shoot the target. When the target person was armed, both black and white participants were faster in deciding to shoot the target when he was black than when he was white. When the target was unarmed, the participants avoided shooting him more quickly when he was white. Time pressure made the shooter bias even more pronounced.[66]

Accuracy[edit]

A magazine feature from Beauty Parade from March 1952 stereotyping women drivers. It features Bettie Page as the model.

Stereotypes can be efficient shortcuts and sense-making tools. They can, however, keep people from processing new or unexpected information about each individual, thus biasing the impression formation process.[6] Early researchers believed that stereotypes were inaccurate representations of reality.[48] A series of pioneering studies in the 1930s found no empirical support for widely held racial stereotypes.[17] By the mid-1950s, Gordon Allport wrote that, «It is possible for a stereotype to grow in defiance of all evidence.»[29]

Research on the role of illusory correlations in the formation of stereotypes suggests that stereotypes can develop because of incorrect inferences about the relationship between two events (e.g., membership in a social group and bad or good attributes). This means that at least some stereotypes are inaccurate.[40][42][44][47]

Empirical social science research shows that stereotypes are often accurate.[67][68] Jussim et al. reviewed four studies of racial stereotypes, and seven studies of gender stereotypes regarding demographic characteristics, academic achievement, personality and behavior. Based on that, the authors argued that some aspects of ethnic and gender stereotypes are accurate while stereotypes concerning political affiliation and nationality are much less accurate.[69] A study by Terracciano et al. also found that stereotypic beliefs about nationality do not reflect the actual personality traits of people from different cultures.[70]

Marlene MacKie argues that while stereotypes are inaccurate, this is a definition rather than empirical claim – stereotypes were simply defined as inaccurate, even though the supposed inaccuracy of stereotypes was treated as though it was an empirical discovery.[71]

Effects[edit]

Attributional ambiguity[edit]

Attributive ambiguity refers to the uncertainty that members of stereotyped groups experience in interpreting the causes of others’ behavior toward them. Stereotyped individuals who receive negative feedback can attribute it either to personal shortcomings, such as lack of ability or poor effort, or the evaluator’s stereotypes and prejudice toward their social group. Alternatively, positive feedback can either be attributed to personal merit or discounted as a form of sympathy or pity.[72][73][74]

Crocker et al. (1991) showed that when black participants were evaluated by a white person who was aware of their race, black subjects mistrusted the feedback, attributing negative feedback to the evaluator’s stereotypes and positive feedback to the evaluator’s desire to appear unbiased. When the black participants’ race was unknown to the evaluator, they were more accepting of the feedback.[75]

Attributional ambiguity has been shown to affect a person’s self-esteem. When they receive positive evaluations, stereotyped individuals are uncertain of whether they really deserved their success and, consequently, they find it difficult to take credit for their achievements. In the case of negative feedback, ambiguity has been shown to have a protective effect on self-esteem as it allows people to assign blame to external causes. Some studies, however, have found that this effect only holds when stereotyped individuals can be absolutely certain that their negative outcomes are due to the evaluators’s prejudice. If any room for uncertainty remains, stereotyped individuals tend to blame themselves.[73]

Attributional ambiguity can also make it difficult to assess one’s skills because performance-related evaluations are mistrusted or discounted. Moreover, it can lead to the belief that one’s efforts are not directly linked to the outcomes, thereby depressing one’s motivation to succeed.[72]

Stereotype threat[edit]

The effect of stereotype threat (ST) on math test scores for girls and boys. Data from Osborne (2007).[76]

Stereotype threat occurs when people are aware of a negative stereotype about their social group and experience anxiety or concern that they might confirm the stereotype.[77] Stereotype threat has been shown to undermine performance in a variety of domains.[78][79]

Claude M. Steele and Joshua Aronson conducted the first experiments showing that stereotype threat can depress intellectual performance on standardized tests. In one study, they found that black college students performed worse than white students on a verbal test when the task was framed as a measure of intelligence. When it was not presented in that manner, the performance gap narrowed. Subsequent experiments showed that framing the test as diagnostic of intellectual ability made black students more aware of negative stereotypes about their group, which in turn impaired their performance.[80] Stereotype threat effects have been demonstrated for an array of social groups in many different arenas, including not only academics but also sports,[81] chess[82] and business.[83]

Not only has stereotype threat been widely criticized by on a theoretical basis,[84][85] but has failed several attempts to replicate its experimental evidence.[85][86][87][88] The findings in support of the concept have been suggested by multiple methodological reviews to be the product of publication bias.[88][89]

Self-fulfilling prophecy[edit]

Stereotypes lead people to expect certain actions from members of social groups. These stereotype-based expectations may lead to self-fulfilling prophecies, in which one’s inaccurate expectations about a person’s behavior, through social interaction, prompt that person to act in stereotype-consistent ways, thus confirming one’s erroneous expectations and validating the stereotype.[90][91][92]

Word, Zanna, and Cooper (1974) demonstrated the effects of stereotypes in the context of a job interview. White participants interviewed black and white subjects who, prior to the experiments, had been trained to act in a standardized manner. Analysis of the videotaped interviews showed that black job applicants were treated differently: They received shorter amounts of interview time and less eye contact; interviewers made more speech errors (e.g., stutters, sentence incompletions, incoherent sounds) and physically distanced themselves from black applicants. In a second experiment, trained interviewers were instructed to treat applicants, all of whom were white, like the whites or blacks had been treated in the first experiment. As a result, applicants treated like the blacks of the first experiment behaved in a more nervous manner and received more negative performance ratings than interviewees receiving the treatment previously afforded to whites.[93]

A 1977 study by Snyder, Tanke, and Berscheid found a similar pattern in social interactions between men and women. Male undergraduate students were asked to talk to female undergraduates, whom they believed to be physically attractive or unattractive, on the phone. The conversations were taped and analysis showed that men who thought that they were talking to an attractive woman communicated in a more positive and friendlier manner than men who believed that they were talking to unattractive women. This altered the women’s behavior: Female subjects who, unknowingly to them, were perceived to be physically attractive behaved in a friendly, likeable, and sociable manner in comparison with subjects who were regarded as unattractive.[94]

A 2005 study by J. Thomas Kellow and Brett D. Jones looked at the effects of self-fulfilling prophecy on African American and Caucasian high school freshman students. Both white and black students were informed that their test performance would be predictive of their performance on a statewide, high stakes standardized test. They were also told that historically, white students had outperformed black students on the test. This knowledge created a self-fulfilling prophecy in both the white and black students, where the white students scored statistically significantly higher than the African American students on the test. The stereotype threat of underperforming on standardized tests affected the African American students in this study.[95]

In accountancy, there is a popular stereotype which represents members of the profession as being humorless, introspective beancounters.[96][97]

Discrimination and prejudice[edit]

Because stereotypes simplify and justify social reality, they have potentially powerful effects on how people perceive and treat one another.[98] As a result, stereotypes can lead to discrimination in labor markets and other domains.[99] For example, Tilcsik (2011) has found that employers who seek job applicants with stereotypically male heterosexual traits are particularly likely to engage in discrimination against gay men, suggesting that discrimination on the basis of sexual orientation is partly rooted in specific stereotypes and that these stereotypes loom large in many labor markets.[21] Agerström and Rooth (2011) showed that automatic obesity stereotypes captured by the Implicit Association Test can predict real hiring discrimination against the obese.[100] Similarly, experiments suggest that gender stereotypes play an important role in judgments that affect hiring decisions.[101][102]

Stereotypes can cause racist prejudice. For example, scientists and activists have warned that the use of the stereotype «Nigerian Prince» for referring to Advance-fee scammers is racist, i.e. «reducing Nigeria to a nation of scammers and fraudulent princes, as some people still do online, is a stereotype that needs to be called out».[103]

Self-stereotyping[edit]

Stereotypes can affect self-evaluations and lead to self-stereotyping.[8][104] For instance, Correll (2001, 2004) found that specific stereotypes (e.g., the stereotype that women have lower mathematical ability) affect women’s and men’s evaluations of their abilities (e.g., in math and science), such that men assess their own task ability higher than women performing at the same level.[105][106] Similarly, a study by Sinclair et al. (2006) has shown that Asian American women rated their math ability more favorably when their ethnicity and the relevant stereotype that Asian Americans excel in math was made salient. In contrast, they rated their math ability less favorably when their gender and the corresponding stereotype of women’s inferior math skills was made salient. Sinclair et al. found, however, that the effect of stereotypes on self-evaluations is mediated by the degree to which close people in someone’s life endorse these stereotypes. People’s self-stereotyping can increase or decrease depending on whether close others view them in stereotype-consistent or inconsistent manner.[107]

Stereotyping can also play a central role in depression, when people have negative self-stereotypes about themselves, according to Cox, Abramson, Devine, and Hollon (2012).[8] This depression that is caused by prejudice (i.e., «deprejudice») can be related to group membership (e.g., Me–Gay–Bad) or not (e.g., Me–Bad). If someone holds prejudicial beliefs about a stigmatized group and then becomes a member of that group, they may internalize their prejudice and develop depression. People may also show prejudice internalization through self-stereotyping because of negative childhood experiences such as verbal and physical abuse.[108]

Substitute for observations[edit]

Stereotypes are traditional and familiar symbol clusters, expressing a more or less complex idea in a convenient way. They are often simplistic pronouncements about gender, racial, ethnic, and cultural backgrounds and they can become a source of misinformation and delusion. For example, in a school when students are confronted with the task of writing a theme, they think in terms of literary associations, often using stereotypes picked up from books, films, and magazines that they have read or viewed.

The danger in stereotyping lies not in its existence, but in the fact that it can become a substitute for observation and a misinterpretation of a cultural identity.[109] Promoting information literacy is a pedagogical approach that can effectively combat the entrenchment of stereotypes. The necessity for using information literacy to separate multicultural «fact from fiction» is well illustrated with examples from literature and media.[110]

Role in art and culture[edit]

American political cartoon titled The Usual Irish Way of Doing Things, depicting a drunken Irishman lighting a powder keg and swinging a bottle. Published in Harper’s Weekly, 1871.

Stereotypes are common in various cultural media, where they take the form of dramatic stock characters. The instantly recognizable nature of stereotypes mean that they are effective in advertising and situation comedy.[111] Alexander Fedorov (2015) proposed a concept of media stereotypes analysis. This concept refers to identification and analysis of stereotypical images of people, ideas, events, stories, themes, etc. in media context.[112]

The characters that do appear in movies greatly affect how people worldwide perceive gender relations, race, and cultural communities. Because approximately 85% of worldwide ticket sales are directed toward Hollywood movies, the American movie industry has been greatly responsible for portraying characters of different cultures and diversity to fit into stereotypical categories.[113] This has led to the spread and persistence of gender, racial, ethnic, and cultural stereotypes seen in the movies.[89]

For example, Russians are usually portrayed as ruthless agents, brutal mobsters and villains in Hollywood movies.[114][115][116] According to Russian American professor Nina L. Khrushcheva, «You can’t even turn the TV on and go to the movies without reference to Russians as horrible.»[117] The portrayals of Latin Americans in film and print media are restricted to a narrow set of characters. Latin Americans are largely depicted as sexualized figures such as the Latino macho or the Latina vixen, gang members, (illegal) immigrants, or entertainers. By comparison, they are rarely portrayed as working professionals, business leaders or politicians.[101]

In Hollywood films, there are several Latin American stereotypes that have historically been used. Some examples are El Bandido, the Halfbreed Harlot, The Male Buffoon, The Female Clown, The Latin Lover, The Dark Lady, The Wise Old Man, and The Poor Peon. Many Hispanic characters in Hollywood films consists of one or more of these basic stereotypes, but it has been rare to view Latin American actors representing characters outside of this stereotypical criteria.[118]

Media stereotypes of women first emerged in the early 20th century. Various stereotypic depictions or «types» of women appeared in magazines, including Victorian ideals of femininity, the New Woman, the Gibson Girl, the Femme fatale, and the Flapper.[88][119]

Stereotypes are also common in video games, with women being portrayed as stereotypes such as the «damsel in distress» or as sexual objects (see Gender representation in video games).[120] Studies show that minorities are portrayed most often in stereotypical roles such as athletes and gangsters (see Racial representations in video games).[121]

In literature and art, stereotypes are clichéd or predictable characters or situations. Throughout history, storytellers have drawn from stereotypical characters and situations to immediately connect the audience with new tales.[122]

See also[edit]

  • Archetype
  • Attribute substitution
  • Attribution bias
  • Base rate fallacy
  • Cognitive bias
  • Conjunction fallacy (Linda problem)
  • Counterstereotype (antonym)
  • Echo chamber (media)
  • Ethnocentrism
  • Face-ism
  • Filter Bubble
  • Habitus (sociology)
  • Implicit stereotype
  • In-group favoritism
  • Labeling theory
  • Negativity effect
  • Out-group homogeneity
  • Role
  • Role reversal
  • Scapegoating
  • Similarity (philosophy)
  • Statistical syllogism
  • Stigma management
  • Stigmatization
  • Trait ascription bias
  • Women-are-wonderful effect
Gender
  • Gender stereotypes
  • Femininity
  • Masculinity

Examples of stereotypes[edit]

Cultural and ethnic
  • Ethnic stereotype
  • List of ethnic slurs
  • Stereotypes of African Americans
  • Stereotypes of Africans
  • Stereotypes of Americans
  • Stereotypes of Arabs and Muslims in the United States
  • Stereotypes of Argentines
  • Stereotypes of the British
  • Stereotypes of Canadians
  • Stereotypes of French people
  • Stereotypes of Germans
  • Stereotypes of groups within the United States
  • Stereotypes of Hispanic and Latino Americans in the United States
  • Stereotypes of Indigenous peoples of Canada and the United States
  • Stereotypes of Japanese people
  • Stereotypes of Jews
  • Stereotypes of Nigerians
  • Stereotypes of Russians
  • Stereotypes of South Asians
  • Stereotypes of East and Southeast Asians
Sexuality related
  • LGBT stereotypes
  • List of sexuality related phobias
Other
  • Blonde stereotypes
  • Nurse stereotypes
  • Physical attractiveness stereotype

References[edit]

