Curious children gather around photographer Toni Frissell, looking at her camera (c. 1945)
Curiosity (from Latin cūriōsitās, from cūriōsus «careful, diligent, curious», akin to cura «care») is a quality related to inquisitive thinking such as exploration, investigation, and learning, evident by observation in humans and animals.[1][2] Curiosity is heavily associated with all aspects of human development, in which derives the process of learning and desire to acquire knowledge and skill.[3]
The term curiosity can also be used to denote the behavior or emotion of being curious, in regard to the desire to gain knowledge or information. Curiosity as a behavior and emotion is attributed over millennia as the driving force behind not only human development, but developments in science, language, and industry.[4]
Causes[edit]
Children peer over shoulders to see what their friends are reading.
Curiosity can be seen as an innate quality of many different species. It is common to human beings at all ages from infancy[5] through adulthood,[1] and is easy to observe in many other animal species; these include apes, cats, and rodents.[2] Early definitions cite curiosity as a motivated desire for information.[6] This motivational desire has been said to stem from a passion or an appetite for knowledge, information, and understanding.
These traditional ideas of curiosity have recently expanded to look at the difference between perceptual curiosity as the innate exploratory behavior that is present in all animals and epistemic curiosity as the desire for knowledge that is specifically attributed to humans.[7]
Daniel Berlyne[8] recognized three major variables playing a role in evoking curiosity; namely, psychophysical variables, ecological variables, and collative variables. Psychophysical variables correspond to physical intensity, while ecological variables to motivational significance and task relevance. Collative variables are called “collative” because they involve a comparison between different stimuli or features, which may be actually perceived or which may be recalled from memory. Berlyne mentioned four collative variables; namely, novelty, complexity, uncertainty, and conflict. At the same time, he suggested that all collative variables probably involve conflict. Additionally, he considered three variables supplementary to novelty, namely change, surprisingness, and incongruity. Finally, curiosity may not only be aroused by the perception of some stimulus associated with the aforementioned variables («specific exploration»), but also by a lack of stimulation, out of “boredom” («diversive exploration»).[8]
Curiosity-driven behavior[edit]
Curiosity-driven behavior is often defined as behavior through which knowledge is gained, and should therefore encompass all behaviors that provide access to or increase sensory information. Berlyne[8] divided curiosity-driven behavior into three categories; namely, orienting responses, locomotor exploration, and investigatory responses, or, investigatory manipulation. Previously, Berlyne[9] had already suggested that curiosity also includes verbal activities, such as asking questions, and symbolic activities, consisting of internally fueled mental processes such as thinking («epistemic exploration»).
Theories[edit]
Like other desires and need states that take on an appetitive quality (e.g. food), curiosity is linked with exploratory behavior and experiences of reward. Curiosity can be described as positive emotions and acquiring knowledge; when one’s curiosity has been aroused it is considered inherently rewarding and pleasurable. Discovering new information may also be rewarding because it can help reduce undesirable states of uncertainty rather than stimulating interest. Theories have arisen in attempts to further understand this need to rectify states of uncertainty and the desire to participate in pleasurable experiences of exploratory behaviors.
Curiosity-drive theory[edit]
Curiosity-drive theory relates to the undesirable experiences of «uncertainty». The reduction of these unpleasant feelings, in turn, is rewarding. This theory suggests that people desire coherence and understanding in their thought processes. When this coherence is disrupted by something that is unfamiliar, uncertain, or ambiguous, it is curiosity-drive that attempts to gather information and knowledge of the unfamiliar to restore coherent thought processes. Through this theory, the general concept dictates that curiosity is developed strictly out of the desire to make sense of unfamiliar aspects of one’s environment through interaction of exploratory behaviors. Once understanding of the unfamiliar has been achieved and coherence has been restored, these behaviors and desires will subside.
Subsets of curiosity-drive theory differ on whether curiosity is a primary or secondary drive and if this curiosity-drive is originated due to one’s need to make sense of and regulate their environment or if it is caused by an external stimulus.[10] Causes can range from basic needs that need to be satisfied (e.g. hunger, thirst) to needs in fear induced situations.[10] Each of these subset theories state that whether the need is primary or secondary curiosity is developed from experiences that create a sensation of uncertainty or perceived unpleasantness. Curiosity then acts as a means in which to dispel this uncertainty. By exhibiting curious and exploratory behavior, one is able to gain knowledge of the unfamiliar and thus reduce the state of uncertainty or unpleasantness. This theory, however, does not address the idea that curiosity can often be displayed even in the absence of new or unfamiliar situations.[11] This type of exploratory behavior is common in many species. Take the example of a human toddler who, if bored in his current situation devoid of arousing stimuli, will walk about until something interesting is found. The observation of curiosity even in the absence of novel stimuli pinpoints one of the major shortcomings in the curiosity-drive model.
