The word fear means

«Scares» redirects here. For the island group in Scotland, see The Scares.

A girl showing signs of fear

Fear is an intensely unpleasant emotion in response to perceiving or recognizing a danger or threat. Fear causes physiological changes that may produce behavioral reactions such as mounting an aggressive response or fleeing the threat. Fear in human beings may occur in response to a certain stimulus occurring in the present, or in anticipation or expectation of a future threat perceived as a risk to oneself. The fear response arises from the perception of danger leading to confrontation with or escape from/avoiding the threat (also known as the fight-or-flight response), which in extreme cases of fear (horror and terror) can be a freeze response.

In humans and other animals, fear is modulated by the process of cognition and learning. Thus, fear is judged as rational and appropriate, or irrational and inappropriate. An irrational fear is called a phobia.

Fear is closely related to the emotion anxiety, which occurs as the result of threats that are perceived to be uncontrollable or unavoidable.[1] The fear response serves survival by engendering appropriate behavioral responses, so it has been preserved throughout evolution.[2] Sociological and organizational research also suggests that individuals’ fears are not solely dependent on their nature but are also shaped by their social relations and culture, which guide their understanding of when and how much fear to feel.[3][page needed]

Fear is sometimes incorrectly considered the opposite of courage. For the reason that courage is a willingness to face adversity, fear is an example of a condition that makes the exercise of courage possible.

Physiological signs[edit]

Many physiological changes in the body are associated with fear, summarized as the fight-or-flight response. An innate response for coping with danger, it works by accelerating the breathing rate (hyperventilation), heart rate, vasoconstriction of the peripheral blood vessels leading to blood pooling, increasing muscle tension including the muscles attached to each hair follicle to contract and causing «goosebumps», or more clinically, piloerection (making a cold person warmer or a frightened animal look more impressive), sweating, increased blood glucose (hyperglycemia), increased serum calcium, increase in white blood cells called neutrophilic leukocytes, alertness leading to sleep disturbance and «butterflies in the stomach» (dyspepsia). This primitive mechanism may help an organism survive by either running away or fighting the danger.[4] With the series of physiological changes, the consciousness realizes an emotion of fear.

There is observable physical reactions to individuals who experience fear. An individual might experience a feeling of dizziness, lightheaded, like they are being choked, sweating, shortness of breath, vomiting or nausea, numbness or shaking and any other like symptoms. This bodily reactions informs the individual that they are a afraid and should proceed to remove or get away from the stimulus that is causing that fear.[5]

Causes[edit]

An influential categorization of stimuli causing fear was proposed by Gray;[6] namely, intensity, novelty, special evolutionary dangers, stimuli arising during social interaction, and conditioned stimuli.[7] Another categorization was proposed by Archer,[8] who, besides conditioned fear stimuli, categorized fear-evoking (as well as aggression-evoking) stimuli into three groups; namely, pain, novelty, and frustration, although he also described “looming,” which refers to an object rapidly moving towards the visual sensors of a subject, and can be categorized as “intensity.” Russell[9] described a more functional categorization of fear-evoking stimuli, in which for instance novelty is a variable affecting more than one category: 1) Predator stimuli (including movement, suddenness, proximity, but also learned and innate predator stimuli); 2) Physical environmental dangers (including intensity and heights); 3) Stimuli associated with increased risk of predation and other dangers (including novelty, openness, illumination, and being alone); 4) Stimuli stemming from conspecifics (including novelty, movement, and spacing behavior); 5) Species-predictable fear stimuli and experience (special evolutionary dangers); and 6) Fear stimuli that are not species predictable (conditioned fear stimuli).

Nature[edit]

A prisoner at Abu Graib shows fear of a US army dog during prisoner abuse.

Although many fears are learned, the capacity to fear is part of human nature. Many studies[10] have found that certain fears (e.g. animals, heights) are much more common than others (e.g. flowers, clouds). These fears are also easier to induce in the laboratory. This phenomenon is known as preparedness. Because early humans that were quick to fear dangerous situations were more likely to survive and reproduce; preparedness is theorized to be a genetic effect that is the result of natural selection.[11]

From an evolutionary psychology perspective, different fears may be different adaptations that have been useful in our evolutionary past. They may have developed during different time periods. Some fears, such as fear of heights, may be common to all mammals and developed during the mesozoic period. Other fears, such as fear of snakes, may be common to all simians and developed during the cenozoic time period (the still-ongoing geological era encompassing the last 66 million of history). Still others, such as fear of mice and insects, may be unique to humans and developed during the paleolithic and neolithic time periods (when mice and insects become important carriers of infectious diseases and harmful for crops and stored foods).[12]

Conditioning[edit]

Nonhuman animals and humans innovate specific fears as a result of learning. This has been studied in psychology as fear conditioning, beginning with John B. Watson’s Little Albert experiment in 1920, which was inspired after observing a child with an irrational fear of dogs. In this study, an 11-month-old boy was conditioned to fear a white rat in the laboratory. The fear became generalized to include other white, furry objects, such as a rabbit, dog, and even a Santa Claus mask with white cotton balls in the beard.

Fear can be learned by experiencing or watching a frightening traumatic accident. For example, if a child falls into a well and struggles to get out, he or she may develop a fear of wells, heights (acrophobia), enclosed spaces (claustrophobia), or water (aquaphobia). There are studies looking at areas of the brain that are affected in relation to fear. When looking at these areas (such as the amygdala), it was proposed that a person learns to fear regardless of whether they themselves have experienced trauma, or if they have observed the fear in others. In a study completed by Andreas Olsson, Katherine I. Nearing and Elizabeth A. Phelps, the amygdala were affected both when subjects observed someone else being submitted to an aversive event, knowing that the same treatment awaited themselves, and when subjects were subsequently placed in a fear-provoking situation.[13] This suggests that fear can develop in both conditions, not just simply from personal history.

Fear is affected by cultural and historical context. For example, in the early 20th century, many Americans feared polio, a disease that can lead to paralysis.[14] There are consistent cross-cultural differences in how people respond to fear.[15] Display rules affect how likely people are to express the facial expression of fear and other emotions.