  1. ^ Critchfield, Austi (15 November 2017). «This is Why Doughnuts Are Associated With Cops». Spoon University. Archived from the original on 19 November 2017. Retrieved 2 November 2020. The funny thing is, the whole cop and doughnuts thing is completely out of date — today, an officer could just as easily swing through a McDonald’s drive through as he could a Krispy Kreme. Yet, the stereotype endures, even though police aren’t seen at doughnut shops in nearly the numbers they used to have been. In a way, it’s become a stereotype of itself, which is pretty meta.
  2. ^ Cardwell, Mike (1999). Dictionary of psychology. Chicago Fitzroy Dearborn. ISBN 978-1579580643.
  3. ^ Myers, David G. (2013). Social psychology. Twenge, Jean M., 1971- (11th ed.). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill. ISBN 978-0-07-803529-6. OCLC 795645100.[page needed]
  4. ^ «Frequently Asked Questions». implicit.harvard.edu. Retrieved 14 April 2018.
  5. ^ Hinton, Perry (1 September 2017). «Implicit stereotypes and the predictive brain: cognition and culture in «biased» person perception». Palgrave Communications. 3 (1): 1–9. doi:10.1057/palcomms.2017.86. S2CID 54036730.
  6. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n McGarty, Craig; Yzerbyt, Vincent Y.; Spears, Russel (2002). «Social, cultural and cognitive factors in stereotype formation» (PDF). Stereotypes as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 1–15. ISBN 978-0-521-80047-1.
  7. ^ Judd, Charles M.; Park, Bernadette (1993). «Definition and assessment of accuracy in social stereotypes». Psychological Review. 100 (1): 109–128. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.100.1.109. PMID 8426877.
  8. ^ a b c d e Cox, William T. L.; Abramson, Lyn Y.; Devine, Patricia G.; Hollon, Steven D. (September 2012). «Stereotypes, Prejudice, and Depression: The Integrated Perspective». Perspectives on Psychological Science. 7 (5): 427–449. doi:10.1177/1745691612455204. PMID 26168502. S2CID 1512121.
  9. ^ Chakkarath, Pradeep (March 2010). «Stereotypes in social psychology: The «West-East» differentiation as a reflection of western traditions of thought». Psychological Studies. 55 (1): 18–25. doi:10.1007/s12646-010-0002-9. S2CID 144061506.
  10. ^ στερεός,
    Henry George Liddell, Robert Scott, A Greek-English Lexicon, on Perseus Digital Library
  11. ^ τύπος, Henry George Liddell, Robert Scott, A Greek-English Lexicon, on Perseus Digital Library
  12. ^ «stereotype – Origin and meaning of stereotype by Online Etymology Dictionary». etymonline.com. Retrieved 22 October 2017.
  13. ^ Kleg, Milton (1993). Hate Prejudice and Racism. Albany: State University of New York Press. pp. 135–137. ISBN 978-0-585-05491-9.
  14. ^ Vega, Tanzina. «Working while brown: What discrimination looks like now». CNNMoney. Retrieved 26 March 2018.
  15. ^ a b c Fiske, Susan T. (1998). «Stereotyping, Prejudice, and Discrimination». In Gilbert, Daniel T.; Fiske, Susan T.; Lindzey, Gardner (eds.). The Handbook of Social Psychology. Vol. Two (4th ed.). Boston, Mass.: McGraw-Hill. p. 357. ISBN 978-0-19-521376-8.
  16. ^ a b c d Denmark, Florence L. (2010). «Prejudice and Discrimination». In Weiner, Irving B.; Craigheaid, W. Edward (eds.). The Corsini Encyclopedia of Psychology. Vol. Three (4th ed.). Hoboken, N.J.: John Wiley. p. 1277. ISBN 978-0-470-47921-6.
  17. ^ a b c d e f Katz, Daniel; Braly, Kenneth W. (1935). «Racial prejudice and racial stereotypes». The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 30 (2): 175–193. doi:10.1037/h0059800.
  18. ^ a b Oakes, P. J., Haslam, S. A., & Turner, J. C. (1994). Stereotyping and social reality. Oxford: Blackwell.
  19. ^ a b c Devine, Patricia G. (1989). «Stereotypes and Prejudice: Their Automatic and Controlled Components». Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 56 (1): 5–18. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.56.1.5.
  20. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m Tajfel, Henri (1981). «Social stereotypes and social groups». In Turner, John C.; Giles, Howard (eds.). Intergroup behaviour. Oxford: Blackwell. pp. 144–167. ISBN 978-0-631-11711-7.
  21. ^ a b Tilcsik, András (2011). «Pride and Prejudice: Employment Discrimination against Openly Gay Men in the United States». American Journal of Sociology. 117 (2): 586–626. doi:10.1086/661653. hdl:1807/34998. PMID 22268247. S2CID 23542996.
  22. ^ a b Operario, Don; Fiske, Susan T. (2003). «Stereotypes: Content, Structures, Processes, and Context». In Brown, Rupert; Gaertner, Samuel L (eds.). Blackwell Handbook of Social Psychology: Intergroup Processes. Malden, MA: Blackwell. pp. 22–44. ISBN 978-1-4051-0654-2.
  23. ^ a b Fiske, Susan T.; Cuddy, Amy J. C.; Glick, Peter; Xu, Jun (2002). «A Model of (Often Mixed) Stereotype Content: Competence and Warmth Respectively Follow From Perceived Status and Competition» (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 82 (6): 878–902. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.320.4001. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.82.6.878. PMID 12051578.
  24. ^ a b c Cuddy, Amy J. C.; Fiske, Susan T. (2002). «Doddering But Dear: Process, Content, and Function in Stereotyping of Older Persons». In Nelson, Todd D (ed.). Ageism: Stereotyping and Prejudice against Older Persons. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press. pp. 7–8. ISBN 978-0-262-14077-5.
  25. ^ Dovidio, John F.; Gaertner, Samuel L. (2010). «Intergroup Bias». In Susan T., Fiske; Gilbert, Daniel T.; Lindzey, Gardner (eds.). Handbook of Social Psychology. Vol. Two (5th ed.). Hooboken, N.J.: John Wiley. p. 1085. ISBN 978-0-470-13747-5.
  26. ^ Cuddy, Amy J. C.; Fiske, Susan T.; Kwan, Virginia S. Y.; Glick, Peter; Demoulin, Stéphanie; Leyens, Jacques-Philippe; Bond, Michael Harris; Croizet, Jean-Claude; Ellemers, Naomi; Sleebos, Ed; Htun, Tin Tin; Kim, Hyun-Jeong; Maio, Greg; Perry, Judi; Petkova, Kristina; Todorov, Valery; Rodríguez-Bailón, Rosa; Morales, Elena; Moya, Miguel; Palacios, Marisol; Smith, Vanessa; Perez, Rolando; Vala, Jorge; Ziegler, Rene (March 2009). «Stereotype content model across cultures: Towards universal similarities and some differences». British Journal of Social Psychology. 48 (1): 1–33. doi:10.1348/014466608X314935. PMC 3912751. PMID 19178758.
  27. ^ Macrae CN, Stangor C, Hewstone M. (eds.) «Stereotypes and stereotyping.» 1995, p. 4
  28. ^ McGarty, Craig; Spears, Russel; Yzerbyt, Vincent Y. (2002). «Conclusion: stereotypes are selective, variable and contested explanations». Stereotypes as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 186–199. ISBN 978-0-521-80047-1.
  29. ^ a b c Allport, Gordon W. (1954). The Nature of Prejudice. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley. p. 189. ISBN 978-0-201-00175-4.
  30. ^ a b c d e f g Haslam, S. Alexander; Turner, John C.; Oakes, Penelope J.; Reynolds, Katherine J.; Doosje, Bertjan (2002). «From personal pictures in the head to collective tools in the world: How shared stereotypes allow groups to represent and change social reality». Stereotypes as Explanations. pp. 157–185. doi:10.1017/CBO9780511489877.009. ISBN 978-0-521-80482-0.
  31. ^ Cadinu, Mara; Latrofa, Marcella; Carnaghi, Andrea (2013). «Comparing Self-stereotyping with In-group-stereotyping and Out-group-stereotyping in Unequal-status Groups: The Case of Gender». Self and Identity. 12 (6): 582–596. doi:10.1080/15298868.2012.712753. S2CID 145053003.
  32. ^ Burkley, Melissa; Blanton, Hart (2008). «Endorsing a negative in-group stereotype as a self-protective strategy: Sacrificing the group to save the self». Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 44: 37–49. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2007.01.008.
  33. ^ Aronson, E., Wilson, T. D., & Akert, R. M. (2010). Social Psychology (7th edition). New York: Pearson.
  34. ^ Mackie, Diane M.; Hamilton, David L.; Susskind, Joshua; Rosselli, Francine (1996). «Social Psychological Foundations of Stereotype Formation». In MacRae, C. Neil; Stangor, Charles; Hewstone, Miles (eds.). Stereotypes and Stereotyping. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 48–49. ISBN 978-1-57230-053-8.
  35. ^ Rogier, Anouk; Yzerbyt, Vincent (1999). «Social attribution, correspondence bias, and the emergence of stereotypes» (PDF). Swiss Journal of Psychology. 58 (4): 233–240. doi:10.1024//1421-0185.58.4.233. Archived from the original (PDF) on 29 July 2013. Retrieved 5 April 2013.
  36. ^ Nier, Jason A.; Bajaj, Priya; McLean, Meghan C.; Schwartz, Elizabeth (2012). «Group status, perceptions of agency, and the correspondence bias: Attributional processes in the formation of stereotypes about high and low status groups». Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 16 (4): 1–12. doi:10.1177/1368430212454925. S2CID 145399213.
  37. ^ Marvel, John D. (30 January 2015). «Unconscious Bias in Citizens’ Evaluations of Public Sector Performance». Journal of Public Administration Research and Theory: muu053. doi:10.1093/jopart/muu053. ISSN 1053-1858.
  38. ^ Döring, Matthias; Willems, Jurgen (22 September 2021). «Processing stereotypes: professionalism confirmed or disconfirmed by sector affiliation?». International Public Management Journal: 1–19. doi:10.1080/10967494.2021.1971125. ISSN 1096-7494. S2CID 239261451.
  39. ^ Willems, Jurgen (2020). «Public servant stereotypes: It is not (at) all about being lazy, greedy and corrupt». Public Administration. 98 (4): 807–823. doi:10.1111/padm.12686. ISSN 1467-9299. S2CID 221760839.
  40. ^ a b c Mullen, Brian; Johnson, Craig (1990). «Distinctiveness-based illusory correlations and stereotyping: A meta-analytic integration». British Journal of Social Psychology. 29 (1): 11–28. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8309.1990.tb00883.x.
  41. ^ Meiser, Thorsten (2006). «Contingency Learning and Biased Group Impressions». In Fiedler, Klaus; Justin, Peter (eds.). Information Sampling and Adaptive Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 183–209. ISBN 978-0-521-83159-8.
  42. ^ a b c d Hamilton, David L.; Gifford, Robert K. (1976). «Illusory correlation in interpersonal perception: A cognitive basis of stereotypic judgments». Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 12 (4): 392–407. doi:10.1016/S0022-1031(76)80006-6.
  43. ^ a b Berndsen, Mariëtte; Spears, Russel; van der Pligt, Joop; McGarty, Craig (2002). «Illusory correlation and stereotype formation: making sense of group differences and cognitive biases». In McGarty, Craig; Yzerbyt, Vincent Y.; Spears, Russel (eds.). Stereotypes as explanations: The formation of meaningful beliefs about social groups. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 90–110. ISBN 978-0-521-80047-1.
  44. ^ a b Stroessner, Steven J.; Plaks, Jason E. (2001). «Illusory Correlation and Stereotype Formation: Tracing the Arc of Research Over a Quarter Century». In Moskowitz, Gordon B. (ed.). Cognitive Social Psychology: The Princeton Symposium on the Legacy and Future of Social Cognition. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 247–259. ISBN 978-0-8058-3414-7.
  45. ^ Ajrouch, Kristine J.; Antonucci, Toni C.; Janevic, Mary R. (1 March 2001). «Social Networks Among Blacks and Whites: The Interaction Between Race and Age». The Journals of Gerontology. 56 (2): S112–S118. doi:10.1093/geronb/56.2.S112. PMID 11245365.
  46. ^ Moskowitz, Gordon B. (2005). Social Cognition: Understanding Self and Others. New York: Guilford Press. p. 182. ISBN 978-1-59385-085-2.
  47. ^ a b c McConnell, Allen R.; Sherman, Steven J.; Hamilton, David L. (1994). «Illusory correlation in the perception of groups: an extension of the distinctiveness-based account» (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 67 (3): 414–429. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.67.3.414. PMID 7965600.
  48. ^ a b Katz, Daniel; Braley, Kenneth (1933). «Racial stereotypes of one hundred college students». The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 28 (3): 280–290. doi:10.1037/h0074049.
  49. ^ Meenes, Max (1943). «A Comparison of Racial Stereotypes of 1935 and 1942». Journal of Social Psychology. 17 (2): 327–336. doi:10.1080/00224545.1943.9712287.
  50. ^ Haslam, S. Alexander; Turner, John C.; Oakes, Penelope J.; McGarty, Craig; Hayes, Brett K. (1992). «Context-dependent variation in social stereotyping 1: The effects of intergroup relations as mediated by social change and frame of reference». European Journal of Social Psychology. 22 (1): 3–20. doi:10.1002/ejsp.2420220104.
  51. ^ Palmer, Wayne (2019). «International Migration and Stereotype Formation: Indonesian Migrants in Hong Kong». Journal of International Migration and Integration. 21 (3): 731–744. doi:10.1007/s12134-019-00680-1. ISSN 1488-3473. S2CID 195363006.
  52. ^ Devine, Patricia G.; Monteith, Margo J. (1999). «Automaticty and Control in Stereotyping». In Chaiken, Shelly; Trope, Yaacov (eds.). Dual-Process Theories in Social Psychology. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 341–342. ISBN 978-1-57230-421-5.
  53. ^ a b Bargh, John A. (1994). «The Four Horsemen of Automaticity: Awareness, Intention, Efficiency, Control in Social Cognition». In Wyer, Robert S.; Srull, Thomas K. (eds.). Handbook of Social Cognition. Vol. Two (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum. p. 21. ISBN 978-0-8058-1056-1.
  54. ^ Banaji, Mahzarin R.; Hardin, Curtis D. (1996). «Automatic Stereotyping» (PDF). Psychological Science. 7 (3): 136–141. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1996.tb00346.x. S2CID 207858486.
  55. ^ Perdue, Charles W.; Gurtman, Michael B. (1990). «Evidence for the automaticity of ageism». Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 26 (3): 199–216. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(90)90035-K.
  56. ^ Brown, Rupert (2010). Prejudice: Its Social Psychology (2nd ed.). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. p. 88. ISBN 978-1-4051-1306-9.
  57. ^ Lepore, Lorella; Brown, Rupert (1997). «Category and Stereotype Activation: Is Prejudice Inevitable?» (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 72 (2): 275–287. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.72.2.275. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2 January 2014.
  58. ^ Kawakami, Kerry; et al. (2000). «Just say no (to stereotyping): effects of training in the negation of stereotypic associations on stereotype activation». Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 78 (5): 871–888. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.78.5.871. PMID 10821195.
  59. ^ a b Gawronski, Bertram; et al. (2008). «When ‘Just Say No’ is not enough: Affirmation versus negation training and the reduction of automatic stereotype activation». Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 44 (2): 370–377. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2006.12.004.
  60. ^ Wheeler, S. Christian; Petty, Richard E. (2001). «The Effects of Stereotype Activation on Behavior: A Review of Possible Mechanisms» (PDF). Psychological Bulletin. 127 (6): 797–826. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.127.6.797. PMID 11726072. Retrieved 10 June 2013.
  61. ^ Devos, Thierry; Huynh, Que-Lam; Banaji, Mahzarin D. (2012). «Implicit self and identity». In Leary, Mark R.; Tangney, June Price (eds.). Handbook of self and identity (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. pp. 164–165. ISBN 978-1-4625-0305-6.