Optimal-arousal theory[edit]
Optimal-arousal theory developed out of the need to explain the desire for some to seek out opportunities to engage in exploratory behaviors without the presence of uncertain or ambiguous situations. Optimal-arousal theory attempts to explain this aspect of curiosity by suggesting that one can be motivated to maintain a pleasurable sense of arousal through these exploratory behaviors.
The concept of optimal-arousal of curiosity suggests that there is a tendency to maintain an optimal level of arousal.[8] When a stimulus is encountered that is associated with complexity, uncertainty, conflict, or novelty, this will increase arousal, and exploratory behavior is employed to learn about that stimulus and thereby reduce arousal again. In contrast, if the environment is boring and lacks excitement, arousal is reduced and exploratory behavior will be engaged in order to increase information input and stimulation, and thereby increasing arousal again. This theory addresses both curiosity elicited by uncertain or unfamiliar situations and curiosity elicited in the absence of such situations.
Cognitive-consistency theory[edit]
Cognitive-consistency theories assume that «when two or more simultaneously active cognitive structures are logically inconsistent, arousal is increased, which activates processes with the expected consequence of increasing consistency and decreasing arousal.»[12] Similar to optimal-arousal theory, cognitive-consistency theory suggests that there is a tendency to maintain arousal at a preferred, or expected, level, but it also explicitly links the amount of arousal to the amount of experienced inconsistency between an expected situation and the actually perceived situation. When this inconsistency is small, exploratory behavior triggered by curiosity is employed to gather information with which expectancy can be updated through learning to match perception, thereby reducing inconsistency.[6][12][13] This approach puts curiosity in a broader perspective, also involving aggression and fear. That is, if the inconsistency is larger, fear or aggressive behavior may be employed to alter the perception in order to make it match expectancy, depending on the size of the inconsistency as well as the specific context. Aggressive behavior is assumed to alter perception by forcefully manipulating it into matching the expected situation, while uninhibited fear results in fleeing, thereby removing the inconsistent stimulus from the perceptual field and resolving the inconsistency.[12]
Integration of the reward pathway into theory[edit]
Taking into account the shortcomings of both curiosity-drive and optimal-arousal theories, attempts have been made to integrate neurobiological aspects of reward, wanting, and pleasure into a more comprehensive theory for curiosity. Research suggests that the act of wanting and desiring new information directly involves mesolimbic pathways of the brain that directly account for dopamine activation. The use of these pathways and dopamine activation may account for the assigning of value to new information and then interpreting as reward.[10][14][15] This aspect of neurobiology can accompany curiosity-drive theory in motivating exploratory behavior.
Role of neurological aspects and structures[edit]
Although the phenomenon of curiosity is widely regarded, its root causes are relatively unknown beyond theory. However, recent studies have provided some insight into the neurological mechanisms that make up what is known as the reward pathway[16] which may impact characteristics associated with curiosity, such as learning, memory, and motivation. Due to the complex nature of curiosity, research that focuses on specific neural processes with these characteristics can help create a better understanding of the phenomenon of curiosity as a whole. The following are characteristics of curiosity and their links to neural aspects that can be thought of as essential in creating exploratory behaviors.
Motivation and reward[edit]
Dopamine pathway in the brain
The drive to learn new information or perform some action is often initiated by the anticipation of reward. In this way, the concepts of motivation and reward are naturally tied to the notion of curiosity.[14]
This idea of reward is defined as the positive reinforcement of an action that encourages a particular behavior by using the emotional sensations of relief, pleasure, and satisfaction that correlate with happiness. Many areas in the brain are used to process reward and come together to form what is called the reward pathway. In this pathway many neurotransmitters play a role in the activation of the reward sensation, including dopamine, serotonin and opioid chemicals.[14]
Dopamine is linked to the process of curiosity, as it is responsible for assigning and retaining reward values of information gained. Research suggests higher amounts of dopamine is released when the reward is unknown and the stimulus is unfamiliar, compared to activation of dopamine when stimulus is familiar.[14]
Nucleus accumbens[edit]
The nucleus accumbens is a formation of neurons and is important in reward pathway activation. As previously mentioned, the reward pathway is an integral part in the induction of curiosity. The release of dopamine in investigating response to novel or exciting stimuli. The fast dopamine release observed during childhood and adolescence is important in development, as curiosity and exploratory behavior are the largest facilitators of learning during early years.