Fear of victimization is a function of perceived risk and seriousness.[16]

Common triggers[edit]

Phobias[edit]

According to surveys, some of the most common fears are of demons and ghosts, the existence of evil powers, cockroaches, spiders, snakes, heights, water, enclosed spaces, tunnels, bridges, needles, social rejection, failure, examinations, and public speaking.[17][18][19]

Uncertainty[edit]

Fear of the unknown or irrational fear is caused by negative thinking (worry) which arises from anxiety accompanied by a subjective sense of apprehension or dread.[20] Irrational fear shares a common neural pathway with other fears, a pathway that engages the nervous system to mobilize bodily resources in the face of danger or threat. Many people are scared of the «unknown». The irrational fear can branch out to many areas such as the hereafter, the next ten years or even tomorrow. Chronic irrational fear has deleterious effects since the elicitor stimulus is commonly absent or perceived from delusions. Such fear can create comorbidity with the anxiety disorder umbrella.[21] Being scared may cause people to experience anticipatory fear of what may lie ahead rather than planning and evaluating for the same. For example, «continuation of scholarly education» is perceived by many educators as a risk that may cause them fear and stress,[22] and they would rather teach things they’ve been taught than go and do research.[citation needed]

The ambiguity of situations that tend to be uncertain and unpredictable can cause anxiety in addition to other psychological and physical problems in some populations; especially those who engage it constantly, for example, in war-ridden places or in places of conflict, terrorism, abuse, etc. Poor parenting that instills fear can also debilitate a child’s psyche development or personality. For example, parents tell their children not to talk to strangers in order to protect them. In school, they would be motivated to not show fear in talking with strangers, but to be assertive and also aware of the risks and the environment in which it takes place. Ambiguous and mixed messages like this can affect their self-esteem and self-confidence. Researchers say talking to strangers isn’t something to be thwarted but allowed in a parent’s presence if required.[23] Developing a sense of equanimity to handle various situations is often advocated as an antidote to irrational fear and as an essential skill by a number of ancient philosophies.

Fear of the unknown (FOTU) «may be a, or possibly the, fundamental fear» from early times when there were many threats to life.[24]

American top tens[edit]

In a 2005 Gallup Poll (U.S.), a national sample of about 1000 adolescents (aged 13 to 17) were asked what they feared the most as an open-ended question. The American adolescents reported perceiving their top ten fears as follows: terrorist attacks, spiders, death, failure, war, criminal or gang violence, being alone, the future, and nuclear war.[25]

In an estimate of what Americans fear the most, book author Bill Tancer analyzed the most frequent online queries that involved the phrase, «fear of…» following the assumption that people tend to seek information on the issues that concern them the most. His top ten list of fears published 2008 consisted of flying, heights, clowns, intimacy, death, rejection, people, snakes, failure, and driving.[26]

Behavior[edit]

Although fear behavior varies from species to species, it is often divided into two main categories; namely, avoidance/flight and immobility.[8] To these, different researchers have added different categories, such as threat display and attack,[27] protective responses (including startle and looming responses),[28] defensive burying,[29] and social responses (including alarm vocalizations and submission).[27][30] Finally, immobility is often divided into freezing and tonic immobility.[27][30]

The decision as to which particular fear behavior to perform is determined by the level of fear as well as the specific context, such as environmental characteristics (escape route present, distance to refuge), the presence of a discrete and localized threat, the distance between threat and subject, threat characteristics (speed, size, directness of approach), the characteristics of the subject under threat (size, physical condition, speed, degree of crypsis, protective morphological structures), social conditions (group size), and the amount of experience with the type of the threat.[7][8][30][31][32]

Mechanism[edit]


Often laboratory studies with rats are conducted to examine the acquisition and extinction of conditioned fear responses.[33] In 2004, researchers conditioned rats (Rattus norvegicus) to fear a certain stimulus, through electric shock.[34] The researchers were able to then cause an extinction of this conditioned fear, to a point that no medications or drugs were able to further aid in the extinction process. The rats showed signs of avoidance learning, not fear, but simply avoiding the area that brought pain to the test rats. The avoidance learning of rats is seen as a conditioned response, and therefore the behavior can be unconditioned, as supported by the earlier research.

Species-specific defense reactions (SSDRs) or avoidance learning in nature is the specific tendency to avoid certain threats or stimuli, it is how animals survive in the wild. Humans and animals both share these species-specific defense reactions, such as the flight-or-fight, which also include pseudo-aggression, fake or intimidating aggression and freeze response to threats, which is controlled by the sympathetic nervous system. These SSDRs are learned very quickly through social interactions between others of the same species, other species, and interaction with the environment.[35] These acquired sets of reactions or responses are not easily forgotten. The animal that survives is the animal that already knows what to fear and how to avoid this threat. An example in humans is the reaction to the sight of a snake, many jump backwards before cognitively realizing what they are jumping away from, and in some cases, it is a stick rather than a snake.

As with many functions of the brain, there are various regions of the brain involved in deciphering fear in humans and other nonhuman species.[36] The amygdala communicates both directions between the prefrontal cortex, hypothalamus, the sensory cortex, the hippocampus, thalamus, septum, and the brainstem. The amygdala plays an important role in SSDR, such as the ventral amygdalofugal, which is essential for associative learning, and SSDRs are learned through interaction with the environment and others of the same species. An emotional response is created only after the signals have been relayed between the different regions of the brain, and activating the sympathetic nervous systems; which controls the flight, fight, freeze, fright, and faint response.[37][38] Often a damaged amygdala can cause impairment in the recognition of fear (like the human case of patient S.M.).[39] This impairment can cause different species to lack the sensation of fear, and often can become overly confident, confronting larger peers, or walking up to predatory creatures.

Robert C. Bolles (1970), a researcher at University of Washington, wanted to understand species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning among animals, but found that the theories of avoidance learning and the tools that were used to measure this tendency were out of touch with the natural world.[40] He theorized the species-specific defense reaction (SSDR).[41] There are three forms of SSDRs: flight, fight (pseudo-aggression), or freeze. Even domesticated animals have SSDRs, and in those moments it is seen that animals revert to atavistic standards and become «wild» again. Dr. Bolles states that responses are often dependent on the reinforcement of a safety signal, and not the aversive conditioned stimuli. This safety signal can be a source of feedback or even stimulus change. Intrinsic feedback or information coming from within, muscle twitches, increased heart rate, are seen to be more important in SSDRs than extrinsic feedback, stimuli that comes from the external environment. Dr. Bolles found that most creatures have some intrinsic set of fears, to help assure survival of the species. Rats will run away from any shocking event, and pigeons will flap their wings harder when threatened. The wing flapping in pigeons and the scattered running of rats are considered species-specific defense reactions or behaviors. Bolles believed that SSDRs are conditioned through Pavlovian conditioning, and not operant conditioning; SSDRs arise from the association between the environmental stimuli and adverse events.[42] Michael S. Fanselow conducted an experiment, to test some specific defense reactions, he observed that rats in two different shock situations responded differently, based on instinct or defensive topography, rather than contextual information.[43]

Species-specific defense responses are created out of fear, and are essential for survival.[44] Rats that lack the gene stathmin show no avoidance learning, or a lack of fear, and will often walk directly up to cats and be eaten.[45] Animals use these SSDRs to continue living, to help increase their chance of fitness, by surviving long enough to procreate. Humans and animals alike have created fear to know what should be avoided, and this fear can be learned through association with others in the community, or learned through personal experience with a creature, species, or situations that should be avoided. SSDRs are an evolutionary adaptation that has been seen in many species throughout the world including rats, chimpanzees, prairie dogs, and even humans, an adaptation created to help individual creatures survive in a hostile world.