  62. ^ Dijksterhuis, Ap (2001). «Automatic social influence: The perception-behavior links as an explanatory mechanism for behavior matching». In Forgas, Joseph P.; Williams, Kipling D. (eds.). Social influence: direct and indirect processes. Philadelphia, PA: Psychology Press. pp. 99–100. ISBN 9781841690391.
  63. ^ Operario, Din; Fiske, Susan T. (2001). «Causes and Consequences of Stereotypes in Organizations». In London, Manuel (ed.). How People Evaluate Others in Organizations. Mahwah, N.J.: Lawrence Erlbaum. p. 2012. ISBN 978-0-8058-3612-7.
  64. ^ «Alzheimer Dementia». Springer Reference. SpringerReference. Springer-Verlag. 2011. doi:10.1007/springerreference_34009.
  65. ^ Bargh, John A.; Chen, Mark; Burrows, Lara (1996). «Automaticity of Social Behavior: Direct Effects of Trait Construct and Stereotype Activation on Action» (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 71 (2): 230–244. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.320.8563. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230. PMID 8765481. Archived from the original (PDF) on 9 October 2013. Retrieved 10 June 2013.
  66. ^ Correll, Joshua; Park, Bernadette; Judd, Charles M.; Wittenbrink, Bernd (2002). «The Police Officer’s Dilemma: Using Ethnicity to Disambiguate Potentially Threatening Individuals». Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 83 (6): 1314–1329. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.466.7243. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.83.6.1314. PMID 12500813.
  67. ^ Yueh-Ting Lee; Lee J. Jussim; Clark R. McCauley, eds. (September 1995). Stereotype Accuracy: Toward Appreciating Group Differences. American Psychological Association. ISBN 978-1-55798-307-7.
  68. ^ Jussim, Lee; Crawford, Jarret T.; Rubinstein, Rachel S. (2015). «Stereotype (In)Accuracy in Perceptions of Groups and Individuals». Current Directions in Psychological Science. 24 (6): 490–497. doi:10.1177/0963721415605257. S2CID 53056616.
  69. ^ Jussim, Lee; Cain, Thomas R.; Crawford, Jarret T.; Harber, Kent; Cohen, Florette (2009). «The unbearable accuracy of stereotypes». In Nelson, Todd D (ed.). Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination. New York: Psychology Press. pp. 199–227. ISBN 978-0-8058-5952-2.
  70. ^ Terracciano, A; Abdel-Khalek, AM; Adám, N; Adamovová, L; Ahn, CK; Ahn, HN; Alansari, BM; Alcalay, L; et al. (2005). «National Character Does Not Reflect Mean Personality Trait Levels in 49 Cultures». Science. 310 (5745): 96–100. Bibcode:2005Sci…310…96T. doi:10.1126/science.1117199. PMC 2775052. PMID 16210536.
  71. ^ Mackie, Marlene. «Arriving at «truth» by definition: The case of stereotype inaccuracy.» Social problems 20, no. 4 (1973): 431–447.
  72. ^ a b Zemore, Sarah E.; Fiske, Susan T.; Kim, Hyun-Jeong (2000). «Gender Stereotypes and the Dynamics of Social Interaction». In Eckes, Thomas; Trautner, Hanns Martin (eds.). The Developmental Social Psychology of Gender. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 229–230. ISBN 978-0-585-30065-8.
  73. ^ a b Crocker, Jennifer; Major, Brenda; Stelle, Claude (1998). «Social Stigma». In Gilbert, Daniel T.; Fiske, Susan T.; Lindzey, Gardner (eds.). The Handbook of Social Psychology. Vol. Two (4th ed.). Oxford: Oxford University Press. pp. 519–521. ISBN 978-0-19-521376-8.
  74. ^ Whiteley, Bernard E.; Kite, Mary E. (2010). The Psychology of Prejudice and Discrimination (2nd ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning. pp. 428–435. ISBN 978-0-495-59964-7.
  75. ^ Crocker, Jennifer; Voelkl, Kristin; Testa, Maria; Major, Brenda (1991). «Social stigma: The affective consequences of attributional ambiguity». Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 60 (2): 218–228. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.60.2.218.
  76. ^ Osborne, Jason W. (2007). «Linking Stereotype Threat and Anxiety». Educational Psychology. 27 (1): 135–154. doi:10.1080/01443410601069929. S2CID 144555070.
  77. ^ Quinn, Diane M.; Kallen, Rachel W.; Spencer, Steven J. (2010). «Stereotype Threat». In Dividio, John F.; et al. (eds.). The SAGE Handbook of Prejudice, Stereotyping and Discrimination. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications. pp. 379–394. ISBN 978-1-4129-3453-4.
  78. ^ Inzlicht, Michael; Tullett, Alexa M.; Gutsell, Jennifer N. (2012). «Stereotype Threat Spillover: The Short- and Long-Term Effects of Coping with Threats to Social Identity». In Inzlicht, Michael; Schmader, Toni (eds.). Stereotype Threat: Theory, Process, and Application. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. p. 108. ISBN 978-0-19-973244-9.
  79. ^ Aronson, Joshua; Stelle, Claude M. (2005). «Chapter 24: Stereotypes and the Fragility of Academic Competence, Motivation, and Self-Concept». In Elliot, Andrew J.; Dweck, Carol S. (eds.). Handbook of Competence and Motivation. New York: Guilford Press. pp. 436, 443. ISBN 978-1-59385-123-1.
  80. ^ Steele, Claude M.; Aronson, Joshua (November 1995). «Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans» (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 69 (5): 797–811. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797. PMID 7473032.
  81. ^ Stone, Jeff; Lynch, Christian I.; Sjomeling, Mike; Darley, John M. (1999). «Stereotype threat effects on Black and White athletic performance». Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 77 (6): 1213–1227. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.687.5039. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.77.6.1213.
  82. ^ Maass, Anne; D’Ettole, Claudio; Cadinu, Mara (2008). «Checkmate? The role of gender stereotypes in the ultimate intellectual sport» (PDF). European Journal of Social Psychology. 38 (2): 231–245. doi:10.1002/ejsp.440. S2CID 144308852. Archived from the original (PDF) on 20 November 2012. Retrieved 5 September 2012.
  83. ^ Gupta, V. K.; Bhawe, N. M. (2007). «The Influence of Proactive Personality and Stereotype Threat on Women’s Entrepreneurial Intentions». Journal of Leadership & Organizational Studies. 13 (4): 73–85. doi:10.1177/10717919070130040901. S2CID 145318243.
  84. ^ Arthur Robert Jensen «The g factor: the science of mental ability» 1998
    ISBN 0-275-96103-6, Praeger Publishers, 88 Post Road West, Westport, CT 06881, pages 513–515: «the phenomenon of stereotype threat can be explained in terms of a
    more general construct, test anxiety, which has been studied since the early days
    of psychometrics. Test anxiety tends to lower performance levels on tests
    in proportion to the degree of complexity and the amount of mental effort they
    require of the subject. The relatively greater effect of test anxiety in the black
    samples, who had somewhat lower SAT scores, than the white subjects in the
    Stanford experiments constitutes an example of the Yerkes-Dodson law … by conducting the same type of experiment using exclusively white (or black)
    subjects, divided into lower- and higher-ability groups, it might be shown that
    the phenomenon attributed to stereotype threat has nothing to do with race as
    such, but results from the interaction of ability level with test anxiety as a
    function of test complexity.»
  85. ^ a b Stoet, G.; Geary, D. C. (2012). «Can stereotype threat explain the gender gap in mathematics performance and achievement?». Review of General Psychology. 16: 93–102. doi:10.1037/a0026617. S2CID 145724069. Pdf. Archived 12 January 2016 at the Wayback Machine
  86. ^ Fryer, R. G.; Levitt, S. D.; List, J. A. (2008). «Exploring the Impact of Financial Incentives on Stereotype Threat: Evidence from a Pilot Study» (PDF). American Economic Review. 98 (2): 370–375. doi:10.1257/aer.98.2.370.
  87. ^ Yong, Ed (9 September 2016). «A Worrying Trend for Psychology’s ‘Simple Little Tricks’«. The Atlantic. Retrieved 11 September 2016.
  88. ^ a b Ganley, Colleen M.; Mingle, Leigh A.; Ryan, Allison M.; Ryan, Katherine; Vasilyeva, Marina; Perry, Michelle (1 January 2013). «An Examination of Stereotype Threat Effects on Girls’ Mathematics Performance» (PDF). Developmental Psychology. 49 (10): 1886–1897. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.353.4436. doi:10.1037/a0031412. PMID 23356523. Archived from the original (PDF) on 19 July 2014.
  89. ^ Flore, Paulette C.; Wicherts, Jelte M. (2014). «Does stereotype threat influence performance of girls in stereotyped domains? A meta-analysis». Journal of School Psychology. 53 (1): 25–44. doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2014.10.002. ISSN 0022-4405. PMID 25636259.
  90. ^ Kassin, Saul M.; Fein, Steven; Markus, Hazel Rose (2011). Social psychology (8th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth, Cengage Learning. p. 172. ISBN 978-0-495-81240-1.
  91. ^ Brown, Rupert (2010). Prejudice: Its Social Psychology (2nd ed.). Oxford: Wiley-Blackwell. pp. 94–97. ISBN 978-1-4051-1306-9.
  92. ^ Chen, Mark; Bargh, John A. (1997). «Nonconscious Behavioral Confirmation Processes: The Self-Fulfilling Consequences of Automatic Stereotype Activation» (PDF). Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 33 (5): 541–560. doi:10.1006/jesp.1997.1329. Archived from the original (PDF) on 22 May 2013. Retrieved 5 April 2013.
  93. ^ Word, Carl O.; Zanna, Mark P.; Cooper, Joel (1974). «The nonverbal mediation of self-fulfilling prophecies in interracial interaction». Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 10 (2): 109–120. doi:10.1016/0022-1031(74)90059-6.
  94. ^ Snyder, Mark; Tanke, Elizabeth D.; Berscheid, Ellen (1977). «Social perception and interpersonal behavior: On the self-fulfilling nature of social stereotypes» (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 35 (9): 656–666. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.35.9.656. Archived from the original (PDF) on 17 June 2012.
  95. ^ Kellow, Thomas (February 2008). «The Effects of Stereotypes on the Achievement Gap: Reexamining the Academic Performance of African American High School Students». Journal of Black Psychology. 34: 94–120. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.553.9188. doi:10.1177/0095798407310537. S2CID 145490359.
  96. ^ Friedman, A. L., & Lyne, S. R. (2001). The beancounter stereotype: towards a general model of stereotype generation. Critical perspectives on accounting, 12(4), 423-451.
  97. ^ Jeacle, I., Miley, F., & Read, A. (2012). Jokes in popular culture: the characterisation of the accountant. Accounting, Auditing & Accountability Journal.
  98. ^ Banaji, Mahzarin R. (2002). «The Social Psychology of Stereotypes». In Smelser, Neil; Baltes, Paul (eds.). International Encyclopedia of the Social and Behavioral Sciences. New York: Pergamon. pp. 15100–15104. doi:10.1016/B0-08-043076-7/01754-X. ISBN 978-0-08-043076-8.
  99. ^ Fiske, Susan T.; Lee, Tiane L. (2008). «Stereotypes and prejudice create workplace discrimination». In Brief, Arthur P (ed.). Diversity at Work. New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 13–52. ISBN 978-0-521-86030-7.
  100. ^ Agerström, Jens; Rooth, Dan-Olof (2011). «The role of automatic obesity stereotypes in real hiring discrimination». Journal of Applied Psychology. 96 (4): 790–805. doi:10.1037/a0021594. PMID 21280934.
  101. ^ a b Davison, Heather K.; Burke, Michael J. (2000). «Sex Discrimination in Simulated Employment Contexts: A Meta-analytic Investigation». Journal of Vocational Behavior. 56 (2): 225–248. doi:10.1006/jvbe.1999.1711.
  102. ^ Rudman, Laurie A.; Glick, Peter (2001). «Prescriptive Gender Stereotypes and Backlash toward Agentic Women» (PDF). Journal of Social Issues. 57 (4): 743–762. doi:10.1111/0022-4537.00239. hdl:2027.42/146421. Archived from the original (PDF) on 6 November 2012.
  103. ^ Yékú, James (9 September 2020). «Anti-Afropolitan ethics and the performative politics of online scambaiting». Social Dynamics. 46 (2): 240–258. doi:10.1080/02533952.2020.1813943. ISSN 0253-3952. S2CID 222232833.
  104. ^ Sinclair, Stacey; Huntsinger, Jeff (2006). «The Interpersonal Basis of Self-Stereotyping». In Levin, Shana; Van Laar, Colette (eds.). Stigma and Group Inequality: Social Psychological Perspectives. Claremont Symposium on Applied Social Psychology. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. p. 239. ISBN 978-0-8058-4415-3.
  105. ^ Correll, Shelley J. (2001). «Gender and the career choice process: The role of biased self-assessments» (PDF). American Journal of Sociology. 106 (6): 1691–1730. doi:10.1086/321299. S2CID 142863258. Archived from the original (PDF) on 12 September 2011.
  106. ^ Correll, Shelley J. (2004). «Constraints into Preferences: Gender, Status, and Emerging Career Aspirations» (PDF). American Sociological Review. 69 (1): 93–113. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.520.8370. doi:10.1177/000312240406900106. S2CID 8735336.
  107. ^ Sinclair, Stacey; Hardin, Curtis D.; Lowery, Brian S. (2006). «Self-Stereotyping in the Context of Multiple Social Identities» (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 90 (4): 529–542. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.90.4.529. PMID 16649853. Archived from the original (PDF) on 27 August 2014.
  108. ^ Sachs, Nicole M.; Veysey, Bonita M.; Rivera, Luis M. (14 November 2017). «Implicit Social Cognitive Processes Underlying Victim Self and Identity: Evidence With College-Aged Adults». Journal of Interpersonal Violence. 36 (3–4): 1256–1282. doi:10.1177/0886260517741625. PMID 29294984. S2CID 206565963.
  109. ^ HAYAKAWA, S. I. (1950). «Recognizing Stereotypes as Substitutes for Thought». ETC: A Review of General Semantics. 7 (3): 208–210. JSTOR 42581302.
  110. ^ Taylor, Rhonda Harris; Patterson, Lotsee (December 2000). «Using information literacy to promote critical thinking». Teacher Librarian. 28 (2): 9–14. OCLC 425340563.
  111. ^ «Lesson 2 – Stock Characters | BYU Theatre Education Database». tedb.byu.edu. Retrieved 30 March 2018.
  112. ^ Fedorov, Alexander (2015). «Media Stereotypes Analysis in the Classroom at the Student Audience». European Journal of Contemporary Education. 12 (2): 158–162. doi:10.13187/ejced.2015.12.158.
  113. ^ Lee, Kevin (January 2008). ««The Little State Department»: Hollywood and the MPAA’s Influence on U.S. Trade Relations». Northwestern Journal of International Law & Business. 28 (2).
  114. ^ «Will the cliche of the ‘Russian baddie’ ever leave our screens?». The Guardian. 10 July 2017.
  115. ^ «Russian film industry and Hollywood uneasy with one another.» Fox News. 14 October 2014
  116. ^ «5 Hollywood Villains That Prove Russian Stereotypes Are Hard to Kill». The Moscow Times. 9 August 2015.
  117. ^ «Hollywood stereotypes: Why are Russians the bad guys?». BBC News. 5 November 2014.
  118. ^ Berg, Charles (Summer 1990). «Stereotyping in films in general and of the Hispanic in Particular». The Howard Journal of Communications. 2 (3): 294–296. doi:10.1080/10646179009359721.
  119. ^ COLTRANE, SCOTT; ADAMS, MICHELE. «Work–Family Imagery and Gender Stereotypes:Television and the Reproduction of Difference». Journal of Vocational Behavior. 50.
  120. ^ Mou, Yi; Peng, Wei. «Gender and Racial Stereotypes in Popular Video Games» (PDF). Michigan State University.
  121. ^ Burgess, Melinda; Dill, Karen (15 September 2011). «Playing With Prejudice: The Prevalence and Consequences of Racial Stereotypes in Video Games». Media Psychology. 14 (3): 289–311. doi:10.1080/15213269.2011.596467. S2CID 1416833.
  122. ^ Auracher, Jan; Hirose, Akiko (2017). «The Influence of Reader’s Stereotypes on the Assessment of Fictional Characters». Comparative Literature Studies. 54 (4): 795–823. doi:10.5325/complitstudies.54.4.0795. JSTOR 10.5325/complitstudies.54.4.0795.