In addition, the sensation pleasure of «liking» can occur when opioids are released by nucleus accumbens. This helps someone evaluate the unfamiliar situation or environment and attach value to the novel object. These processes of both wanting and liking play a role in activating the reward system of the brain, and perhaps in the stimulation of curious or information-seeking tendencies as well.[11][15][17]
Caudate nucleus[edit]
The caudate nucleus, is a region of the brain that is highly responsive to dopamine. The caudate nucleus is another component of the reward pathway. Research has suggested that the role of the caudate nucleus is to anticipate the possibility and reward of exploratory behavior and gathered information, thus contributing to factors of curiosity.[17][18]
Anterior cortices[edit]
Regions of the anterior cortices correspond to both conflict and arousal and, as such, seem to reinforce certain exploratory models of curiosity.[19]
Cortisol[edit]
Cortisol is a chemical known for its role in stress regulation. However, cortisol may also be associated with curious or exploratory behavior. Findings in recent studies suggesting the role of cortisol with curiosity support the idea of optimal arousal theory. It is suggested the release of a small amount cortisol causing stress encourages curious behavior, while too much stress can initiate a «back away» response.[18][20]
Attention[edit]
Attention is important to the understanding of curiosity because it directly correlates with one’s abilities to selectively focus and concentrate on particular stimuli in the surrounding environment. As there are limited cognitive and sensory resources to understand and evaluate various stimuli, attention allows the brain to better focus on what it perceives to be the most important or relevant of these stimuli. Individuals tend to focus their energies on stimuli that are particularly stimulating or engaging. Indicating that the more attention a stimulus garners, the more frequent one’s energy and focus will be directed towards that stimulus. This idea suggests an individual will focus their attention on new or unfamiliar stimuli in an effort to better understand or make sense of the unknown over the more familiar or repetitive stimuli, creating the idea that curiosity demands attention.[21]
Striatum[edit]
The striatum is a part of the brain which coordinates motivation with body movement. It would seem natural that the striatum plays a role in attention and reward anticipation, both of which are important in the provocation of curiosity.[19]
Precuneus[edit]
The precuneus is a region of the brain that is involved in attention, episodic memory, and visuospatial processing. There has been a correlation found between the amount of grey matter in the precuneus and levels of curious and exploratory behaviors; suggesting that the precuneus density has an influence on levels of curiosity.[22]
Memory and learning[edit]
Memory plays an important role in the understanding of curiosity. If curiosity is the desire to seek out and understand unfamiliar or novel stimuli, one’s memory is important in determining if the stimulus is indeed unfamiliar.
Memory is the process by which the brain can store and access information. In order to determine if the stimulus is novel, an individual must remember if the stimulus has been encountered before. Thus, memory plays an integral role in dictating the level of novelty or unfamiliarity, and the level of need for curiosity.
It can also be suggested that curiosity can affect memory. As previously mentioned, stimuli that are novel tend to capture more of our attention. Additionally, novel stimuli usually have a reward value associated with them, the anticipated reward of what learning that new information may bring. With stronger associations and more attention devoted to a stimulus, it is probable that the memory formed from that stimulus will be longer lasting and easier to recall, both of which facilitate better learning.
Hippocampus and the parahippocampal gyrus[edit]
The hippocampus is important in memory formation and recall and therefore instrumental in determining the novelty of various stimuli.[23] Research suggests the hippocampus is involved in generating the underlying motivation to explore for the purpose of learning.[2][24][25]
The parahippocampal gyrus (PHG), the area of grey matter surrounding the hippocampus, has recently been implicated in the process of curiosity. This finding suggests that the PHG may be involved in the amplification of curiosity more so than the primary induction of curiosity.[17]
Amygdala[edit]
The amygdala often is associated with emotional processing, particularly for the emotion of fear, as well as memory. It is suggested the amygdala is important in processing emotional reactions towards novel or unexpected stimuli and the induction of exploratory behavior. This implies a potential connection between curiosity levels and the amygdala. However, more research is needed on direct correlation.[26]
Early development[edit]
Jean Piaget is considered to be the most influential child researcher. He argued that babies and children are constantly trying to make sense of their reality and that it contributed to their intellectual development. According to Piaget, children develop hypotheses, conduct experiments and then reassess their hypotheses depending on what they observe. Piaget was the first to closely document children’s actions and interpret them as consistent, calculated effort to test and learn about their environment.[27]
There is no universally accepted definition for curiosity in children. Most research on curiosity has been focused on adults and which typically used self-report measures are inappropriate and inapplicable for studying children. Curiosity is mostly thought of as attributable to a mature person and is characterized in young children as a fledgling feature of their outlook on the world.[28]