Fear learning changes across the lifetime due to natural developmental changes in the brain.[46][47] This includes changes in the prefrontal cortex and the amygdala.[48]

The visual exploration of an emotional face does not follow a fixed pattern but modulated by the emotional content of the face. Scheller et al.[49] found that participants paid more attention to the eyes when recognising fearful or neutral faces, while the mouth was fixated on when happy faces are presented, irrespective of task demands and spatial locations of face stimuli. These findings were replicated when fearful eyes are presented[50] and when canonical face configurations are distorted for fearful, neutral and happy expressions.[51]

Neurocircuitry in mammals[edit]

  • The thalamus collects sensory data from the senses
  • Sensory cortex receives data from the thalamus and interprets it
  • Sensory cortex organizes information for dissemination to the hypothalamus (fight or flight), amygdalae (fear), hippocampus (memory)

The brain structures that are the center of most neurobiological events associated with fear are the two amygdalae, located behind the pituitary gland. Each amygdala is part of a circuitry of fear learning.[2] They are essential for proper adaptation to stress and specific modulation of emotional learning memory. In the presence of a threatening stimulus, the amygdalae generate the secretion of hormones that influence fear and aggression.[52] Once a response to the stimulus in the form of fear or aggression commences, the amygdalae may elicit the release of hormones into the body to put the person into a state of alertness, in which they are ready to move, run, fight, etc. This defensive response is generally referred to in physiology as the fight-or-flight response regulated by the hypothalamus, part of the limbic system.[53] Once the person is in safe mode, meaning that there are no longer any potential threats surrounding them, the amygdalae will send this information to the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) where it is stored for similar future situations, which is known as memory consolidation.[54]

Some of the hormones involved during the state of fight-or-flight include epinephrine, which regulates heart rate and metabolism as well as dilating blood vessels and air passages, norepinephrine increasing heart rate, blood flow to skeletal muscles and the release of glucose from energy stores,[55] and cortisol which increases blood sugar, increases circulating neutrophilic leukocytes, calcium amongst other things.[56]

After a situation which incites fear occurs, the amygdalae and hippocampus record the event through synaptic plasticity.[57] The stimulation to the hippocampus will cause the individual to remember many details surrounding the situation.[58] Plasticity and memory formation in the amygdala are generated by activation of the neurons in the region. Experimental data supports the notion that synaptic plasticity of the neurons leading to the lateral amygdalae occurs with fear conditioning.[59] In some cases, this forms permanent fear responses such as post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) or a phobia.[60] MRI and fMRI scans have shown that the amygdalae in individuals diagnosed with such disorders including bipolar or panic disorder are larger and wired for a higher level of fear.[61]

Pathogens can suppress amygdala activity. Rats infected with the toxoplasmosis parasite become less fearful of cats, sometimes even seeking out their urine-marked areas. This behavior often leads to them being eaten by cats. The parasite then reproduces within the body of the cat. There is evidence that the parasite concentrates itself in the amygdala of infected rats.[62] In a separate experiment, rats with lesions in the amygdala did not express fear or anxiety towards unwanted stimuli. These rats pulled on levers supplying food that sometimes sent out electrical shocks. While they learned to avoid pressing on them, they did not distance themselves from these shock-inducing levers.[63]

Several brain structures other than the amygdalae have also been observed to be activated when individuals are presented with fearful vs. neutral faces, namely the occipitocerebellar regions including the fusiform gyrus and the inferior parietal / superior temporal gyri.[64] Fearful eyes, brows and mouth seem to separately reproduce these brain responses.[64] Scientists from Zurich studies show that the hormone oxytocin related to stress and sex reduces activity in your brain fear center.[65]

Pheromones and contagion[edit]

In threatening situations, insects, aquatic organisms, birds, reptiles, and mammals emit odorant substances, initially called alarm substances, which are chemical signals now called alarm pheromones. This is to defend themselves and at the same time to inform members of the same species of danger and leads to observable behavior change like freezing, defensive behavior, or dispersion depending on circumstances and species. For example, stressed rats release odorant cues that cause other rats to move away from the source of the signal.

After the discovery of pheromones in 1959, alarm pheromones were first described in 1968 in ants[66] and earthworms,[67] and four years later also found in mammals, both mice and rats.[68] Over the next two decades, identification and characterization of these pheromones proceeded in all manner of insects and sea animals, including fish, but it was not until 1990 that more insight into mammalian alarm pheromones was gleaned.

In 1985, a link between odors released by stressed rats and pain perception was discovered: unstressed rats exposed to these odors developed opioid-mediated analgesia.[69] In 1997, researchers found that bees became less responsive to pain after they had been stimulated with isoamyl acetate, a chemical smelling of banana, and a component of bee alarm pheromone.[70] The experiment also showed that the bees’ fear-induced pain tolerance was mediated by an endorphine.

By using the forced swimming test in rats as a model of fear-induction, the first mammalian «alarm substance» was found.[71] In 1991, this «alarm substance» was shown to fulfill criteria for pheromones: well-defined behavioral effect, species specificity, minimal influence of experience and control for nonspecific arousal. Rat activity testing with the alarm pheromone, and their preference/avoidance for odors from cylinders containing the pheromone, showed that the pheromone had very low volatility.[72]

In 1993 a connection between alarm chemosignals in mice and their immune response was found.[73] Pheromone production in mice was found to be associated with or mediated by the pituitary gland in 1994.[74]

In 2004, it was demonstrated that rats’ alarm pheromones had different effects on the «recipient» rat (the rat perceiving the pheromone) depending which body region they were released from: Pheromone production from the face modified behavior in the recipient rat, e.g. caused sniffing or movement, whereas pheromone secreted from the rat’s anal area induced autonomic nervous system stress responses, like an increase in core body temperature.[75] Further experiments showed that when a rat perceived alarm pheromones, it increased its defensive and risk assessment behavior,[76] and its acoustic startle reflex was enhanced.