Further reading[edit]

  • Hilton, James L.; von Hippel, William (1996). «Stereotypes». Annual Review of Psychology. 47 (1): 237–271. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.47.1.237. PMID 15012482.
  • Stuart Ewen, Elizabeth Ewen, Typecasting: On the Arts and Sciences of Human Inequality. New York (Seven Stories Press) 2006
  • Stereotype & Society A Major Resource: Constantly updated and archived
  • Regenberg, Nina (2007). «Are Blonds Really Dumb?». The Inquisitive Mind (3). Archived from the original on 26 July 2011. Retrieved 4 May 2010.
  • Are Stereotypes True?
  • Turner, Chris (2004). Planet Simpson: How a Cartoon Masterpiece Documented an Era and Defined a Generation. Foreword by Douglas Coupland. (1st ed.). Toronto: Random House Canada. ISBN 978-0679313182. OCLC 55682258..
  • Crawford, M. & Unger, R. (2004). Women and Gender: A Feminist Psychology. McGraw Hill New York. New York. 45–49.
  • Spitzer, B.L.; Henderson, K; Zavian, M. T. (1999). «Gender differences in population versus media body sizes: A comparison over four decades». Sex Roles. 40 (7/8): 545–565. doi:10.1023/a:1018836029738. S2CID 55674520.

External links[edit]

Last Update: Jan 03, 2023

This is a question our experts keep getting from time to time. Now, we have got the complete detailed explanation and answer for everyone, who is interested!


Asked by: Donnell Labadie

Score: 4.5/5
(8 votes)

The term stereotype comes from the French adjective stéréotype and derives from the Greek words στερεός (stereos), «firm, solid» and τύπος (typos), impression, hence «solid impression on one or more ideas/theories.»

Who came up with the term stereotype?

The term stereotype was first coined by Lippmann in 1922 (as cited in Dovidio, Hewstone, Glick & Esses, 2010) in order to describe a social group’s perceived characteristics. Furthermore, according to Allport (1954, p. 191), a stereotype is an ‘an exaggerated belief associated with a category.

What is the real meaning of stereotype?

A stereotype is a fixed general image or set of characteristics that a lot of people believe represent a particular type of person or thing. … If someone is stereotyped as something, people form a fixed general idea or image of them, so that it is assumed that they will behave in a particular way.

What is the meaning of stereotype in simple words?

A stereotype is a preconceived notion, especially about a group of people. … You have probably heard stereotypes: commonly held ideas or preconceptions about specific groups. You most often hear about negative stereotypes, but some are positive — the stereotype that tall people are good at basketball, for example.

What is the main idea of stereotype?

In social psychology, a stereotype is a fixed, over generalized belief about a particular group or class of people. By stereotyping we infer that a person has a whole range of characteristics and abilities that we assume all members of that group have.

35 related questions found

What is stereotype explain with example?

Stereotypes become overgeneralized and applied to all members of a group. For example, someone holding prejudiced attitudes toward older adults, may believe that older adults are slow and incompetent (Cuddy, Norton, & Fiske, 2005; Nelson, 2004).

What do you know about stereotype?

In social psychology, a stereotype is any thought widely adopted about specific types of individuals or certain ways of behaving intended to represent the entire group of those individuals or behaviors as a whole. These thoughts or beliefs may or may not accurately reflect reality.

What is meant by stereotype answer?

A stereotype is a mistaken idea or belief many people have about a thing or group that is based upon how they look on the outside, which may be untrue or only partly true.

Is stereotype a negative word?

Stereotype has a negative connotation. But a stereotype is simply a generalization about how a group of people behaves. It may be statistically accurate but not universally valid. Many believe we shouldn’t make decisions a ecting an individual based on a stereotype, even if it is statistically accurate.

When was stereotyping invented?

stereotype, type of printing plate developed in the late 18th century and widely used in letterpress, newspaper, and other high-speed press runs.

What is negative stereotype?

a stereotype that purports to describe the undesirable, objectionable, or unacceptable qualities and characteristics of the members of a particular group or social category.

What is the definition of a negative stereotype?

Definition. Negative stereotypes are traits and characteristics, negatively valenced and attributed to a social group and to its individual members.

What is the synonym of stereotype?

Some common synonyms of stereotyped are hackneyed, threadbare, and trite.

What is meant by stereotype Class 6?

The process of fixing people into an image is called stereotype. When we fix people into one image, we create a stereotype. Problems created by stereotypes. They stop us from looking at each person as a unique individual.

What do you mean by stereotype Class 7?

Question 1: What is stereotype? Answer: It refers to the belief that people belonging to a particular religion, community, gender, etc. have certain traits and can do only a certain type of work.

What is the meaning of stereotype in paragraph 8?

Craft and Structure: What is the meaning of stereotype in paragraph 8? In this context, stereotype means an overgeneralized belief about a group of people.

What is an example of a stereotype character?

Stereotypical Character

If the labels “jock,” “old lady,” “bully,” or “cowboy” automatically bring to mind visual images, then those particular character types have become stereotyped for you.

What are three examples of stereotyping?

Examples of Gender Stereotypes

  • Girls should play with dolls and boys should play with trucks.
  • Boys should be directed to like blue and green; girls toward red and pink.
  • Boys should not wear dresses or other clothes typically associated with «girl’s clothes»

What are the four types of stereotypes?

Altogether, these four kinds of stereotypes and their associated emotional prejudices (pride, disgust, envy, pity) occur all over the world for each of society’s own groups.

What are two synonyms for stereotype?

Synonyms of stereotype

  • concept,
  • conception,
  • generality,
  • generalization,
  • notion.

What is the synonym of stigma?

shame, disgrace, dishonour. stain, taint, blot, blot on one’s escutcheon, blemish, brand, mark, slur.

What is the difference between generalizing and stereotyping?

Generalizations become stereotypes when all members of a group are categorized as having the same characteristics. Stereotypes can be linked to any type of cultural membership, such as nationality, religion, gender, race, or age.

What is stereotyping in communication?

Stereotyping occurs when a person ascribes the collective characteristics associated with a particular group to every member of that group, discounting individual characteristics. From: Communication Disorders in Multicultural and International Populations (Fourth Edition), 2012.

How does stereotyping affect communication?

Stereotypes also can affect the way communicators respond to their audience, according to 2014 research from the University of Portland. In face-to-face communication, for example, employees may feel uncomfortable communicating honestly with those who they perceive as aggressive or uncooperative based on stereotypes.

What is stereotypes in intercultural communication?

as the set of characteristics which are associated with specific social groups (e.g., intelligent/unintelligent, serious/funny, rude/polite, shy/ outgoing). Eagly and Chaiken (1993), for example claim that a stereotype consists of “attributes that an individual ascribes to a social group” (p. 104).

ThoughtCo. / Melissa Ling


Updated on February 04, 2021

Stereotypes are characteristics imposed upon groups of people because of their race, nationality, and sexual orientation. These characteristics tend to be oversimplifications of the groups involved and, even if they seem «positive,» stereotypes are harmful.

Did You Know?

Even when framed as «positive,» stereotypes of certain groups have negative effects. An example of this is the myth of the «model minority» that has attached itself broadly to people of Asian descent.

Stereotypes Vs. Generalizations

While all stereotypes are generalizations, not all generalizations are stereotypes. Stereotypes are widely circulated oversimplifications of a group of people, while generalizations can be based more on personal experience, not a widely accepted factor.

In the United States, certain racial groups have been linked to stereotypes such as being good at math, athletics, and dancing. These stereotypes are so well-known that the average American wouldn’t hesitate if asked to identify which racial group in this country, for example, has a reputation for excelling in basketball. In short, when one stereotypes, one repeats the cultural mythology already present in a particular society.

On the other hand, a person can make a generalization about an ethnic group that hasn’t been perpetuated in society. For example, someone who meets a few individuals from a particular country and finds them to be quiet and reserved may say that all citizens of the country in question are quiet and reserved. A generalization such as this doesn’t allow for diversity within groups and may result in stigmatization and discrimination of groups if the stereotypes linked to them are largely negative.

Intersectionality

While stereotypes may refer to a specific gender, race, religion, or country, often they link various aspects of identity together. This is known as intersectionality. A stereotype about Black gay men, for example, would involve race, gender, and sexual orientation. Although such a stereotype targets a specific group rather than Black people as a whole, it’s still problematic to insinuate that Black gay men are all the same. Too many other factors make up any one person’s identity to ascribe a fixed list of characteristics to him.

Differing stereotypes can also be present within larger groups, resulting in things like gender-based stereotypes within the same race. Certain stereotypes apply to Asian Americans generally, but when the Asian American population is broken down by gender, one finds that stereotypes of Asian American men and Asian American women differ. For example, the women of a racial group may be deemed attractive due to fetishization and the men in that same racial group may be viewed as the exact opposite.

Even stereotypes applied to a racial group become inconsistent when members of that group are broken down by origin. Stereotypes about Black Americans differ from those about Black people from the Caribbean or Black people from African nations.

Educalingo cookies are used to personalize ads and get web traffic statistics. We also share information about the use of the site with our social media, advertising and analytics partners.

Download the app
educalingo

I have to say I wasn’t a huge fan of ‘Star Wars’, and I’m still not, really, but you have to acknowledge that there’s a huge fan base for it, and these people are really sweet. You can’t stereotype a ‘Star Wars’ fan.

Ralph Brown

section

PRONUNCIATION OF STEREOTYPE

facebooktwitterpinterestwhatsapp

GRAMMATICAL CATEGORY OF STEREOTYPE

Stereotype is a verb and can also act as a noun.

A noun is a type of word the meaning of which determines reality. Nouns provide the names for all things: people, objects, sensations, feelings, etc.

The verb is the part of the sentence that is conjugated and expresses action and state of being.

See the conjugation of the verb stereotype in English.

WHAT DOES STEREOTYPE MEAN IN ENGLISH?

stereotype

Stereotype

In social psychology, a stereotype is a thought that can be adopted about specific types of individuals or certain ways of doing things. These thoughts or beliefs may or may not accurately reflect reality. However, this is only a fundamental psychological definition of a stereotype. Within psychology and spanning across other disciplines, there are different conceptualizations and theories of stereotyping that provide their own expanded definition. Some of these definitions share commonalities, though each one may also harbor unique aspects that may contradict the others.


Definition of stereotype in the English dictionary

The first definition of stereotype in the dictionary is a method of producing cast-metal printing plates from a mould made from a forme of type matter in papier-mâché or some other material. Other definition of stereotype is the plate so made. Stereotype is also an idea, trait, convention, etc, that has grown stale through fixed usage.

CONJUGATION OF THE VERB TO STEREOTYPE

PRESENT

Present

I stereotype

you stereotype

he/she/it stereotypes

we stereotype

you stereotype

they stereotype

Present continuous

I am stereotyping

you are stereotyping

he/she/it is stereotyping

we are stereotyping

you are stereotyping

they are stereotyping

Present perfect

I have stereotyped

you have stereotyped

he/she/it has stereotyped

we have stereotyped

you have stereotyped

they have stereotyped

Present perfect continuous

I have been stereotyping

you have been stereotyping

he/she/it has been stereotyping

we have been stereotyping

you have been stereotyping

they have been stereotyping

Present tense is used to refer to circumstances that exist at the present time or over a period that includes the present time. The present perfect refers to past events, although it can be considered to denote primarily the resulting present situation rather than the events themselves.

PAST

Past

I stereotyped

you stereotyped

he/she/it stereotyped

we stereotyped

you stereotyped

they stereotyped

Past continuous

I was stereotyping

you were stereotyping

he/she/it was stereotyping

we were stereotyping

you were stereotyping

they were stereotyping

Past perfect

I had stereotyped

you had stereotyped

he/she/it had stereotyped

we had stereotyped

you had stereotyped

they had stereotyped

Past perfect continuous

I had been stereotyping

you had been stereotyping

he/she/it had been stereotyping

we had been stereotyping

you had been stereotyping

they had been stereotyping

Past tense forms express circumstances existing at some time in the past,

FUTURE

Future

I will stereotype

you will stereotype

he/she/it will stereotype

we will stereotype

you will stereotype

they will stereotype

Future continuous

I will be stereotyping

you will be stereotyping

he/she/it will be stereotyping

we will be stereotyping

you will be stereotyping

they will be stereotyping

Future perfect

I will have stereotyped

you will have stereotyped

he/she/it will have stereotyped

we will have stereotyped

you will have stereotyped

they will have stereotyped

Future perfect continuous

I will have been stereotyping

you will have been stereotyping

he/she/it will have been stereotyping

we will have been stereotyping

you will have been stereotyping

they will have been stereotyping

The future is used to express circumstances that will occur at a later time.