Exploratory behaviour is commonly observed in children and is associated with their curiosity development. Several studies look at children’s curiosity by simply observing their interaction with novel and familiar toys.[28]
There has been evidence found of a relationship between the anxiety children might feel and their curiosity. One study found that object curiosity in 11-year-olds was negatively related to psychological maladjusted so children who exhibit more anxiety in classroom settings engaged in less curious behaviour. It has also been suggested that certain aspects of classroom learning is dependent on curiosity which can be affected by students’ anxiety.[28]
Other measures of childhood curiosity have used exploratory behaviour as a basis but differing on how which parts of this behaviour is best to focus on. Some studies have examined children’s preference for complexity/the unknown as a basis for their curiosity measure; others have relied on novelty preference as their basis.[28]
Researchers have also looked at the relationship between a child’s reaction to surprise and curiosity. It has been suggested that children are further motivated to learn when dealing with uncertainty. It is argued that their reactions to not having their expectations met would fuel their curiosity more than the introduction of a novel or complex object would.[28]
Ethicality[edit]
The trait of curiosity has been a subject of interest to philosophers. Curiosity has been recognised as an important intellectual (or ‘epistemic’) virtue, due to the role that it plays in motivating people to acquire knowledge and understanding of the world around them.[29] It has also been suggested to be an important moral virtue, as curiosity can help humans find meaning in their lives and to cultivate a sense of care about others and things in the world.[30]
Due to its importance, there are debates about the extent to which contemporary societies do an effective job of cultivating the right type of curiosity.
Some believe that children’s curiosity becomes discouraged throughout the process of formal education: «Children are born scientists. From the first ball they send flying to the ant they watch carry a crumb, children use science’s tools—enthusiasm, hypotheses, tests, conclusions—to uncover the world’s mysteries. But somehow students seem to lose what once came naturally.»[28]
Sir Ken Robinson discusses a similar phenomenon in his TED Talk titled «Do schools kill creativity?» When curiosity in young people leads to knowledge-gathering it is widely seen as a positive.[31]
Impact from disease[edit]
Left: normal brain. Right: AD afflicted brain. Severe degeneration of areas implicated in curiosity
Different neurodegenerative diseases or other psychological disorders can affect various characteristics of curiosity, for instance Alzheimer’s disease’s effects on memory or depression on motivation and reward. Alzheimer’s is a neurodegenerative disease that directly affects the capability and capacity for memory. Depression is a mood disorder that is characterized by a lack of interest in one’s environment and feelings of sadness or hopelessness. A lack of curiosity for novel stimuli might also be used as a potential predictor for these and other illnesses.[21]
Morbid curiosity[edit]
A crowd mills around the site of a car accident in Czechoslovakia in 1980.
Morbid curiosity exemplifies an aspect of curiosity that can be seen as focused on objects of death, violence, or any other event that may cause harm physically or emotionally.[32]
The idea of morbid curiosity typically is described as having an addictive quality. This addictive aspect of the need to understand or make sense of topics that surround harm, violence or death can be attributed to the idea of one’s need to relate unusual and often difficult circumstances to a primary emotion or experience of their own, described as meta-emotions.[33]
Understanding these difficult circumstances dates back to Aristotle in his Poetics, stating, «We enjoy and admire paintings of objects that in themselves would annoy or disgust us.»[34]
State and trait curiosity[edit]
There are two distinct classifications of types of curiosity: state and trait curiosity. Both types determine whether curiosity comes from within or outside of a person. State curiosity is external such as wondering why things happen for the sake of just curiousness, for example, wondering why most stores open at 8 a.m. This type of curiosity tends to be the most relatable for people on a day-to-day basis since state curiosity relates to high levels of reward. On the other hand, trait curiosity relates to people who are interested in learning. Generally, it could be trying out a new sport or food, or traveling to a new unknown place. One can look at curiosity as the urge that draws people out of their comfort zones and fears as the agent that keeps them within those zones.[35]
Curiosity in artificial intelligence[edit]
AI agents can exhibit curiosity through intrinsic motivation, this can be useful for improving the success of an AI agent at various tasks. In artificial intelligence, curiosity is typically defined quantitatively, as the uncertainty the agent has in predicting its own actions given its current state.[36][37]
In 2019, a study trained AI agents to play video games, but they were rewarded only for curiosity. The agents reliably learned advantageous game behaviors based solely on the curiosity reward.[38]
See also[edit]
- Broaden-and-build
- Interest (emotion)
- Inquiry
- Play (activity)
References[edit]
- ^ a b Berlyne DE (August 1954). «A theory of human curiosity». British Journal of Psychology. 45 (3): 180–191. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1954.tb01243.x. PMID 13190171.