It was not until 2011 that a link between severe pain, neuroinflammation and alarm pheromones release in rats was found: real time RT-PCR analysis of rat brain tissues indicated that shocking the footpad of a rat increased its production of proinflammatory cytokines in deep brain structures, namely of IL-1β, heteronuclear Corticotropin-releasing hormone and c-fos mRNA expressions in both the paraventricular nucleus and the bed nucleus of the stria terminalis, and it increased stress hormone levels in plasma (corticosterone).[77]

The neurocircuit for how rats perceive alarm pheromones was shown to be related to the hypothalamus, brainstem, and amygdalae, all of which are evolutionary ancient structures deep inside or in the case of the brainstem underneath the brain away from the cortex, and involved in the fight-or-flight response, as is the case in humans.[78]

Alarm pheromone-induced anxiety in rats has been used to evaluate the degree to which anxiolytics can alleviate anxiety in humans. For this, the change in the acoustic startle reflex of rats with alarm pheromone-induced anxiety (i.e. reduction of defensiveness) has been measured. Pretreatment of rats with one of five anxiolytics used in clinical medicine was able to reduce their anxiety: namely midazolam, phenelzine (a nonselective monoamine oxidase (MAO) inhibitor), propranolol, a nonselective beta blocker, clonidine, an alpha 2 adrenergic agonist or CP-154,526, a corticotropin-releasing hormone antagonist.[79]

Faulty development of odor discrimination impairs the perception of pheromones and pheromone-related behavior, like aggressive behavior and mating in male rats: The enzyme Mitogen-activated protein kinase 7 (MAPK7) has been implicated in regulating the development of the olfactory bulb and odor discrimination and it is highly expressed in developing rat brains, but absent in most regions of adult rat brains. Conditional deletion of the MAPK7gene in mouse neural stem cells impairs several pheromone-mediated behaviors, including aggression and mating in male mice. These behavior impairments were not caused by a reduction in the level of testosterone, by physical immobility, by heightened fear or anxiety or by depression. Using mouse urine as a natural pheromone-containing solution, it has been shown that the impairment was associated with defective detection of related pheromones, and with changes in their inborn preference for pheromones related to sexual and reproductive activities.[80]

Lastly, alleviation of an acute fear response because a friendly peer (or in biological language: an affiliative conspecific) tends and befriends is called «social buffering». The term is in analogy to the 1985 «buffering» hypothesis in psychology, where social support has been proven to mitigate the negative health effects of alarm pheromone mediated distress.[81] The role of a «social pheromone» is suggested by the recent discovery that olfactory signals are responsible in mediating the «social buffering» in male rats.[82] «Social buffering» was also observed to mitigate the conditioned fear responses of honeybees. A bee colony exposed to an environment of high threat of predation did not show increased aggression and aggressive-like gene expression patterns in individual bees, but decreased aggression. That the bees did not simply habituate to threats is suggested by the fact that the disturbed colonies also decreased their foraging.[83]

Biologists have proposed in 2012 that fear pheromones evolved as molecules of «keystone significance», a term coined in analogy to keystone species. Pheromones may determine species compositions and affect rates of energy and material exchange in an ecological community. Thus pheromones generate structure in a food web and play critical roles in maintaining natural systems.[84]

Humans[edit]

Evidence of chemosensory alarm signals in humans has emerged slowly: Although alarm pheromones have not been physically isolated and their chemical structures have not been identified in humans so far, there is evidence for their presence. Androstadienone, for example, a steroidal, endogenous odorant, is a pheromone candidate found in human sweat, axillary hair and plasma. The closely related compound androstenone is involved in communicating dominance, aggression or competition; sex hormone influences on androstenone perception in humans showed a high testosterone level related to heightened androstenone sensitivity in men, a high testosterone level related to unhappiness in response to androstenone in men, and a high estradiol level related to disliking of androstenone in women.[85]

A German study from 2006 showed when anxiety-induced versus exercise-induced human sweat from a dozen people was pooled and offered to seven study participants, of five able to olfactorily distinguish exercise-induced sweat from room air, three could also distinguish exercise-induced sweat from anxiety induced sweat. The acoustic startle reflex response to a sound when sensing anxiety sweat was larger than when sensing exercise-induced sweat, as measured by electromyography analysis of the orbital muscle, which is responsible for the eyeblink component. This showed for the first time that fear chemosignals can modulate the startle reflex in humans without emotional mediation; fear chemosignals primed the recipient’s «defensive behavior» prior to the subjects’ conscious attention on the acoustic startle reflex level.[86]

In analogy to the social buffering of rats and honeybees in response to chemosignals, induction of empathy by «smelling anxiety» of another person has been found in humans.[87]

A study from 2013 provided brain imaging evidence that human responses to fear chemosignals may be gender-specific. Researchers collected alarm-induced sweat and exercise-induced sweat from donors extracted it, pooled it and presented it to 16 unrelated people undergoing functional brain MRI. While stress-induced sweat from males produced a comparably strong emotional response in both females and males, stress-induced sweat from females produced markedly stronger arousal in women than in men. Statistical tests pinpointed this gender-specificity to the right amygdala and strongest in the superficial nuclei. Since no significant differences were found in the olfactory bulb, the response to female fear-induced signals is likely based on processing the meaning, i.e. on the emotional level, rather than the strength of chemosensory cues from each gender, i.e. the perceptual level.[88]

An approach-avoidance task was set up where volunteers seeing either an angry or a happy cartoon face on a computer screen pushed away or pulled toward them a joystick as fast as possible. Volunteers smelling androstadienone, masked with clove oil scent responded faster, especially to angry faces than those smelling clove oil only, which was interpreted as androstadienone-related activation of the fear system.[89] A potential mechanism of action is, that androstadienone alters the «emotional face processing». Androstadienone is known to influence the activity of the fusiform gyrus which is relevant for face recognition.

Cognitive-consistency theory[edit]

Cognitive-consistency theories assume that «when two or more simultaneously active cognitive structures are logically inconsistent, arousal is increased, which activates processes with the expected consequence of increasing consistency and decreasing arousal.»[90] In this context, it has been proposed that fear behavior is caused by an inconsistency between a preferred, or expected, situation and the actually perceived situation, and functions to remove the inconsistent stimulus from the perceptual field, for instance by fleeing or hiding, thereby resolving the inconsistency.[90][91][8] This approach puts fear in a broader perspective, also involving aggression and curiosity. When the inconsistency between perception and expectancy is small, learning as a result of curiosity reduces inconsistency by updating expectancy to match perception. If the inconsistency is larger, fear or aggressive behavior may be employed to alter the perception in order to make it match expectancy, depending on the size of the inconsistency as well as the specific context. Aggressive behavior is assumed to alter perception by forcefully manipulating it into matching the expected situation, while in some cases thwarted escape may also trigger aggressive behavior in an attempt to remove the thwarting stimulus.[90]

Research[edit]

In order to improve our understanding of the neural and behavioral mechanisms of adaptive and maladaptive fear, investigators use a variety of translational animal models.[92] These models are particularly important for research that would be too invasive for human studies. Rodents such as mice and rats are common animal models, but other species are used. Certain aspects of fear research still requires more research such as sex, gender, and age differences.