CONDITIONAL

Conditional

I would stereotype

you would stereotype

he/she/it would stereotype

we would stereotype

you would stereotype

they would stereotype

Conditional continuous

I would be stereotyping

you would be stereotyping

he/she/it would be stereotyping

we would be stereotyping

you would be stereotyping

they would be stereotyping

Conditional perfect

I would have stereotype

you would have stereotype

he/she/it would have stereotype

we would have stereotype

you would have stereotype

they would have stereotype

Conditional perfect continuous

I would have been stereotyping

you would have been stereotyping

he/she/it would have been stereotyping

we would have been stereotyping

you would have been stereotyping

they would have been stereotyping

Conditional or «future-in-the-past» tense refers to hypothetical or possible actions.

IMPERATIVE

Imperative

you stereotype
we let´s stereotype
you stereotype

The imperative is used to form commands or requests.

NONFINITE VERB FORMS

Past participle

stereotyped

Present Participle

stereotyping

Infinitive shows the action beyond temporal perspective. The present participle or gerund shows the action during the session. The past participle shows the action after completion.

WORDS THAT RHYME WITH STEREOTYPE

Synonyms and antonyms of stereotype in the English dictionary of synonyms

SYNONYMS OF «STEREOTYPE»

The following words have a similar or identical meaning as «stereotype» and belong to the same grammatical category.

Translation of «stereotype» into 25 languages

online translator

TRANSLATION OF STEREOTYPE

Find out the translation of stereotype to 25 languages with our English multilingual translator.

The translations of stereotype from English to other languages presented in this section have been obtained through automatic statistical translation; where the essential translation unit is the word «stereotype» in English.

Translator English — Chinese


成见

1,325 millions of speakers

Translator English — Spanish


estereotipo

570 millions of speakers

Translator English — Hindi


स्टीरियोटाइप

380 millions of speakers

Translator English — Arabic


شَكْلٌ نَـمَطِّيّ

280 millions of speakers

Translator English — Russian


стереотип

278 millions of speakers

Translator English — Portuguese


estereótipo

270 millions of speakers

Translator English — Bengali


বাঁধাধরা

260 millions of speakers

Translator English — French


stéréotype

220 millions of speakers

Translator English — Malay


stereotaip

190 millions of speakers

Translator English — Japanese


ステレオタイプ

130 millions of speakers

Translator English — Korean


고정관념

85 millions of speakers

Translator English — Javanese


Stereotype

85 millions of speakers

Translator English — Vietnamese


khuôn mẫu

80 millions of speakers

Translator English — Tamil


ஒரே மாதிரியான

75 millions of speakers

Translator English — Marathi


देखावा

75 millions of speakers

Translator English — Turkish


klişe

70 millions of speakers

Translator English — Italian


stereotipo

65 millions of speakers

Translator English — Polish


stereotyp

50 millions of speakers

Translator English — Ukrainian


стереотип

40 millions of speakers

Translator English — Romanian


stereotip

30 millions of speakers

Translator English — Greek


στερεότυπο

15 millions of speakers

Translator English — Afrikaans


stereotipe

14 millions of speakers

Translator English — Swedish


stereotyp

10 millions of speakers

Translator English — Norwegian


stereotyp

5 millions of speakers

Trends of use of stereotype

TENDENCIES OF USE OF THE TERM «STEREOTYPE»

The term «stereotype» is quite widely used and occupies the 28.471 position in our list of most widely used terms in the English dictionary.

Trends

FREQUENCY

Quite widely used

The map shown above gives the frequency of use of the term «stereotype» in the different countries.

Principal search tendencies and common uses of stereotype

List of principal searches undertaken by users to access our English online dictionary and most widely used expressions with the word «stereotype».

FREQUENCY OF USE OF THE TERM «STEREOTYPE» OVER TIME

The graph expresses the annual evolution of the frequency of use of the word «stereotype» during the past 500 years. Its implementation is based on analysing how often the term «stereotype» appears in digitalised printed sources in English between the year 1500 and the present day.

Examples of use in the English literature, quotes and news about stereotype

10 QUOTES WITH «STEREOTYPE»

Famous quotes and sentences with the word stereotype.

The whole idea of a stereotype is to simplify. Instead of going through the problem of all this great diversity — that it’s this or maybe that — you have just one large statement; it is this.

It was an ongoing struggle to say no, I don’t want to be a part of the perpetuation of this stereotype.

I hate the stereotype of the pitfalls of the child actor. There are so many amazing examples — Natalie Portman, Joseph Gordon-Levitt, Jodie Foster, Drew Barrymore — of people who have made it through.

Americans are a lot more open, of course. There’s something more declamatory in the way you express emotions. It’s a stereotype but it’s true. British people can appear repressed in expressing emotions. Not very good at self-evaluating, or affirming situations, touching, anything like that.

It was good because it helped me get where I’m at today, but then people stereotype and say we don’t want to use her because she’s known as the ‘Wonderbra girl’.

I have to say I wasn’t a huge fan of ‘Star Wars’, and I’m still not, really, but you have to acknowledge that there’s a huge fan base for it, and these people are really sweet. You can’t stereotype a ‘Star Wars’ fan.

The stereotype of psychotherapy portrayed in popular books and movies is lying on the couch and saying whatever comes into your mind, while a kindly psychoanalyst listens and nods knowingly from time to time. After years and years, something wonderful is supposed to happen.

I believe the biggest challenge is just getting the courage to try something different or new. Try to forget the stereotype in your mind. Yoga is for everyone — children, athletes, moms, dads, accountants, truck drivers, even country stars.

Honestly, what can really be said about ‘the Jewish people’ as a whole? Is it not a lamentable stereotype to make large generalizations about all Jews, and to presume they all share the same political commitments?

There’s such a stereotype about men and women. Obviously, people think men are faster and stronger and all these other things, and I don’t want people to get sucked into that anymore. I want them to realize that the women are out here and doing just as awesome things. They can be just as great, too.

10 ENGLISH BOOKS RELATING TO «STEREOTYPE»

Discover the use of stereotype in the following bibliographical selection. Books relating to stereotype and brief extracts from same to provide context of its use in English literature.

1

Film and Stereotype: A Challenge for Cinema and Theory

However, Flaubert was aware, Amossy notes, that even great antipathy toward
the stereotype does not permit one to actually go the full lengths of abandoning it
altogether. This would ultimately only lead to an even greater reliance on it.

2

Unraveling the «model minority» stereotype: listening to …

The second edition of Unraveling the «Model Minority» Stereotype: Listening to Asian American Youth extends Stacey Lee’s groundbreaking research on the educational experiences and achievement of Asian American youth.

3

Stereotypes and Stereotyping

Following a broad overview that defines stereotypes, the book addresses how they are formed and developed in chapters that cover the social psychology of stereotypes, the impact of physical appearance on their formation, and methods of …

C. Neil Macrae, Charles Stangor, Miles Hewstone, 1996

4

Ouch! That Stereotype Hurts

5

Stereotype Dynamics: Language-Based Approaches to the …

This volume addresses the role of communication in stereotype dynamics, while placing the phenomenon of social stereotypes appropriately in the socio-cultural context.

Yoshihisa Kashima, Klaus Fiedler, Peter Freytag, 2012

6

From Stereotype to Metaphor: The Jew in Contemporary Drama

The Jew in Contemporary Drama Ellen Schiff. inevitable that the revised
perceptions work their way into dramatic literature, especially in societies like the
ones under consideration here where Jews are active in the entertainment arts.
Radio …

7

The Politics of Stereotype: Psychology and Affirmative Action

Highlights the history of Affirmative Action and spotlights its advantages, problems, and consequences.

8

A Question of Class: The Redneck Stereotype in Southern Fiction

Ultimately, this work is an evaluation of individual Southern fiction writers in their capacity to rise above stereotyping.

9

Sociology and the Stereotype of the Criminal

Such practices depend on selection by the police of vulnerable subjects, that is,
persons who conform to the stereotype of criminal and whose word is unlikely to
be accepted by the court in contradiction to the evidence of the police.

10

Stereotype Activation and Inhibition: Advances in Social …

This volume continues the series format of a target article on a hot topic in social cognition accompanied by commentary from other leading researchers.

Robert S. Wyer, Jr., 2013

10 NEWS ITEMS WHICH INCLUDE THE TERM «STEREOTYPE»

Find out what the national and international press are talking about and how the term stereotype is used in the context of the following news items.

Muslims in Moscow Work to Break a Stereotype

Russian tabloids and television have reinforced that stereotype. But in the last year and a half, as turmoil in Ukraine has dominated the news media’s attention, … «New York Times, Jul 15»

The Negative In Positive Stereotypes

Reid’s remarks reflect a positive stereotype — a belief that attributes a favorable characteristic to a group. In this case, it’s that women have patience. Women are … «NPR, Jul 15»

Gender-science stereotype strongest in Holland

Washington: In the largest such study, researchers have found that stereotypes associating science with men are the strongest in the Netherlands and widely … «Zee News, May 15»

Vergara takes Carmen Miranda stereotype to the bank

Vergara takes Carmen Miranda stereotype to the bank. By SORAYA NADIA MCDONALD The Washington Post. This article was published May 17, 2015 at 2:17 … «Arkansas Online, May 15»

Amy Schumer slays the ‘humourless feminist’ stereotype once and …

If anyone still believed the «humourless feminist» stereotype, Amy Schumer delivered the knock-out punch this week. For a full half hour, the comedian dedicated … «Sydney Morning Herald, May 15»

Fifty years on, the overachiever stereotype is still hurting Asian …

A-Town Boyz is a timely reminder that across the United States, the “model minority” stereotype commonly used to understand Asian Americans is badly in need … «Quartz, Apr 15»

Job interview? Beat the Millennial stereotype

Be aware of the stereotype and take special care not to reinforce it. For one, don’t start every sentence with the word: «I.» A marketing executive I know has … «CNNMoney, Mar 15»

Stereotypes lower math performance in women, but effects go …

BLOOMINGTON, Ind. — A new study from Indiana University suggests that gender stereotypes about women’s ability in mathematics negatively impact their … «IU Bloomington Newsroom, Mar 15»

Here’s Some History Behind That ‘Angry Black Woman’ Riff the NY …

These stereotypes served the needs of a slave regime that wanted to justify the sexual exploitation of enslaved women by painting them as Jezebels, like the … «The Root, Sep 14»

Matt Forte wants to break old running back stereotype

«I want to break the stereotype of old running backs going downhill,» Forte said Saturday, via the Chicago Tribune. «This offseason I feel better than I have the … «NFL.com, Jul 14»

REFERENCE

« EDUCALINGO. Stereotype [online]. Available <https://educalingo.com/en/dic-en/stereotype>. Apr 2023 ».

Download the educalingo app


Discover all that is hidden in the words on educalingo

In this article we will discuss about Stereotypes. After reading this article you will learn about: 1. Definition of Stereotypes 2. Nature of Stereotypes 3. Causes and Development 4. Functions 5. Important Studies.

Contents:

  1. Definition of Stereotypes
  2. Nature of Stereotypes
  3. Causes and Development of Stereotypes
  4. Functions of Stereotypes
  5. Important Studies of Stereotypes

1. Definition of Stereotypes:

Stereotypes have been defined as a false classificatory concept to which as a rule a strong emotional feeling tone of likes or dislikes, approval or disapproval is attached. According to Lippmam, stereotypes are individual attitudes so strongly inter conditioned by collective contacts that they become highly standardized and uniform within the group.

He also held that a stereotype is a composite of idea or attitudes that make up the picture of our heads or the “apperceptive mass” which means that all the experiences acquired in the past determine our perception about the object at a particular moment.

In other words, one’s behaviour, perception and judgement about others as well as about the self is determined by the pattern of stereotype we acquire from our culture. It largely determines what group of facts we shall see and what light and what shadow we will reflect on them.

According to Allport (1954) whether favourable or unfavourable, a stereotype is an exaggerated belief asserted with a category.

Vinacke has defined stereotypes as “A collection of trait names upon which a large percentage of people agree as appropriate for describing some sort of individuals”. The process of stereotyping, therefore, appears as a tendency to attribute generalized and simplified characters to groups of peoples in the verbal levels.

Since, the attempt to see all things freshly and in detail, bit by bit is time taking and exhausting overgeneralization of facts is made through already existing mental pictures and readymade facts.

In this connection, Lippman says “we do not first see and then define, we first define and then see. In the great blooming and budging confusion of the outer world, we pick out what our culture has already defined for us.”

Due to overgeneralization or false generalization people are classified into different types without taking time to see actually what they are. “We are told about the world before we see it, between these two there is big gap, but it is one of degree and not of kind.”

Kupuswamy observes that stereotypes are socially standardized concepts concerning other groups. They make it possible for the members of the group to communicate with each other quickly and efficiently about the other groups.

Baron and Byrne (1988) hold that stereotypes are negative schemata for social groups. They are a type of cognitive framework for interpreting and studying social information. It strongly affects the incoming information and information processing. Information relevant to a particular stereotype is processed and accepted more quickly than information not related to that stereotype.

Further, those informations consistent or equivalent to the stereotypes are attended to first and speedily. If someone has the stereotype that Indians are lazy people, at once, he will accept if he receives such an information from the environment. But the reverse he may not easily accept.

Even strong stereotypes may lead the person to make determined efforts to refute it. We usually remember those information which are consistent with our stereotypes and forget the rest because informations consistent with one’s cognitive frame work readily fits into it.

The stereotype is, therefore, to a large degree self confirming inducing the individual to bring supporting information to mind. Evidences for the operation of such negative schematas have been reported by Dovidio, Evans and Tyler (1986) and Greenberg and Zeynski (1985).

Sherif and Sherif (1969) have said that group stereotype is a popular term referring to agreement among members of a group on their image of another group and its members. They have operationally defined it in terms of the proportion of group members agreeing on levels or attributions for another group and its members. They further observe that a stereotype may be said to exist when a high proportion agrees on the image of the out group.

Psychologically, the phenomena of stereotype or group stereotype is not fundamentally different from the concept of prejudice or attitude. Sherif and Sherif hold that their distinctiveness is derived from the nature of the stimulus situation to which they refer (intergroup relations), not from the psychological principles governing concept and attitude formation or their change.

According to Bird “Stereotyped responses are perceptions of meaningful arrangement of ideas having their origin primarily in feelings and emotions rather than in some characteristics of the stimulating circumstances.” In stereotypes the thoughts and perceptions are mutually entangled.

From the above definitions and deliberations on stereotypes and its nature, the properties of stereotypes can be summarized in the following manner:

1. Stereotypes are basically fixed mental pictures in one’s head.

2. Stereotypes may have some stimulus value, but they are unscientific generalizations.

3. Stereotypes are mostly false elements.

4. Stereotypes are overgeneralized ideas.

5. Stereotypes are linked with emotional experience.

6. Stereotypes are shared by the group.

7. They are mostly negative in nature.

8. Stereotypes originate and grow like attitudes, prejudices and other social concepts.

9. Stereotypes are quite rigid and not easily amenable to change.

10. Stereotypes arise out of ingroup outgroup relationship and personal and group conflicts into which a good deal of fantasy is attached.