- ^ a b c Berlyne DE (August 1955). «The arousal and satiation of perceptual curiosity in the rat». Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 48 (4): 238–246. doi:10.1037/h0042968. PMID 13252149.
- ^ Zuss M (2012). The Practice of Theoretical Curiosity. New York: Springer Publishing. ISBN 978-9-40072-117-3.
- ^ Curiosity and Exploration. Berlin: Springer-Verlag. 1994. ISBN 978-0-387-54867-8.
- ^ Ofer G, Durban J (1999). «Curiosity: reflections on its nature and functions». American Journal of Psychotherapy. 53 (1): 35–51. doi:10.1176/appi.psychotherapy.1999.53.1.35. PMID 10207585.
- ^ a b Loewenstein G (1994). «The psychology of curiosity: a review and reinterpretation». Psychological Bulletin. 116 (1): 75–98. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.320.1976. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.116.1.75.
- ^ Kidd C, Hayden BY (November 2015). «The Psychology and Neuroscience of Curiosity». Neuron. 88 (3): 449–460. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.010. PMC 4635443. PMID 26539887.
- ^ a b c d Berlyne DE (1960). Conflict, arousal, and curiosity. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
- ^ Berlyne DE (November 1957). «Uncertainty and conflict: a point of contact between information-theory and behavior-theory concepts». Psychological Review. 64, Part 1 (6): 329–339. doi:10.1037/h0041135. PMID 13505970.
- ^ a b c Edleman, S. 1997. Curiosity and Exploration. California State University, Northridge. Retrieved from «Curiosity and Exploration». Archived from the original on 2011-12-30. Retrieved 2011-12-28.
- ^ a b Litman J (2005). «Curiosity and the pleasures of learning: Wanting and liking new information». Cognition & Emotion. 19 (6): 793–814. doi:10.1080/02699930541000101. ISSN 0269-9931. S2CID 144976076.
- ^ a b c van Kampen HS (February 2019). «The principle of consistency and the cause and function of behaviour». Behavioural Processes. 159: 42–54. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2018.12.013. PMID 30562561. S2CID 56478466.
- ^ Inglis IR (1983). «Towards a cognitive theory of exploratory behaviour». In Archer J, Birke LI (eds.). Exploration in Animals and Humans. Wokingham, England: Van Nostrand Reinhold. pp. 72–112.
- ^ a b c d Costa VD, Tran VL, Turchi J, Averbeck BB (October 2014). «Dopamine modulates novelty seeking behavior during decision making». Behavioral Neuroscience. 128 (5): 556–566. doi:10.1037/a0037128. PMC 5861725. PMID 24911320.
- ^ a b Kakade S, Dayan P (2002). «Dopamine: generalization and bonuses». Neural Networks. 15 (4–6): 549–559. doi:10.1016/s0893-6080(02)00048-5. PMID 12371511. Archived from the original on 2017-09-13.
- ^ «How Curiosity Changes Your Brain | for Better | US News». Archived from the original on 2017-08-04. Retrieved 2017-08-04.
- ^ a b c Kang MJ, Hsu M, Krajbich IM, Loewenstein G, McClure SM, Wang JT, Camerer CF (August 2009). «The wick in the candle of learning: epistemic curiosity activates reward circuitry and enhances memory». Psychological Science. 20 (8): 963–973. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02402.x. PMID 19619181. S2CID 11719012.
- ^ a b Lehrer J. «The Itch of Curiosity». Wired. Wired. Archived from the original on 24 July 2015. Retrieved 21 July 2015.
- ^ a b Jepma M, Verdonschot RG, van Steenbergen H, Rombouts SA, Nieuwenhuis S (2012). «Neural mechanisms underlying the induction and relief of perceptual curiosity». Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 6: 5. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2012.00005. PMC 3277937. PMID 22347853.
- ^ Parker KJ, Rainwater KL, Buckmaster CL, Schatzberg AF, Lindley SE, Lyons DM (August 2007). «Early life stress and novelty seeking behavior in adolescent monkeys». Psychoneuroendocrinology. 32 (7): 785–792. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2007.05.008. PMC 2716798. PMID 17604913.
- ^ a b Stuart Z, Cecelia M, Allan L, James L (2011). «Predicting the onset of Alzheimer’s disease with a behavioral task». Alzheimer’s & Dementia. 7 (4): S549. doi:10.1016/j.jalz.2011.05.1549. S2CID 54259243.