Models[edit]

These animal models include, but are not limited to, fear conditioning, predator-based psychosocial stress, single prolonged stress, chronic stress models, inescapable foot/tail shocks, immobilization or restraint, and stress enhanced fear learning. While the stress and fear paradigms differ between the models, they tend to involve aspects such as acquisition, generalization, extinction, cognitive regulation, and reconsolidation.[93][94]

Pavlovian[edit]

Fear conditioning, also known as Pavlovian or classical conditioning, is a process of learning that involves pairing a neutral stimulus with an unconditional stimulus (US).[95] A neutral stimulus is something like a bell, tone, or room that doesn’t illicit a response normally where a US is a stimulus that results in a natural or unconditioned response (UR — in Pavlov’s famous experiment the neutral stimulus is a bell and the US would be food with the dog’s salvation being the UR. Pairing the neutral stimulus and the US results in the UR occurring not only with the US but also the neutral stimulus. When this occurs the neutral stimulus is referred to as the conditional stimulus (CS) and the response the conditional response (CR). In the fear conditioning model of Pavlovian conditioning the US is an aversive stimulus such as a shock, tone, or unpleasant odor.

[edit]

Predator-based psychosocial stress (PPS) involves a more naturalistic approach to fear learning.[96] Predators such as a cat, a snake, or urine from a fox or cat are used along with other stressors such as immobilization or restraint in order to generate instinctual fear responses.[97]

Chronic stress models[edit]

Chronic stress models include chronic variable stress, chronic social defeat, and chronic mild stress.[96][98] These models are often used to study how long-term or prolonged stress/pain can alter fear learning and disorders.[96][99]

Single prolonged stress[edit]

Single prolonged stress (SPS) is a fear model that is often used to study PTSD.[100][101] It’s paradigm involves multiple stressors such as immobilization, a force swim, and exposure to ether delivered concurrently to the subject.[101] This is used to study non-naturalistic, uncontrollable situations that can cause a maladaptive fear responses that is seen in a lot of anxiety and traumatic based disorders.

Stress enhanced fear learning[edit]

Stress enhanced fear learning (SEFL) like SPS is often used to study the maladaptive fear learning involved in PTSD and other traumatic based disorders.[96][102] SEFL involves a single extreme stressor such as a large number of footshocks simulating a single traumatic stressor that somehow enhances and alters future fear learning.[96][103][104]

Management[edit]

Pharmaceutical[edit]

A drug treatment for fear conditioning and phobias via the amygdalae is the use of glucocorticoids.[105] In one study, glucocorticoid receptors in the central nuclei of the amygdalae were disrupted in order to better understand the mechanisms of fear and fear conditioning. The glucocorticoid receptors were inhibited using lentiviral vectors containing Cre-recombinase injected into mice. Results showed that disruption of the glucocorticoid receptors prevented conditioned fear behavior. The mice were subjected to auditory cues which caused them to freeze normally. A reduction of freezing was observed in the mice that had inhibited glucocorticoid receptors.[106]

Psychological[edit]

Cognitive behavioral therapy has been successful in helping people overcome their fear. Because fear is more complex than just forgetting or deleting memories, an active and successful approach involves people repeatedly confronting their fears. By confronting their fears in a safe manner a person can suppress the «fear-triggering memories» or stimuli.[107]

Exposure therapy has known to have helped up to 90% of people with specific phobias to significantly decrease their fear over time.[54][107]

Another psychological treatment is systematic desensitization, which is a type of behavior therapy used to completely remove the fear or produce a disgusted response to this fear and replace it. The replacement that occurs will be relaxation and will occur through conditioning. Through conditioning treatments, muscle tensioning will lessen and deep breathing techniques will aid in de-tensioning.

Literary and religious[edit]

There are other methods for treating or coping with one’s fear, such as writing down rational thoughts regarding fears. Journal entries are a healthy method of expressing one’s fears without compromising safety or causing uncertainty. Another suggestion is a fear ladder. To create a fear ladder, one must write down all of their fears and score them on a scale of one to ten. Next, the person addresses their phobia, starting with the lowest number.

Religion can help some individuals cope with fear.[108]

Incapability[edit]

People who have damage to their amygdalae, which can be caused by a rare genetic disease known as Urbach–Wiethe disease, are unable to experience fear. The disease destroys both amygdalae in late childhood. Since the discovery of the disease, there have only been 400 recorded cases. A lack of fear can allow someone to get into a dangerous situation they otherwise would have avoided.[109]

Society and culture[edit]

Death[edit]

The fear of the end of life and its existence is, in other words, the fear of death. Historically, attempts were made to reduce this fear by performing rituals which have helped collect the cultural ideas that we now have in the present.[citation needed] These rituals also helped preserve the cultural ideas. The results and methods of human existence had been changing at the same time that social formation was changing.

When people are faced with their own thoughts of death, they either accept that they are dying or will die because they have lived a full life or they will experience fear. A theory was developed in response to this, which is called the terror management theory. The theory states that a person’s cultural worldviews (religion, values, etc.) will mitigate the terror associated with the fear of death through avoidance. To help manage their terror, they find solace in their death-denying beliefs, such as their religion. Another way people cope with their death related fears is pushing any thoughts of death into the future or by avoiding these thoughts all together through distractions.[110] Although there are methods for one coping with the terror associated with their fear of death, not everyone suffers from these same uncertainties. People who believe they have lived life to the «fullest» typically do not fear death.