11. Stereotypes grow out of social interaction and ones past experience.

12. Stereotypes help in solving current problems and adjust with the present situation in a short time by the already formed readymade ideas.

13. Stereotypes are a type of cognitive framework and to a large degree are self conforming inducing the individual to bring supporting information to mind.

14. Informations supporting a particular stereotype are readily accepted and remembered while rejected informations do not go in the line of stereotypes.

15. Through stereotypes unfavourable traits or adjectives are attributed to the outgroup and favourable traits to the ingroup.

16. The idea of stereotype is based on few facts. It has only stimulus value, but no scientific value. It is usually based on partial truths. All the Kabuliwalas are not Sylocks. Only a few of them may be miser and our experience are limited with a few Kabuliwalas. The generalization about the Kabuliwalas can be found out by making a statistical analysis.

17. Stereotype is a major mechanism in sustaining prejudice and it resists change.

18. Stereotypes influence and colour many of our daily and day to day activities, perceptions and behaviour at large.

According to Lippmann “We do not first see and than define, we define first and then see. In the great blooming, budging confusion of the outer world, we pick out what our culture has already defined for us and we tend to perceive that which we have picked up in the form of stereotypes for us by our culture. Stereotypes are more or less consistent picture of the head to which our habits, tastes, capacities, comforts and hopes have adjusted themselves. They may not be a complete picture of the world. But they are a picture of the possible world to which we are adopted. In that world, people and things, all have their well known places and do certain expected things. We feel at home there we fit in. We are members, we know the way around. There we find the charm of the familiar, the normal, the dependable, its groups and share where we are accustomed to find them.”


2. Nature of Stereotypes:

When the different members of the society interact with the material objects of the external world and with each other, they develop certain ideas, attitudes, and mental pictures towards them. These ideas and attitudes named as cognitive framework developed out of past experience are used for processing and interpreting subsequent social information.

Thus, these readymade ideas otherwise known as schematas, “pictures in our head” or mental picture help one in determining his present behaviour and response mechanism. But, whatever, mental image one formulates about a person or place, idea or event may not be true. These are called stereotypes.

In this modern world, which is passing through conflicts and prejudices, interstate and international tensions, a knowledge of the factor underlying tension and conflicts in the social field may be necessary.

It is essential not only to have mere knowledge of stereotype, but knowledge of stereotype based on the findings of scientific studies and systematic investigations. From this stand point studies on national, racial group and linguistic stereotypes have been considered of tremendous importance.

Due to the presence of certain fixed mental pictures about others, there is misunderstanding at all levels beginning from familial to national and international level. The groups of people accepted by a group due to some reason or other get favourable reaction and it becomes the ingroup.

Sherif and Hovland point out that the attitudes associated with the groups placed in acceptable categories are predominately favourable and attributes associated with reflected groups are largely unfavourable. Thus, the acceptance or rejection of a group is bound up with the nature of stereotypes.

As experience shows every human organism has certain static ideas and fixed mental images in his head of himself as well as others.

Reene said “Mummy says not to play with black children “why”? asked Margaret. “Alas! don’t you know mummy told they are very sabby and illdressed” exclaimed Reene. This is a simple, but mating example of the picture of black children in a white girl’s head. To mental images and fixed kons of this kind Lippman (1920) for the first time attached the level “Stereotypes.”

To Political comme Walter Lippman, who authored the famous title “Public Opinion” (1922) the credit goes for introducing the concept of stereotype to modern psychology.

Stereotypes are the picture in the head that filters the news effect what one notices and how one views it. It simply means denoting any false image of others. It is a posture or gesture which does not change. Etimologically defined the terms stereotype has been derived from the Greek word “stereo” which means solid.

Once they are acquired, stereotypes become fixed conceptions in human mind. If one says, for instance, that Americans are materialistic, Englishmen are formal and diplomatic and Indians are superstitious, he is expressing a stereotyped generalization, a fixed idea about a category of people representing a particular nation or country.

Such generalizations are true to the extent that the whole concept may not be wrong, but the truth may be limited to a few people only and it is a case of over generalization which may be due to emotional causes or ingroup outgroup feeling etc.


3. Causes and Development of Stereotypes:

Stereotypes develop in the same manner attitude and prejudices develop. Social learning and social perception, group norms and reference groups play a tremendous role in the development of stereotypes.

Stereotype is, thus, purely acquired and is solely influence by socio-cultural conditioning. Stereotypes are also based on remours, stories, anecdotes and sometimes actual experience has tremendous additive value in the formation and development of stereotypes.

From the personal point of view stereotypes may have an unconscious self reference. Thus, Allport says that one may imagine his own qualities in a group and hate the group because he is in conflict over the same qualities in himself. Bird says that stereotypes originate more due to the feelings and emotions of the individual with less emphasis in the characteristics of the stimulating circumstances.

Once a girl told her Mummy “Mummy a woman wants to see you” Mummy retorted “do not say woman, say lady”. The small girl replied “But Mummy she is a scheduled caste and you always call them woman”? This shows how stereotypes grow due to social learning and imitation.

In the great blooming budging confusion of the outer world, we pick up what our culture has already defined for us and we tend to perceive that which we have picked up in the form of stereotypes for us by our culture.

Our perception consists of two types of objects:

(a) Perception of natural object

(b) Perception of social object. Perception of the natural object depends upon the objectivity of the stimulus

(c) But we learn from others how to perceive the social objects.

Here the perception is subjective, influenced by preconceived nations. In social perception, we have stereotypes on the basis of what are taught by others.

We are told about the world before we see it. In perception of natural object, we see things as they are, while in perception of social objects we see things as we are or we are told about these things well in advance before we actually see them. Emotional concepts and attitudes are more important for the development of stereotypes than knowledge and familiarity.

Stereotypes grow out of experience. As already indicated a stereotype is an undiscrimated construct which assimilates varying types of experiences into the same pattern on the basis of a minor resemblance or a fallacious similarity. Stereotypes are so persuasive and important that many investigators have tried to explore their psychological basis. Stereotypes are often based on cognitive processes as per recent views.

Recent thinking holds that their functioning results from special processes of thought. As long as a stereotype is in our consciousness an individual is never forced to examine the reasons underlying it. Such stereotypes can be used as an excuse to continue hostility.

Throwing further light on the causes and development of stereotype, Lippman remarks “Each of us lives and works on a small part of earth’s surface, moves in a small circle and of these acquaintances knows only a few intimately. Inevitably our opinion covers a bigger space, a longer reach of time, a greater number of things than we can entirely observe. They have, therefore, to be pieced together out of which others have reported and what we can imagine.”

According to Ruth Benedit (1942) “The first lesson of history in this respect is that when any group in power wishes to persecute or expropriate another group, it uses as justification reasons which are familiar and easily acceptable at the time.” The truth of her view can be realised in the tragic consequences of full blown racism in Nazi Germany bent on world conquest.

Sherif and Sherif (1961) hold “since world was to the racist justification, while still virulent in certain parts of Africa and in the American South has fallen into disfavour in knowledgeable circles.” They further add “attempts to classify people have produced several different classifications, none of which is without inconsistencies. In any event, none of these classifications includes cultural traits or achievements and none includes mental or psychological characteristics”.

Results of several observations, experiences and studies show that undoubtedly there are group differences in behaviour and attitudes. But the origin of such differences can not necessarily be attributed to biological and racial factors.

Though, a few psychologists still hold that differences in psychological test scores by Negroes and whites indicate inherant differences in biological make up, no evidence or empirical data supports the above view. But there are on the other hand many studies which indicate that such differences are not gentical or biological but environmental.


4. Functions of Stereotypes:

The functioning of stereotypes result from special processes of thought. According to Kenneth and Mary (1985) stereotypes help to a great extent in the persistence and sustenance of prejudice. Stereotypes are so rigid that they resist any kind of change in belief and attitude. Differentiation of polarization, negative memory, bias and illusory correlations speak about the way the stereotypes functions.

A common tendency is found to regard stereotypes as a sort of negative attitude. Lay men also think that stereotypes are always bad which signify a kind of defect in social integration.

So, it has been said that stereotypes are baseless and unpleasant and provide us with targets for abuse and socially acceptable objectives for aggression. But this only speaks a part of the nature of stereotype because stereotypes are both good and bad, palatable and unpalatable, liked and disliked and hence serve both good and bad purposes.

Stereotypes play a very powerful role in the development of prejudice and national and international tension. It controls, determines and directs social behaviour.

Eating habits, relationship with parents, family members, seniors and juniors, traditions and customs of marriage celebrations and sexual behaviour etc. are controlled by stereotypes. In addition to this, the behaviour of various castes, creeds and communities are also controlled by various stereotypes.

Stereotypes also influence perception and judgements of social objects. Allport (1954) holds that the stereotype acts both as a justificatory device for categorical acceptance, or rejection of a group and as a screening and selecting device to maintain simplicity in perception and thinking.

Stereotypes may be based on few facts or have little scientific value and may distort or direct human judgement, but not necessarily foolish or based on fantasy. They are convenient to use and act as time savers. But they are not accurate as based on over categorization and over generalization.

Though, the stimulus value of a stereotype is quite less as it is based only on a few cases, it does not always serve negative purpose and it not always social evil. Stereotypes are useful for smooth and cooperative interaction, for developing satisfying relationships, for quick perception and learning and prompt decisions. Many stereotypes appear to contain some grains of truth as viewed by G. Allport (1954).

They are often based on just enough facts for them to be useful in predicting other people actions. Peabody (1985) made a study of stereotypes when people in six different countries like English, Russians, Germans, Americans, French, Italians showed almost unanimous agreement in judging their own and others national character.

Stereotypes are not, therefore completely false or baseless like prejudice. They have some social value and some stimulus value.

However, the most discouraging aspects of the stereotypes are that many of our stereotypes serve emotional and selfish interests and, therefore, go deeper than what actually is. The system of stereotype may be the core of one’s personal culture and the defences of his position in the society.

Many stereotypes may be logically false concepts, but since people live not by logic, but by love and hatred, fear and anger, anxiety and tension, superiority and insecurity, stereotypes have existed, are existing and would exist in future. They are inevitable in human social and personal life.

On has to rely on stereotypes for quick perception and learning and for speedy adjustment with the immediate environment surrounding him. So, stereotypes are necessary. But to avoid the negative consequences of stereotypes, one should be cautious.

Donald and Campbell (1967) say that one must be cautious about:

(i) Overestimation of defences among groups

(ii) Underestimation of the variations within a group and

(iii) Justification of hostility or oppression.


5. Important Studies on Stereotypes:

Due to the tremendous importance of stereotypes in social life several psychologists have been attracted to the study on stereotypes. A few of the available important studies are briefly discussed here.

Goring made a study on the photograph of criminals and judgement of artists and found that the artists’ judgement of criminals was influenced by his idea of criminals. Shereman found a negative correlation between high eye brow and scholastic achievement.

Rice (1926) made a classical study on the effect of stereotype on human judgement, Results indicated that the stereotypes may distort or direct human judgement but are not necessarily false. Katz and Barly (1932) studied the racial and national stereotypes of college students. Katz and Barely (1932) made a famous study on the stereotypes of college students.

They have a list of 84 traits to American college students which they had obtained from the students themselves on a previous occasion and instructed each subject to select 5 traits they considered typical of the various national groups.

Findings of the study indicated a relatively high degree of consistency among the students in assigning traits to various national groups. 84% viewed that Negroes were superstitious and 75% asserted that they were lazy.

They held that 79% of the Jews are mercenary. On the other hand, 48% and 47% of them attributed to Americans as industrious and intelligent respectively. This shows that they attributed unfavourable qualities to the outgroups and favourable qualities to the in group (Americans). Sumner (1906) much earlier to the above study viewed this type of stereotype due to “Ethocentrism”.

According to him, ethno-centrism is “view of things in which one’s own group is the centre of everything and all others are scaled with reference to it………. each group nourishes its own object and affinity, boasts its superiority, exalts its divinity and looks with contempt on outsiders”.

According to Sherif, Kupuswamy and many others, when we do not like the other group, we are biased and usually attribute unfavourable traits to them and when we like groups or persons, we attribute favourable qualities to them. There are only a very few, who are judicious and critical in their view in attaching favourable and unfavourable traits.

In this context Sherif (1956) opines “Infact stereotypes can be taken as an index of social distance…………………………….. the favourableness and unfavourableness of stereotypes attributed to the different groups varies in terms of their position on the social distance scale”.

When some people do not like a particular group or community due to whatever reasons and experiences, they start building up a social distance towards them. This social distance interacts in the pictures in our mind about that group.

The degree of unfavourable qualities increase with the degree of social distance. The more the gap or distance there is greater attribution of unfavourable qualities. The less is the gap, more favourable qualities are attributed. This study reflected some common stereotypes used by the public at large.

Their results were similar to Bogardus’s result in social distance experiment. Zillig made a study on two groups of students, liked and disliked and they were given similar tasks to perform.

Individuals in favour of the liked group reported that the students have performed their task correctly even though they made mistakes. In a study by Horwitz certain beautiful houses and pictures were shown to some whites of South America. Questions were asked “what the coloured woman is doing?” Even though there were no coloured woman, the responses were “she is washing the plates or cleaning the house.”

Blake and Dennis asked some judges to check traits that were characteristics of Negroes and Whites. Gilbert (1950) repeated the experiment by Katz and found that stereotypes grow weaker with time, Stanger did a study on fascist attitude. The role of frame of reference was associated with the meaning of different words. Some studies to be describe.

The problem of stereotype has been studied in India on a large scale by the UNESCO. Rath and Das (1957) conducted a study on the stereotypes of college fresh men in Orissa. The purpose of the study was to find out the stereotypes of Oriya college freshmen towards other nationalities on the basis of admired and derogatory traits.

Sinha and Upadhaya (1960) have conducted a study on group and national stereotypes, Rath and Sircar (1960) on caste stereotype, Kupuswamy and Parashiva Murty on linguistic stereotype.

The present author, Mohanty (1968) conducted a study on the correlation the entire study is to be given between tension and contact in a study of provincial stereotypes on a group of 200 male and female Oriya college students to find out:

(a) The derogatory stereotypes, hostility and tension of any of the sample towards the people of seven other states such as Punjab, Maharashtra, Bihar, Bengal, Assam, Orissa and towards Nepal.

(b) The favourable stereotypes towards the above groups and towards themselves, the correlation between tension and contact of the ingroup i.e. Oriya subjects towards the other outgroups.

(c) When the contact between two groups or nationalities increases, the tension and hostility between them may decrease as each group tries to understand the other with more sympathy and friendliness and his mental picture for such a group may change in the positive direction.

On the contrary, less contact may not give the correct information about the qualities of a group or nationality to another group and hence their fixed notions may not change.

Following the Katz and Barly method of studying stereotypes a pilot study was conducted to prepare the final questionnaire which contained 60 traits out of which 30 were favourable and the remaining 30 were derogatory. Subjects filled up the inventory by checking as many favourable and derogatory traits as they wished for each nationality.