- ^ Phillips KA, Subiaul F, Sherwood CC (June 2012). «Curious monkeys have increased gray matter density in the precuneus». Neuroscience Letters. 518 (2): 172–175. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2012.05.004. PMID 22579821. S2CID 10265034.
- ^ Saab BJ, Georgiou J, Nath A, Lee FJ, Wang M, Michalon A, et al. (September 2009). «NCS-1 in the dentate gyrus promotes exploration, synaptic plasticity, and rapid acquisition of spatial memory». Neuron. 63 (5): 643–656. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.08.014. PMID 19755107. S2CID 5321020.
- ^ Sahay A, Scobie KN, Hill AS, O’Carroll CM, Kheirbek MA, Burghardt NS, et al. (April 2011). «Increasing adult hippocampal neurogenesis is sufficient to improve pattern separation». Nature. 472 (7344): 466–470. Bibcode:2011Natur.472..466S. doi:10.1038/nature09817. PMC 3084370. PMID 21460835.
- ^ Leussis MP, Berry-Scott EM, Saito M, Jhuang H, de Haan G, Alkan O, et al. (April 2013). «The ANK3 bipolar disorder gene regulates psychiatric-related behaviors that are modulated by lithium and stress». Biological Psychiatry. 73 (7): 683–690. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.10.016. PMID 23237312. S2CID 12981146.
- ^ Montgomery KC (August 1955). «The relation between fear induced by novel stimulation and exploratory behavior». Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 48 (4): 254–260. doi:10.1037/h0043788. PMID 13252152.
- ^ Engel, S. 2011. Children’s need to know: Curiosity in schools. Harvard Educational Review. Retrieved from Engel S (2011). «Children’s Need to Know: Curiosity in Schools». Harvard Educational Review. 81 (4): 625–645. doi:10.17763/haer.81.4.h054131316473115. Archived from the original on 2015-07-10. Retrieved 2017-12-04.
- ^ a b c d e f Jirout J, Klahr D (June 2012). «Children’s scientific curiosity: In search of an operational definition of an elusive concept». Developmental Review. 32 (2): 125–160. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2012.04.002.
- ^ Ross, Lewis (2020). The Virtue of Curiosity. Episteme. Volume 17 , Issue 1, pp. 105 — 120: https://doi.org/10.1017/epi.2018.31
- ^ Baumgartner, Elias. (2001). Curiosity as a Moral Virtue. International Journal of Applied Philosophy, Volume 15, Issue 2: 10.5840/ijap200115215
- ^ Cohen R (2013). The Development of Spatial Cognition. Psychology Press. p. 99. ISBN 9781134926664.
- ^ Scrivner C (2021). «The Psychology of Morbid Curiosity: Development and Initial Validation of the Morbid Curiosity Scale». Personality and Individual Differences. 183: 111139. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2021.111139.
- ^ Zuckerman M, Litle P (1986). «Personality and Curiosity About Morbid and Sexual Events». Personality and Individual Differences. 7 (1): 49–56. doi:10.1016/0191-8869(86)90107-8.
- ^ Aristotle (2013). Poetics. OUP Oxford. ISBN 978-0191635809. Retrieved 15 August 2020.
- ^ «How Curiosity Works». howstuffworks.com. 28 January 2010. Archived from the original on 18 September 2017. Retrieved 4 May 2018.
- ^ Pathak D. «Curiosity-driven Exploration by Self-supervised Prediction». University of California, Berkeley. Retrieved 5 August 2020.
- ^ Pathak D (15 May 2017). «Curiosity-driven Exploration by Self-supervised Prediction». Cornell University. arXiv:1705.05363.
- ^ Burda Y, Edwards H, Pathak D, Storkey A, Darrell T, Efros AA (2019). «Large-Scale Study of Curiosity-Driven Learning». ICLR. arXiv:1808.04355.
Further reading[edit]
- Edelman, Susan (Spring 1997). «Curiosity and Exploration». California State University, Northridge
- Manguel, Alberto (2015). Curiosity. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. ISBN 978-0300184785.
- Livio, Mario (2017). Why?: What Makes Us Curious. Simon & Schuster. ISBN 978-1476792095.
Other forms: curiosities
Curiosity is the urge you feel to know more about something. If you find a diary in a coffee shop, curiosity will make you want to look inside — but respect for the writer’s privacy may convince you not to.
Curiosity is the state of being curious: inquisitive, wondering, ready to poke around and figure something out. The word used to mean «very, very careful,» and only in the last few hundred years turned into a word expressing the desire to know more. Maybe that’s because when you’re poking around in secret business, you need to be very, very careful. We also call weird things curiosities, like a phone shaped like a duck, or a shark in a jar.