Death anxiety is multidimensional; it covers «fears related to one’s own death, the death of others, fear of the unknown after death, fear of obliteration, and fear of the dying process, which includes fear of a slow death and a painful death».[111]

The Yale philosopher Shelly Kagan examined fear of death in a 2007 Yale open course[112] by examining the following questions: Is fear of death a reasonable appropriate response? What conditions are required and what are appropriate conditions for feeling fear of death? What is meant by fear, and how much fear is appropriate? According to Kagan for fear in general to make sense, three conditions should be met:

  1. the object of fear needs to be «something bad»
  2. there needs to be a non-negligible chance that the bad state of affairs will happen
  3. there needs to be some uncertainty about the bad state of affairs

The amount of fear should be appropriate to the size of «the bad». If the three conditions are not met, fear is an inappropriate emotion. He argues, that death does not meet the first two criteria, even if death is a «deprivation of good things» and even if one believes in a painful afterlife. Because death is certain, it also does not meet the third criterion, but he grants that the unpredictability of when one dies may be cause to a sense of fear.[112]

In a 2003 study of 167 women and 121 men, aged 65–87, low self-efficacy predicted fear of the unknown after death and fear of dying for women and men better than demographics, social support, and physical health. Fear of death was measured by a «Multidimensional Fear of Death Scale» which included the 8 subscales Fear of Dying, Fear of the Dead, Fear of Being Destroyed, Fear for Significant Others, Fear of the Unknown, Fear of Conscious Death, Fear for the Body After Death, and Fear of Premature Death. In hierarchical multiple regression analysis, the most potent predictors of death fears were low «spiritual health efficacy», defined as beliefs relating to one’s perceived ability to generate spiritually based faith and inner strength, and low «instrumental efficacy», defined as beliefs relating to one’s perceived ability to manage activities of daily living.[111]

Psychologists have tested the hypotheses that fear of death motivates religious commitment, and that assurances about an afterlife alleviate the fear, with equivocal results.[citation needed] Religiosity can be related to fear of death when the afterlife is portrayed as time of punishment. «Intrinsic religiosity», as opposed to mere «formal religious involvement», has been found to be negatively correlated with death anxiety.[111] In a 1976 study of people of various Christian denominations, those who were most firm in their faith, who attended religious services weekly, were the least afraid of dying. The survey found a negative correlation between fear of death and «religious concern».[113][better source needed]

In a 2006 study of white, Christian men and women the hypothesis was tested that traditional, church-centered religiousness and de-institutionalized spiritual seeking are ways of approaching fear of death in old age. Both religiousness and spirituality were related to positive psychosocial functioning, but only church-centered religiousness protected subjects against the fear of death.[114][115][better source needed]

Religion[edit]

From a theological perspective, the word fear encompasses more than simple fear. Robert B. Strimple says that fear includes the «… convergence of awe, reverence, adoration…».[116] Some translations of the Bible, such as the New International Version, sometimes replace the word fear with reverence.

Fear in religion can be seen throughout the years, including in the Crusades. Pope Urban II allowed for Christian mercenary troops to be sent on a mission in order to recover the Holy Lands from the Muslims. The message was misinterpreted and as a result, innocent people were slaughtered. Although the Crusades were meant to stay between the Muslims and the Christians, the hate spread onto the Jewish culture. Jewish people who feared for their lives gave in to the forced conversion of Christianity because they believed this would secure their safety. Other Jewish people feared betraying their God by conceding to a conversion, and instead, secured their own fate, which was death.[117]

Manipulation[edit]

Fear may be politically and culturally manipulated to persuade citizenry of ideas which would otherwise be widely rejected or dissuade citizenry from ideas which would otherwise be widely supported. In contexts of disasters, nation-states manage the fear not only to provide their citizens with an explanation about the event or blaming some minorities, but also to adjust their previous beliefs.

Fear can alter how a person thinks or reacts to situations because fear has the power to inhibit one’s rational way of thinking. As a result, people who do not experience fear, are able to use fear as a tool to manipulate others. People who are experiencing fear, seek preservation through safety and can be manipulated by a person who is there to provide that safety that is being sought after. «When we’re afraid, a manipulator can talk us out of the truth we see right in front of us. Words become more real than reality»[118] By this, a manipulator is able to use our fear to manipulate us out the truth and instead make us believe and trust in their truth. Politicians are notorious for using fear to manipulate the people into supporting their policies.[119]

Fiction and mythology[edit]

Fear is found and reflected in mythology and folklore as well as in works of fiction such as novels and films.

Works of dystopian and (post)apocalyptic fiction convey the fears and anxieties of societies.[120][121]

The fear of the world’s end is about as old as civilization itself.[122] In a 1967 study, Frank Kermode suggests that the failure of religious prophecies led to a shift in how society apprehends this ancient mode.[123] Scientific and critical thought supplanting religious and mythical thought as well as a public emancipation may be the cause of eschatology becoming replaced by more realistic scenarios. Such might constructively provoke discussion and steps to be taken to prevent depicted catastrophes.

The Story of the Youth Who Went Forth to Learn What Fear Was is a German fairy tale dealing with the topic of not knowing fear.
Many stories also include characters who fear the antagonist of the plot. One important characteristic of historical and mythical heroes across cultures is to be fearless in the face of big and often lethal enemies.[citation needed]

Athletics[edit]

In the world of athletics, fear is often used as a means of motivation to not fail.[124] This situation involves using fear in a way that increases the chances of a positive outcome. In this case, the fear that is being created is initially a cognitive state to the receiver.[125] This initial state is what generates the first response of the athlete, this response generates a possibility of fight or flight reaction by the athlete (receiver), which in turn will increase or decrease the possibility of success or failure in the certain situation for the athlete.[126] The amount of time that the athlete has to determine this decision is small but it is still enough time for the receiver to make a determination through cognition.[125] Even though the decision is made quickly, the decision is determined through past events that have been experienced by the athlete.[127] The results of these past events will determine how the athlete will make his cognitive decision in the split second that he or she has.[124]

Fear of failure as described above has been studied frequently in the field of sport psychology. Many scholars have tried to determine how often fear of failure is triggered within athletes, as well as what personalities of athletes most often choose to use this type of motivation. Studies have also been conducted to determine the success rate of this method of motivation.

Murray’s Exploration in Personal (1938) was one of the first studies that actually identified fear of failure as an actual motive to avoid failure or to achieve success. His studies suggested that inavoidance, the need to avoid failure, was found in many college-aged men during the time of his research in 1938.[128] This was a monumental finding in the field of psychology because it allowed other researchers to better clarify how fear of failure can actually be a determinant of creating achievement goals as well as how it could be used in the actual act of achievement.[129]

In the context of sport, a model was created by R.S. Lazarus in 1991 that uses the cognitive-motivational-relational theory of emotion.[125]

It holds that Fear of Failure results when beliefs or cognitive schemas about aversive consequences of failing are activated by situations in which failure is possible. These belief systems predispose the individual to make appraisals of threat and experience the state anxiety that is associated with Fear of Failure in evaluative situations.[129][125]

Another study was done in 2001 by Conroy, Poczwardowski, and Henschen that created five aversive consequences of failing that have been repeated over time. The five categories include (a) experiencing shame and embarrassment, (b) devaluing one’s self-estimate, (c) having an uncertain future, (d) important others losing interest, (e) upsetting important others.[124] These five categories can help one infer the possibility of an individual to associate failure with one of these threat categories, which will lead them to experiencing fear of failure.