Data showed that Biharis secured the first position in the scale of tension and seventh (last but one) position in the scale of contact being considered as the most hostile group. The Oriya students have put themselves in first ranking in the scale of contact and last in the scale of tension i.e., they thought themselves superior to all other groups.

The other groups such as Assamese, Maharastrian, Bengali, Punjabi, Nepali and South Indians got 2.5, 2.5, 4.4 and 7.0 respectively in the scale of tension and 6.8, 2.3, 5.0 and 4.0 respectively in the scale of contact. Correlation between tension and contact is -.57 which indicated that there is a negative relationship between tension and contact.

This suggested that the more is the contact between two groups, the less is the tension and vice versa. Mohanty (1968) conducted another study on sex differences in linguistic stereotypes among University students which is probably first of its kind in India.

Detailed note on linguistic stereotype be given. The purpose was to a certain:

(a) The degree of uniformity or agreement in assigning traits to various linguistic groups by two groups of male and female university students

(b) The degree of difference between the two sexes in attributing traits to different linguistic groups.

Males and females constitute equal part in a society and take responsibilities in promoting interstate harmony and understanding. Therefore, it is essential to know whether males and females differ in their linguistic stereotypes.

The sample of the study consisted of 2 groups of male and female university students, each groups consisting of 100 subjects. They were matched for educational status, socioeconomic background, age and mother tongue.

The seven linguistic groups included were Punjabee, Madrasi, Behari, Bengali. Assamese, Marahathies, and Oriya. The data was collected following the same procedure as in the earlier study of Mohanty discussed above.

The findings of the study indicated that Beharees and Assamese have been considered as the most unfavourable outgroup by both the male and female groups. Marahatthas were considered as the most liked group securing first position in the scale of favourable traits by both the sexes.

However, the female group demonstrated less hostile tendency towards the Bengaleese than the male group who judged the Bengalees as a hostile group next to Biharees and Assamese. In other cases, the difference between the groups was not significant.

The results further show that some standard stereotypes were attributed to different linguistic groups expressed by both the sexes such as Punjabi—brave and beautiful, Oriya—lazy, Biharee— coward, Assamese—illiterate, Bengali-showy and proud and Marahatthas-brave and honest.

Also difference in male and female groups in certain stereotypes were marked which indicated the type of relationship existing between the rated and the rating group. However, there is more uniformity than diversity in the stereotypes of both the groups towards the linguistic groups.

Both these studies were conducted about 20 years back by (he present author and hence the data relate to the stereotypes and mental pictures existing 20 years back. A follow up study on this problem, perhaps, might show if the same stereotypes are still existing or they have faded or have been completely changed.

A comparison between the data obtained from the second study conducted in 1968 with the study conducted in 1954 on college freshmen of Orissa, however, show some similarity.

Buchanan and Cantril (1953) compared the adjectives used by most persons in representative cross sections in nine European and American countries to describe their own nationalities and found them mostly favourable

“when the German Psychologist Hofstatter (1957) correlated the responses in each country with those in each of the others, he found very high correspondence between the images people hold of their own countries.”

English[edit]

Etymology[edit]

Borrowed from French stéréotype. Printing sense is from 1817, the “conventional, formulaic, and oversimplified conception, opinion, or image” sense is recorded from 1922 in Walter Lippmann’s book Public Opinion.[1]

Pronunciation[edit]

  • IPA(key): /ˈstɛ.ɹi.əˌtaɪp/, /ˈstɪə.ɹi.əˌtaɪp/
  • Audio (Southern England) (file)
  • Audio (Southern England) (file)

Noun[edit]

stereotype (countable and uncountable, plural stereotypes)

  1. A conventional, formulaic, and often oversimplified or exaggerated conception, opinion, or image of (a person or a group of people).
    Synonyms: cliché, platitude
  2. (psychology) A person who is regarded as embodying or conforming to a set image or type.
  3. (printing) A metal printing plate cast from a matrix moulded from a raised printing surface.
    Synonym: cliché
  4. (software engineering) An extensibility mechanism of the Unified Modeling Language, allowing a new element to be derived from an existing one with added specializations.

Derived terms[edit]

  • metastereotype
  • stereotypic
  • stereotypical

Translations[edit]

conventional, formulaic, and oversimplified conception, opinion, or image

  • Arabic: صُورَة نَمَطِيَّة‎ f (ṣūra namaṭiyya)
  • Armenian: կարծրատիպ (hy) (karcratip)
  • Bulgarian: навик (bg) m (navik), стереотип (bg) m (stereotip)
  • Catalan: estereotip (ca) m
  • Chinese:
    Mandarin: 刻板印象 (zh) (kèbǎn yìnxiàng)
  • Czech: stereotyp (cs) m
  • Danish: stereotyp c
  • Dutch: stereotype (nl) n
  • Esperanto: stereotipo, kliŝo (eo)
  • Faroese: javngjørd, stereotýpa
  • Finnish: stereotyyppi (fi), kangistuma (fi), kaavoittuma (fi), stereotypia (fi)
  • French: stéréotype (fr) m or f
  • Galician: estereotipo m
  • German: Stereotyp (de) m, Klischee (de) n, (psych.) Rollenklischee (de) n
  • Greek: στερεότυπο (el) n (stereótypo)
  • Hungarian: sztereotípia (hu)
  • Icelandic: staðalímynd f
  • Indonesian: stéréotipê
  • Italian: stereotipo (it) m
  • Japanese: ステレオタイプ (sutereotaipu)
  • Malay: stereotaip
  • Norwegian:
    Bokmål: stereotyp
    Nynorsk: stereotyp
  • Persian: تفکر قالبی(tafakkor-e qâlebi)
  • Portuguese: estereótipo (pt) m
  • Russian: стереоти́п (ru) m (stereotíp), клише́ (ru) n (klišé)
  • Serbo-Croatian: stereotip (sh) m
  • Slovene: stereotip m
  • Spanish: estereotipo (es) m, tópico (es) m
  • Swedish: stereotyp (sv) c
  • Turkish: basmakalıp söz, (sociology) kalıplaşmış yargı, (psychology) stereotip (tr)

printing plate

  • Armenian: կարծրատիպ (hy) (karcratip)
  • Bulgarian: клише (bg) n (kliše)
  • Catalan: estereotip (ca) m
  • Chinese:
    Mandarin: please add this translation if you can
  • Danish: kliché c, kliche c
  • Finnish: stereotyyppi (fi)
  • French: cliché (fr) m
  • Galician: estereotipo m
  • German: Stereotypie f
  • Greek: στερεοτυπία (el) f (stereotypía)
  • Hungarian: tömöntvény, klisé (hu), sztereotípia (hu), nyomóforma, nyomódúc, dúc (hu), nyomólemez, sablon (hu)
  • Spanish: estereotipo (es) m
  • Turkish: baskı klişesi

extensibility mechanism of UML

  • Chinese:
    Mandarin: please add this translation if you can
  • Finnish: stereotyyppi (fi)
  • French: stéréotype (fr) m
  • German: Stereotyp (de) n
  • Italian: stereotipo (it) m
  • Polish: stereotyp (pl) m
  • Portuguese: estereótipo (pt) m
  • Russian: стереотип (ru) m (stereotip)
  • Spanish: estereotipo (es) m

Verb[edit]

stereotype (third-person singular simple present stereotypes, present participle stereotyping, simple past and past participle stereotyped)

  1. (transitive) To make a stereotype of someone or something, or characterize someone by a stereotype.
    • 1957, Karl Popper, chapter 24, in The Poverty of Historicism, FIRST HARPER TORCH BOOK edition, page 90:

      Unable to ascertain what is in the minds of so many individuals, he must try to simplify his problems by eliminating individual differences: he must try to control and stereotype interests and beliefs by education and propaganda.

  2. (transitive, printing) To prepare for printing in stereotype; to produce stereotype plates of.

    to stereotype the Bible

  3. (transitive, printing) To print from a stereotype.
  4. (transitive, figurative) To make firm or permanent; to fix.
    • 1887, George Campbell, 8th Duke of Argyll, Scotland as it was and as it is
      Powerful causes tending to stereotype and aggravate the poverty of old conditions.

Translations[edit]

print from a stereotype

  • Chinese:
    Mandarin: please add this translation if you can
  • Finnish: stereotypioida
  • Irish: plátáil

References[edit]

  1. ^ Douglas Harper (2001–2023), “stereotype”, in Online Etymology Dictionary, retrieved 12 November 2020.

Swedish[edit]

Adjective[edit]

stereotype

  1. absolute definite natural masculine singular of stereotyp.

What do we mean by stereotype?

A conventional, formulaic, and oversimplified conception, opinion, or image. noun

One that is regarded as embodying or conforming to a set image or type. noun

A metal printing plate cast from a matrix molded from a raised printing surface, such as type. noun

To make a stereotype of. transitive verb

To characterize by a stereotype. transitive verb

To give a fixed, unvarying form to. transitive verb

To print from a stereotype. transitive verb

To cast a stereotype plate from: as, to stereotype a page or a form.

To prepare for printing by means of stereotype plates: as, to stereotype the New Testament.

To fix or establish firmly or unchangeably.

The duplicate, in one piece of type metal, of the face of a. collection of types composed for printing. noun

Loosely, an electrotype. noun

The art of making plates of fixed metallic types; the process of producing printed work by means of such plates. noun

Of or pertaining to stereotype, or stereotyping, or stereotype printing: as, stereotype work: stereotype plates.

A plate forming an exact faximile of a page of type or of an engraving, used in printing books, etc.; specifically, a plate with type-metal face, used for printing. noun

The art or process of making such plates, or of executing work by means of them. noun

A block, usually of wood, to which a stereotype plate is attached while being used in printing. noun

To prepare for printing in stereotype; to make the stereotype plates of. transitive verb

Fig.: To make firm or permanent; to fix. transitive verb

A conventional, formulaic, and oversimplified conception, opinion, or image. noun

A conventional, formulaic, and often oversimplified or exaggerated conception, opinion, or image of (a person).

A person who is regarded as embodying or conforming to a set image or type.

A metal printing plate cast from a matrix moulded from a raised printing surface.

(UML) An extensibility mechanism of the Unified Modeling Language, allowing a new element to be derived from an existing one with added specializations.

An overly simplified, negative description of a people group with a high statistical probability of being true that is inexpedient to progressive platforms. The new regressive left would like you to blind yourself from the fact that these may ring true, which is preventing people from coming together to have an honest dialogue about causality and realistic solutions. Note that the term «stereotype» will not be applied to overly simplified and probable generalizations that are either supportive of progressive policy conclusions, or demonize/dismiss the opposition. Examples of these include: «All Republicans are older, wealthy white men.» «Trump supporters are uneducated and uninformed.» «Women don’t make as much as men because of institutionalized sexism.» Urban Dictionary

N. a small kernel of truth that lies at the bottom of a pillowcase full of shit. Urban Dictionary

A term used to define all people of a certain belief into a mostly negative category that may only reflect a selected few of the racial demographics. All people of all nationalities have been victims of being stereotyped, even those whom have made most of the stereotypes of other people.
Note: Most stereotypes are involuntarily lived out by a small segment of who they are made to offend. Here are a few examples of stereotypes. Urban Dictionary

A stereotype is used to catergorize a group of people. People don’t understand that type of person, so they put them into classifications, thinking that everyone who is that needs to be like that, or anyone who acts like their classifications is one. Urban Dictionary

98% of the «definitions» on this website.
See: ‘ICP’
People should stop posting how much they hate stuff. It’s not hard, just dont listen to it. Urban Dictionary

A stereotype is a label given to a person, a prequisite judgement. It goes hand in hand with bias, prejudice, and discrimination. Urban Dictionary

An assumption of a certain group of ppl. the simpsons area perfect example of a stereotypical town. where homer is the fat lazy husband with a dead end job, marge is the housewife that doesnt have a life outside the house, bart is the trouble maker boy, lisa is the forgotten, misunderstood girl, maggie is the quiet baby who never speaks, mr. burns is the evil bossman, apu is the foreign shop keeper, and the polie are lazy ppl who eat donuts all day. all the other characters are self explanitory.
stereotypes seem to be an alternative to ppl who dun weanna get to know other ppl. so they classify them with out gettin the facts. Urban Dictionary

Another word for truth.
Everyone likes to act like it’s a bad thing, so they can tell you not to speak truth.
Sometimes, they are incorrect, but 99% of the time, it’s exactly what you think. Urban Dictionary

A belief based on similarities between people of the same race, class, or religion. Usually absolutly true about most of that group, but offends stupid people. Urban Dictionary

A stereotype is someone who judges someone by how they look, their race, how they dress, thier ‘label’, etc. Urban Dictionary

A stereotype is a popular belief about specific social groups or types of individuals. The concepts of «stereotype» and «prejudice» are often confused with many other different meanings. Stereotypes are standardized and simplified conceptions of groups based on some prior assumptions.

Contents

  • 1 Etymology
  • 2 Dynamics
    • 2.1 Theories on stereotypes
  • 3 Effects, accuracy, terminology
  • 4 Role in art and culture
  • 5 Stereotypes
  • 6 See also
  • 7 References
  • 8 Bibliography
  • 9 External links

Etymology

The term stereotype derives from the Greek words στερεός (stereos), «firm, solid»[1] and τύπος (typos), «impression,»[2] hence «solid impression».

It was invented by Firmin Didot in the world of printing; it was originally a duplicate impression of an original typographical element, used for printing instead of the original. American journalist Walter Lippmann coined the metaphor, calling a stereotype a «picture in our heads» saying, «Whether right or wrong (…) imagination is shaped by the pictures seen (…) originally printers’ words, and in their literal printers’ meanings were synonymous. Specifically, cliché was a French word for the printing surface for a stereotype.[3] The first reference to «stereotype,» in its modern, English use was in 1850, in the noun, meaning «image perpetuated without change.»[4]

The term, in its modern psychology sense, was first used by Walter Lippmann in his 1922 work Public Opinion[5] although in the printing sense it was first coined in 1798.

Dynamics

In one perspective of the stereotyping process, there are the concepts of ingroups and outgroups. From each individual’s perspective, ingroups are viewed as normal and superior, and are generally the group that they already associate with, or aspire to join. An outgroup is simply all the other groups. They are seen as lesser than or inferior to the in-groups. An example of this would be: Asians are smarter than Americans. In this example Asians are looked at as being smarter because their education systems are more strict than that of the Americans.

A second perspective is that of automatic and explicit or subconscious and conscious. Automatic or subconscious stereotyping is that which everyone does without noticing. Automatic stereotyping is quickly preceded by an explicit or conscious check which permits time for any needed corrections. Automatic stereotyping is affected by explicit stereotyping because frequent conscious thoughts will quickly develop into subconscious stereotypes.

A third method to categorizing stereotypes is general types and sub-types. Stereotypes consist of hierarchical systems consisting of broad and specific groups being the general types and sub-types respectively. A general type could be defined as a broad stereotype typically known among many people and usually widely accepted, whereas the sub-group would be one of the several groups making up the general group. These would be more specific, and opinions of these groups would vary according to differing perspectives.