Definitions of curiosity
-
noun
a state in which you want to learn more about something
-
noun
something unusual — perhaps worthy of collecting
-
synonyms:
curio, oddity, oddment, peculiarity, rarity
see moresee less-
types:
- show 5 types…
- hide 5 types…
-
bric-a-brac, knickknack, knickknackery, nicknack, whatnot
miscellaneous curios
-
collectable, collectible
things considered to be worth collecting (not necessarily valuable or antique)
-
collector’s item, piece de resistance, showpiece
the outstanding item (the prize piece or main exhibit) in a collection
-
chachka, tchotchke, tsatske, tshatshke
(Yiddish) an inexpensive showy trinket
-
rariora
(plural) rare collector’s items
-
type of:
-
object, physical object
a tangible and visible entity; an entity that can cast a shadow
DISCLAIMER: These example sentences appear in various news sources and books to reflect the usage of the word ‘curiosity’.
Views expressed in the examples do not represent the opinion of Vocabulary.com or its editors.
Send us feedback
EDITOR’S CHOICE
Look up curiosity for the last time
Close your vocabulary gaps with personalized learning that focuses on teaching the
words you need to know.
Sign up now (it’s free!)
Whether you’re a teacher or a learner, Vocabulary.com can put you or your class on the path to systematic vocabulary improvement.
Get started
любопытство, любознательность, редкость, антикварный
существительное ↓
- любознательность; пытливость
curiosity about unknown lands — желание узнать побольше о неизвестных странах
inbred curiosity — природная любознательность
omnivorous curiosity — ненасытное любопытство
- любопытство
to evince curiosity — проявлять любопытство
idle curiosity — пустое любопытство
to be eaten up with curiosity — быть снедаемым любопытством
I was dying of curiosity [burning with curiosity] — я умирал [сгорал] от любопытства
- странность
- (a curiosity) редкая антикварная вещь
old curiosity shop — антикварный магазин, «лавка древностей»
Мои примеры
Словосочетания
curiosity killed a cat — ≅ любопытство до добра не доведёт
Примеры с переводом
I was burning with curiosity.
Я сгорал от любопытства.
Curiosity moved me to open the box.
Любопытство заставило меня открыть ящик.
I am dying of curiosity / boredom.
Я умираю от любопытства / скуки.
It will slake your curiosity.
Это удовлетворит ваше любопытство.
She was burning with curiosity.
Она сгорала от любопытства.
Just out of curiosity, why did you take that job?
Можно полюбопыствовать, почему вы взялись за эту работу?
Paul was agog with curiosity.
Пол сгорал от любопытства.
ещё 23 примера свернуть
Примеры, ожидающие перевода
She was hostile to him, which piqued his curiosity.
It’s not worth much, but I kept it for its curiosity value.
It was only from idle curiosity that she went into the barn.
Для того чтобы добавить вариант перевода, кликните по иконке ☰, напротив примера.
Возможные однокоренные слова
curious — любопытный, любознательный, странный, курьезный, пытливый
incuriosity — отсутствие любопытства
incurious — нелюбопытный, нелюбознательный, безразличный, невнимательный
Формы слова
noun
ед. ч.(singular): curiosity
мн. ч.(plural): curiosities
- concern
- inquisitiveness
- interest
- eagerness
- interestedness
- intrusiveness
- investigation
- meddlesomeness
- meddling
- nosiness
- officiousness
- prying
- questioning
- snoopiness
- inquiring mind
- inquiringness
- mental acquisitiveness
- searching
- snooping
- thirst for knowledge
- curio
- novelty
- rarity
- knickknack
- objet d’art
- trinket
- exoticism
- bibelot
- conversation piece
- marvel
- oddity
- one of a kind
- peculiar object
- rara avis
- rare bird
- singular object
- unusual object
- wonder
On this page you’ll find 68 synonyms, antonyms, and words related to curiosity, such as: concern, inquisitiveness, interest, eagerness, interestedness, and intrusiveness.
- norm
Roget’s 21st Century Thesaurus, Third Edition Copyright © 2013 by the Philip Lief Group.
TRY USING curiosity
See how your sentence looks with different synonyms.
How to use curiosity in a sentence
There’s been a lot of quarterback movement around the league — and a lot of curiosity specifically about what the change at QB will mean for New England and Tampa Bay — but those moves haven’t affected our predictions too much.
WE PREVIEWED BOTH KINDS OF FOOTBALL FOR YOUSARAH SHACHATSEPTEMBER 8, 2020FIVETHIRTYEIGHT
She had never had this curiosity in relation to George Cannon—she had only wondered about his affairs with other women.