In summary, the two studies that were done above created a more precise definition of fear of failure, which is «a dispositional tendency to experience apprehension and anxiety in evaluative situations because individuals have learned that failure is associated with aversive consequences».[129]

See also[edit]

  • Appeal to fear
  • Culture of fear
  • Ecology of fear
  • Hysteria
  • Nightmare
  • Night terror
  • Ontogenetic parade
  • Panic attack
  • Paranoia
  • Phobophobia
  • Psychological trauma
  • Social anxiety disorder
  • Social anxiety
  • Voodoo death
  • Anger

References[edit]

  1. ^ Öhman, A. (2000). «Fear and anxiety: Evolutionary, cognitive, and clinical perspectives». In M. Lewis & J.M. Haviland-Jones (Eds.). Handbook of emotions. pp. 573–93. New York: The Guilford Press.
  2. ^ a b Olsson A, Phelps EA (September 2007). «Social learning of fear». Nature Neuroscience. 10 (9): 1095–102. doi:10.1038/nn1968. PMID 17726475. S2CID 11976458.
  3. ^ Gill, M.J. and Burrow, R., 2017. The function of fear in institutional maintenance: Feeling frightened as an essential ingredient in haute cuisine. Organization Studies
  4. ^ Edmundson LD. «The Neurobiology of Fear». Serendip. Retrieved 9 April 2012.
  5. ^ «Symptoms-phobias». https://www.nhs.uk/. 27 July 2022. Archived from the original on 20 October 2022. Retrieved 30 March 2023.
  6. ^ Gray JA (1987). The Psychology of Fear and Stress (2nd ed.). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.
  7. ^ a b Adolphs R (January 2013). «The biology of fear». Current Biology. 23 (2): R79-93. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.11.055. PMC 3595162. PMID 23347946.
  8. ^ a b c d Archer J (1976). «The organization of aggression and fear in vertebrates». In Bateson PP, Klopfer PH (eds.). Perspectives in Ethology (Vol.2). New York, NY: Plenum. pp. 231–298.
  9. ^ Russell PA (1976). «Fear-evoking stimuli». In Sluckin W (ed.). Fear in Animals and Man. Wokingham, UK: Van Nostrand Reinhold. pp. 86–124.
  10. ^ Garcia R (September 2017). «Neurobiology of fear and specific phobias». Learning & Memory. 24 (9): 462–471. doi:10.1101/lm.044115.116. PMC 5580526. PMID 28814472.
  11. ^ Ohman A, Mineka S (July 2001). «Fears, phobias, and preparedness: toward an evolved module of fear and fear learning». Psychological Review. 108 (3): 483–522. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.108.3.483. PMID 11488376. S2CID 7920871.
  12. ^ Bracha HS (July 2006). «Human brain evolution and the «Neuroevolutionary Time-depth Principle:» Implications for the Reclassification of fear-circuitry-related traits in DSM-V and for studying resilience to warzone-related posttraumatic stress disorder» (PDF). Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry. 30 (5): 827–53. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2006.01.008. PMC 7130737. PMID 16563589.
  13. ^ Olsson A, Nearing KI, Phelps EA (March 2007). «Learning fears by observing others: the neural systems of social fear transmission». Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2 (1): 3–11. doi:10.1093/scan/nsm005. PMC 2555428. PMID 18985115.
  14. ^ «Polio: MedlinePlus Medical Encyclopedia». Archived from the original on 2017-01-29. Retrieved 2017-01-25.
  15. ^ Kim K, Markman AB (3 May 2005). «Differences in Fear of Isolation as an explanation of Cultural Differences: Evidence from memory and reasoning». Journal of Experimental Social Psychology. 42 (3): 350–364. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2005.06.005.
  16. ^ Warr M, Stafford M (1983). «Fear of Victimization: A Look at the Proximate Causes». Social Forces. 61 (4): 1033–43. doi:10.1093/sf/61.4.1033.
  17. ^ Welch A (October 15, 2015). «Things Americans fear most». CBS News. Archived from the original on June 22, 2016.
  18. ^ Ingraham C (October 30, 2014). «America’s top fears: Public speaking, heights and bugs». The Washington Post. Archived from the original on October 5, 2016.
  19. ^ Brewer G (March 19, 2001). «Snakes Top List of American’s Fears». Gallup. Archived from the original on July 14, 2016.
  20. ^ Zerwekh J (2013). Illustrated Study Guide for the NCLEX-RN® Exam (8th ed.). Mosby, Inc. pp. 178–179. ISBN 978-0-323-08232-7. Retrieved 7 July 2020.
  21. ^ Barrettn LF, Lewis M, Haviland-Jones JM (2016). Handbook of Emotions. Guilford Publications. pp. 751–73. ISBN 978-1-4625-2534-8. Archived from the original on 2017-03-02.
  22. ^ Burton LD (2011). «Fear». Journal of Research on Christian Education. 20 (2): 113–16. doi:10.1080/10656219.2011.592801. S2CID 216092318.
  23. ^ Fox ER (June 1987). «Fear of the unknown». The Western Journal of Medicine. 146 (6): 759–60. doi:10.1108/17578043200800026. PMC 1307488. PMID 18750277. S2CID 72767139.
  24. ^ Carleton RN (June 2016). «Fear of the unknown: One fear to rule them all?». Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 41 (June2016): 5–21. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.03.011. PMID 27067453.
  25. ^ Gallup Poll: What Frightens America’s Youth Archived 2008-11-21 at the Wayback Machine, gallup.com (29 March 2005).
  26. ^ Tancer B (September 2, 2008). Click: What millions of people are doing online and why it matters. Hyperion. ISBN 978-1-4013-2304-2.
  27. ^ a b c Misslin R (April 2003). «The defense system of fear: behavior and neurocircuitry». Neurophysiologie Clinique = Clinical Neurophysiology. 33 (2): 55–66. doi:10.1016/s0987-7053(03)00009-1. PMID 12837573. S2CID 35133426.
  28. ^ Blanchard RJ, Blanchard DC, Rodgers J, Weiss SM (1990). «The characterization and modelling of antipredator defensive behavior». Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 14 (4): 463–72. doi:10.1016/s0149-7634(05)80069-7. PMID 2287483. S2CID 10132051.