Certain circumstances can affect the way an individual stereotypes. Some theorists argue in favor of the conceptual connection and that one’s own subjective thought about someone is sufficient information to make assumptions about that individual. Other theorists argue that at minimum there must be a causal connection between mental states and behavior to make assumptions or stereotypes. Thus results and opinions may vary according to circumstance and theory. An example of a common, incorrect assumption is that of assuming certain internal characteristics based on external appearance. The explanation for one’s actions is his or her internal state (goals, feeling, personality, traits, motives, values, and impulses), not his or her appearance.

Sociologist Charles E. Hurst, «One reason for stereotypes is the lack of personal, concrete familiarity that individuals have with persons in other racial or ethnic groups. Lack of familiarity encourages the lumping together of unknown individuals.»[6]

Stereotypes focus upon and thereby exaggerate differences between groups. Competition between groups minimizes similarities and magnifies differences.[7] This makes it seem as if groups are very different when in fact they may be more alike than different. For example, among African Americans, identity as an American citizen is more salient than racial background; that is, African Americans are more American than African.[8]

Theories on stereotypes

Different disciplines give different accounts of how stereotypes develop: Psychologists may focus on an individual’s experience with groups, patterns of communication about those groups, and intergroup conflict. Pioneering psychologist William James cautioned psychologists themselves to be wary of their own stereotyping, in what he called the psychologist’s fallacy. Sociologists focus on the relations among different groups in a social structure. Psychoanalytically-oriented humanists (e.g., Sander Gilman) have argued that stereotypes, by definition, are representations that are not accurate, but a projection of one to another.

A number of theories have been derived from sociological studies of stereotyping and prejudicial thinking. In early studies it was believed that stereotypes were only used by rigid, repressed, and authoritarian people. Sociologists concluded that this was a result of conflict, poor parenting, and inadequate mental and emotional development. This idea has been overturned; more recent studies have concluded that stereotypes are commonplace.

One theory as to why people stereotype is that it is too difficult to take in all of the complexities of other people as individuals. Even though stereotyping is inexact, it is an efficient way to mentally organize large blocks of information. Categorization is an essential human capability because it enables us to simplify, predict, and organize our world. Once one has sorted and organized everyone into tidy categories, there is a human tendency to avoid processing new or unexpected information about each individual. Assigning general group characteristics to members of that group saves time and satisfies the need to predict the social world in a general sense.

Another theory is that people stereotype because of the need to feel good about oneself. Stereotypes protect one from anxiety and enhance self-esteem. By designating one’s own group as the standard or normal group and assigning others to groups considered inferior or abnormal, it provides one with a sense of worth.

Some psychologist believe that childhood influences are some of the most complex and influential factors in developing stereotypes. Though they can be absorbed at any age, stereotypes are usually acquired in early childhood under the influence of parents, teachers, peers, and the media. Once a stereotype is learned, it often becomes self-perpetuating.

Other theories propose that the praising of intelligence and ability rather than effort and hard work inevitably changes the prospective from a malleable sense of self-worth to a definite concept of self-worth as seen from the individual and others around them.

Another prominent theory is the stereotype content model which attempts to predict behavior based on levels of warmth and competence.

Effects, accuracy, terminology

Stereotypes can have a negative and positive impact on individuals. Joshua Aronson and Claude M. Steele have done research on the psychological effects of stereotyping, particularly its effect on African Americans and women.[9] They argue that psychological research has shown that competence is highly responsive to situation and interactions with others.[10] They cite, for example, a study which found that bogus feedback to college students dramatically affected their IQ test performance, and another in which students were either praised as very smart, congratulated on their hard work, or told that they scored high. The group praised as smart performed significantly worse than the others. They believe that there is an ‘innate ability bias’. These effects are not just limited to minority groups. Mathematically competent white males, mostly math and engineering students, were asked to take a difficult math test. One group was told that this was being done to determine why Asians were scoring better. This group performed significantly worse than the control group.[10]:443

Possible prejudicial effects of stereotypes are:

  • Justification of ill-founded prejudices or ignorance
  • Unwillingness to rethink one’s attitudes and behavior towards stereotyped group
  • Preventing some people of stereotyped groups from entering or succeeding in activities or fields

Stereotypes allow individuals to make better informed evaluations of individuals about whom they possess little or no individuating information, and in many, but not all circumstances stereotyping helps individuals arrive at more accurate conclusions.[11] Over time, some victims of negative stereotypes display self-fulfilling prophecy behavior, in which they assume that the stereotype represents norms to emulate. Negative effects may include forming inaccurate opinions of people, scapegoating, erroneously judgmentalism, preventing emotional identification, distress, and impaired performance.

Yet, the stereotype that stereotypes are inaccurate, resistant to change, overgeneralized, exaggerated, and destructive is not founded on empirical social science research, which instead shows that stereotypes are often accurate and that people do not rely on stereotypes when relevant personal information is available.[12] Indeed, Jussim et al. comment that ethnic and gender stereotypes are surprisingly accurate, while stereotypes concerning political affiliation and nationality [13] are much less accurate; the stereotypes assessed for accuracy concerned intelligence, behavior, personality, and economic status.[11] Stereotype accuracy is a growing area of study and for Yueh-Ting Lee and his colleagues they have created an EPA Model (Evaluation, Potency, Accuracy) to describe the continuously changing variables of stereotypes.

Role in art and culture

Stereotypes are common in various cultural media, where they take the form of dramatic stock characters. These characters are found in the works of playwright Bertold Brecht, Dario Fo, and Jacques Lecoq, who characterize their actors as stereotypes for theatrical effect. In commedia dell’arte this is similarly common. The instantly recognizable nature of stereotypes mean that they are effective in advertising and situation comedy. These stereotypes change, and in modern times only a few of the stereotyped characters shown in John Bunyan’s The Pilgrim’s Progress would be recognizable.

In literature and art, stereotypes are clichéd or predictable characters or situations. Throughout history, storytellers have drawn from stereotypical characters and situations, in order to connect the audience with new tales immediately. Sometimes such stereotypes can be sophisticated, such as Shakespeare’s Shylock in The Merchant of Venice. Arguably a stereotype that becomes complex and sophisticated ceases to be a stereotype per se by its unique characterization. Thus while Shylock remains politically unstable in being a stereotypical Jew, the subject of prejudicial derision in Shakespeare’s era, his many other detailed features raise him above a simple stereotype and into a unique character, worthy of modern performance. Simply because one feature of a character can be categorized as being typical does not make the entire character a stereotype.

Despite their proximity in etymological roots, cliché and stereotype are not used synonymously in cultural spheres. For example a cliché is a high criticism in narratology where genre and categorization automatically associates a story within its recognizable group. Labeling a situation or character in a story as typical suggests it is fitting for its genre or category. Whereas declaring that a storyteller has relied on cliché is to pejoratively observe a simplicity and lack of originality in the tale. To criticize Ian Fleming for a stereotypically unlikely escape for James Bond would be understood by the reader or listener, but it would be more appropriately criticized as a cliché in that it is overused and reproduced. Narrative genre relies heavily on typical features to remain recognizable and generate meaning in the reader/viewer.

In movies and TV the halo effect is often used. This is when, for example, attractive men and women are assumed to be happier, stronger, nicer people .[14]

Stereotypes

African
North and South American;

Main articles: Stereotypes of Hispanics, Stereotypes of Native Americans, and Stereotypes of groups within the United States

East Asian and South Asian
European

See also: List of terms for white people in non-Western cultures

Middle Eastern, West and Central Asian
Jewish

Main article: Stereotypes of Jews

Sexual orientation
Gender
Other
  • Nurse stereotypes
  • Animal stereotypes
  • Ethnic stereotype
  • Physical attractiveness stereotype

See also

  • Archetype
  • Assumption
  • Attribute substitution
  • Attributional bias
  • Cognitive bias
  • Communication noise
  • Counterstereotype (antonym)
  • Face-ism
  • Idée fixe (psychology)
  • Implicit stereotypes
  • Labeling theory
  • Milieu control
  • Scapegoating
  • Self-Stereotyping
  • Social control theory
  • Stigmatization
  • Negativity effect
  • Outgroup homogeneity bias
  • Trait ascription bias

References

  1. ^ στερεός, Henry George Liddell, Robert Scott, A Greek-English Lexicon, on Perseus Digital Library
  2. ^ τύπος, Henry George Liddell, Robert Scott, A Greek-English Lexicon, on Perseus Digital Libray
  3. ^ <Merriam-Webster’s Dictionary of English Usage.> Springfield, Illinois: Merriam-Webster, Inc., 1994. p. 250.
  4. ^ Online Etymology Dictionary
  5. ^ Milton Kleg (August 1993). Hate Prejudice and Racism. State University of New York Press. ISBN 079141535X.
  6. ^ Hurst, Charles E. Social Inequality: Forms, Causes, and. 6. Boston: Pearson Education, Inc, 2007
  7. ^ Brewer, M (1979). «In-group bias in the minimal intergroup situation: A cognitive-motivational analysis». Psychological Bulletin 86: 307–324. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.307.
  8. ^ McAndrew, FT; Akande, A (1995). «African of Americans of African and European descent». Journal of Social Psychology 135 (5): 649–655. doi:10.1080/00224545.1995.9712238.
  9. ^ Steele CM, Aronson J (November 1995). «Stereotype threat and the intellectual test performance of African Americans». J Pers Soc Psychol 69 (5): 797–811. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.69.5.797. PMID 7473032. http://content.apa.org/journals/psp/69/5/797.
  10. ^ a b Aronson J, Steele CM. (2005). Chapter 24:Stereotypes and the Fragility of Academic Competence, Motivation, and Self-Concept. In Handbook of Competence, [ p. 436].
  11. ^ a b Todd D. Nelson, ed (February 2009). The unbearable accuracy of stereotypes in Handbook of prejudice, stereotyping, and discrimination. Psychology Press. ISBN 978-0-8058-5952-2.
  12. ^ Yueh-Ting Lee, Lee J. Jussim, and Clark R. McCauley, ed (September 1995). Stereotype Accuracy: Toward Appreciating Group Differences. American Psychological Association. ISBN 978-1-55798-307-7.
  13. ^ Terracciano, A (2005). «National character does not reflect mean personality trait levels in 49 cultures.». Science 310 (5745): 96–100.. doi:10.1126/science.1117199. PMC 2775052. PMID 16210536. http://www.sciencemag.org/content/310/5745/96.long.
  14. ^ Greenwald and Banaji from Psychological Review

Bibliography

  • Stuart Ewen, Elizabeth Ewen, Typecasting: On the Arts and Sciences of Human Inequality. New York (Seven Stories Press) 2006
  • Stereotype & Society A Major Resource: Constantly updated and archived
  • Social Psychology Network Stereotyping
  • Media Awareness Network. What is a stereotype? Definition, role of stereotyping in the media, more links
  • Regenberg, Nina (2007). «Are Blonds Really Dumb?». In mind (magazine) (3). http://beta.in-mind.org/issue-3/are-blonds-really-dumb
  • Are Stereotypes True?
  • Stereotype Susceptibility: Identity Salience and Shifts in Quantitative Performance, Margaret Shih, Todd L. Pittinsky, Nalini Ambady Research about the effects of ‘positive’ and negative stereotypes on encouraging/discouraging performance.
  • Turner, Chris (2004). Planet Simpson: How a Cartoon Masterpiece Documented an Era and Defined a Generation. Toronto: Random House Canada.
  • Crawford, M. & Unger, R. (2004). Women and Gender: A Feminist Psychology. McGraw Hill New York. New York. 45-49.*
  • Spitzer, B.L., Henderson, K, A., & Zavian, M. T. (1999). Gender differences in population versus media body sizes: A comparison over four decades. Sex Roles, 40, 545-565.*
  • Terracciano A, Abdel-Khalek AM, Adám N, et al. (Oct 2005). «National character does not reflect mean personality trait levels in 49 cultures». Science 310 (5745): 96–100. doi:10.1126/science.1117199. PMC 2775052. PMID 16210536. http://www.pubmedcentral.nih.gov/articlerender.fcgi?tool=pmcentrez&artid=2775052.

External links

  • Social Psychology Network Stereotyping
  • Understanding Stereotypes — Educational information about stereotypes.
  • SEPARATING the GOLD from the DROSS: a guide to multicultural literature
  • Media Awareness Network. What is a stereotype? Definition, role of stereotyping in the media, more links

стереотип, стереотипность, стереотипный, шаблонный, стереотипировать

существительное

- стереотип

stereotype edition — стереотипное издание

- стереотипия
- стереотипность, шаблон, стандарт; избитость
- физиол. стереотип

глагол

- стереотипировать
- печатать со стереотипа
- придавать шаблонность, делать избитым; опошлять

Мои примеры

Словосочетания

the stereotype of the absentminded professor — стереотип рассеянного профессора  
women who don’t fit the stereotype of the good mother — женщины, которые не соответствуют стереотипному представлению о хорошей матери  
the stereotype of the cold, standoffish Brit — стереотип холодного, чопорного британца  
stereotype ideas — стереотипные представления  
plane for bevelling stereotype metal — рубанок для снятия фацета на стереотипах  
plaster stereotype process — процесс изготовления стереотипных гипсовых матриц  
plastic stereotype — пластмассовый стереотип  
preregistering of stereotype mats — предварительная приводка стереотипных матриц  
round stereotype — круглый стереотип  
rubber stereotype — резиновый стереотип  
semicylindrical plate stereotype press — машина для печатания с полукруглых стереотипов  
social stereotype — социальный стереотип  

Примеры с переводом

The church stereotypes a system of faith.

Церковь развивает стереотипное представление о понятиях веры.

Homeless people are stereotyped as alcoholics or addicts.

Существует стереотип о том, что все бездомные либо алкоголики, либо наркоманы.

I was stereotyped as a lazy Southern European

Меня шаблонно воспринимали ленивым жителем Южной Европы.

Joseph does not conform to the stereotype of a policeman.

Джозеф не соответствует расхожему мнению о полицейских.

It’s not fair to stereotype a whole group of people based on one person you don’t like.

Несправедливо считать всю группу людей похожей на одного из них, который тебе не нравится.

Regional stereotypes have been part of America since its founding.

Местные стереотипы являются частью Америки с самого момента её основания.

The noble savage was a stereotype that appealed to 18th-century intellectuals, who viewed European civilization as decadent and corrupt.

Благородный дикарь был стереотипом, который нравился интеллектуалам восемнадцатого века, считавшим европейскую цивилизацию упадочной и испорченной.

Возможные однокоренные слова

Формы слова

noun
ед. ч.(singular): stereotype
мн. ч.(plural): stereotypes

Like this post? Please share to your friends:
  • The word stems from
  • The word steel in a sentence
  • The word stay strong
  • The word stay in a sentence
  • The word status mean