HILDA LESSWAYSARNOLD BENNETT
Miss Thangue sat forward with the frank curiosity of the Englishwoman when inspecting a foreign specimen.
ANCESTORSGERTRUDE ATHERTON
Her directness had made all possible ‘buts’ seem ridiculous and futile, and had made the expression of curiosity seem offensive.
HILDA LESSWAYSARNOLD BENNETT
SYNONYM OF THE DAY
OCTOBER 26, 1985
WORDS RELATED TO CURIOSITY
- bauble
- curio
- curiosity
- gaud
- gewgaw
- gimcrack
- knickknack
- novelty
- ornament
- trifle
- whatnot
- aberration
- abortion
- anomaly
- chimera
- curiosity
- geek
- malformation
- miscreation
- monster
- monstrosity
- mutant
- mutation
- oddity
- rarity
- sport
- weirdo
- buffoon
- computer specialist
- curiosity
- dolt
- dork
- freak
- goon
- guru
- nerd
- techie
- weirdo
- bagatelle
- bauble
- bibelot
- conversation piece
- curio
- curiosity
- gaud
- gimcrack
- item
- knickknack
- memento
- objet d’art
- souvenir
- toy
- trifle
- trinket
- whatnot
- bagatelle
- bauble
- bibelot
- bric-a-brac
- conversation piece
- curio
- curiosity
- embellishment
- frill
- furbelow
- gadget
- miniature
- notion
- novelty
- objet d’art
- ornament
- plaything
- souvenir
- thingamajig
- toy
- trifle
- trinket
- whatnot
- whimsy
- curiosity
- genius
- miracle
- one for the books
- phenomenon
- portent
- prodigy
- sensation
- something else
- stunner
- whiz
Roget’s 21st Century Thesaurus, Third Edition Copyright © 2013 by the Philip Lief Group.
1
a
: inquisitive interest in others’ concerns : nosiness
The construction inside their house aroused the curiosity of their neighbors.
b
: interest leading to inquiry
Her natural curiosity led her to ask more questions.
2
archaic
: undue nicety or fastidiousness
3
a
: one that arouses interest especially for uncommon or exotic characteristics
Tobacco was once regarded as a curiosity in Europe.
b
: an unusual knickknack : curio
The antique shop was full of curiosities.
Synonyms
Example Sentences
Her natural curiosity led her to ask more questions.
The arrival of a construction crew at their house attracted the curiosity of their neighbors.
The movie failed to satisfy her curiosity about the assassination.
Tobacco was once a curiosity in Europe.
The antique shop was full of curiosities.
Recent Examples on the Web
News of this curiosity had appeared in the New York Times, which had published a photograph of the peculiar dwelling.
—William Jeanes, Car and Driver, 9 Apr. 2023
Intelligence, desire, courage, curiosity, diligence, imagination, innovation, boldness?
—Michaela Zee, Variety, 8 Apr. 2023
Inspired by the playgrounds of Miyamoto’s youth, the game was a strikingly bright digital world centered on curiosity.
—Todd Martens, Los Angeles Times, 5 Apr. 2023
The magnetic tug of duty and curiosity made sense to me.
—Joshua Yaffa, The New Yorker, 31 Mar. 2023
It’s gotten to be that our inquiries into the wellness of others result from good manners more than genuine curiosity.
—Joseph Lezza, Longreads, 30 Mar. 2023
Hank Green, like his sibling, is an author, but this podcast brings them together to create an ideal combo of humor, deep thoughts, and mind-blowing curiosity.
—Jesse David Fox, Vulture, 29 Mar. 2023
The most important thing is to approach the subject of cultural identity and history with openness and curiosity with the intention of self-education.
—Shanon Maglente, Good Housekeeping, 28 Mar. 2023
Ranquet hopes that these preliminary findings might attract more curiosity toward this understudied topic.
—Jacopo Prisco, CNN, 28 Mar. 2023
See More
These examples are programmatically compiled from various online sources to illustrate current usage of the word ‘curiosity.’ Any opinions expressed in the examples do not represent those of Merriam-Webster or its editors. Send us feedback about these examples.
Word History
First Known Use
14th century, in the meaning defined at sense 1
Time Traveler
The first known use of curiosity was
in the 14th century
Dictionary Entries Near curiosity
Cite this Entry
“Curiosity.” Merriam-Webster.com Dictionary, Merriam-Webster, https://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/curiosity. Accessed 14 Apr. 2023.
Share
More from Merriam-Webster on curiosity
Last Updated:
12 Apr 2023
— Updated example sentences
Subscribe to America’s largest dictionary and get thousands more definitions and advanced search—ad free!
Merriam-Webster unabridged