  29. ^ De Boer SF, Koolhaas JM (February 2003). «Defensive burying in rodents: ethology, neurobiology and psychopharmacology». European Journal of Pharmacology. 463 (1–3): 145–61. doi:10.1016/s0014-2999(03)01278-0. PMID 12600707.
  30. ^ a b c Archer J (1979). «Behavioural aspects of fear». In Sluckin W (ed.). Fear in Animals and Man. Workingham, UK: Van Nostrand Reinhold. pp. 56–85.
  31. ^ Stankowich T, Blumstein DT (December 2005). «Fear in animals: a meta-analysis and review of risk assessment». Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 272 (1581): 2627–34. doi:10.1098/rspb.2005.3251. PMC 1559976. PMID 16321785.
  32. ^ Osborn JW, Liard JF, Cowley AW (April 1987). «Effect of AVP on pressor responses to peripheral sympathetic stimulation in the rat». The American Journal of Physiology. 252 (4 Pt 2): H675-80. doi:10.1016/S0065-3454(08)60192-8. ISBN 9780120045167. PMID 3565586.
  33. ^ Morgan MA, LeDoux JE (August 1995). «Differential contribution of dorsal and ventral medial prefrontal cortex to the acquisition and extinction of conditioned fear in rats». Behavioral Neuroscience. 109 (4): 681–8. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.109.4.681. PMID 7576212. S2CID 3167606.
  34. ^ Cammarota M, Bevilaqua LR, Kerr D, Medina JH, Izquierdo I (February 2003). «Inhibition of mRNA and protein synthesis in the CA1 region of the dorsal hippocampus blocks reinstallment of an extinguished conditioned fear response». The Journal of Neuroscience. 23 (3): 737–41. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-03-00737.2003. PMC 6741935. PMID 12574401.
  35. ^ Davis S (2008). 21st Century Psychology: A Reference Handbook, Vol. 1. Thousand Oaks, California: SAGE Publication, Inc. pp. 282–86. ISBN 978-1-4129-4968-2.
  36. ^ Robert P. «The Amygdala and Its Allies». 2002. The Brain. Archived from the original on 6 August 2013. Retrieved 2 October 2013.
  37. ^ Schmidt NB, Richey JA, Zvolensky MJ, Maner JK (September 2008). «Exploring human freeze responses to a threat stressor». Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry. 39 (3): 292–304. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2007.08.002. PMC 2489204. PMID 17880916.
  38. ^ Bracha HS (September 2004). «Freeze, flight, fight, fright, faint: adaptationist perspectives on the acute stress response spectrum» (PDF). CNS Spectrums. 9 (9): 679–85. doi:10.1017/s1092852900001954. PMID 15337864. S2CID 8430710.
  39. ^ Adolphs R, Gosselin F, Buchanan TW, Tranel D, Schyns P, Damasio AR (January 2005). «A mechanism for impaired fear recognition after amygdala damage». Nature. 433 (7021): 68–72. Bibcode:2005Natur.433…68A. doi:10.1038/nature03086. PMID 15635411. S2CID 2139996.
  40. ^ Bolles R (1970). «Species-Specific Defense Reactions and Avoidance Learning». Psychological Review. 77 (1): 32–48. doi:10.1037/h0028589.
  41. ^ Crawford M, Masterson FA (1982). «Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning. An evaluative review». The Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science. 17 (4): 204–14. doi:10.1007/BF03001275. PMID 6891452. S2CID 142436039.
  42. ^ Kiein S (2002). Biological Influences on Learning. Mississippi State University: McGraw-Hill Higher Education. Archived from the original on 2008-12-05.
  43. ^ Fanselow M (1986). «Associative vs topographical accounts of the immediate shock-freezing deficit in rats: Implications for the response selection rules governing species-specific defensive reactions». Learning and Motivation. 17 (1): 16–39. doi:10.1016/0023-9690(86)90018-4.
  44. ^ Crawford M, Masterson FA (October 1982). «Species-specific defense reactions and avoidance learning». The Pavlovian Journal of Biological Science. Springer Science and Business Media LLC. 17 (4): 204–214. doi:10.1007/bf03001275. ISSN 0093-2213. PMID 6891452. S2CID 142436039.
  45. ^ Brocke B, Lesch KP, Armbruster D, Moser DA, Müller A, Strobel A, Kirschbaum C (January 2010). «Stathmin, a gene regulating neural plasticity, affects fear and anxiety processing in humans». American Journal of Medical Genetics. Part B, Neuropsychiatric Genetics. 153B (1): 243–51. doi:10.1002/ajmg.b.30989. PMID 19526456. S2CID 14851460.
  46. ^ Kim JH, Ganella DE (2015-02-01). «A Review of Preclinical Studies to Understand Fear During Adolescence». Australian Psychologist. 50 (1): 25–31. doi:10.1111/ap.12066. ISSN 1742-9544. S2CID 142760996.
  47. ^ Kim JH, Richardson R (February 2010). «New findings on extinction of conditioned fear early in development: theoretical and clinical implications». Biological Psychiatry. 67 (4): 297–303. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.003. PMID 19846065. S2CID 33444381.
  48. ^ Li S, Kim JH, Richardson R (April 2012). «Differential involvement of the medial prefrontal cortex in the expression of learned fear across development». Behavioral Neuroscience. 126 (2): 217–25. doi:10.1037/a0027151. PMID 22448855.
  49. ^ Scheller E, Büchel C, Gamer M (2012-07-25). «Diagnostic features of emotional expressions are processed preferentially». PLOS ONE. 7 (7): e41792. Bibcode:2012PLoSO…741792S. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0041792. PMC 3405011. PMID 22848607.
  50. ^ Smith ML, Cottrell GW, Gosselin F, Schyns PG (March 2005). «Transmitting and decoding facial expressions». Psychological Science. 16 (3): 184–9. doi:10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00801.x. PMID 15733197. S2CID 2622673.
  51. ^ Elsherif MM, Sahan MI, Rotshtein P (2017-03-07). «The perceptual saliency of fearful eyes and smiles: A signal detection study». PLOS ONE. 12 (3): e0173199. Bibcode:2017PLoSO..1273199E. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0173199. PMC 5340363. PMID 28267761.
  52. ^ Best, Ben (2004). The Amygdala and the Emotions Archived 2007-03-09 at the Wayback Machine. benbest.com
  53. ^ Gleitman, Henry; Fridlund, Alan J. and Reisberg, Daniel (2004). Psychology (6 ed.). W.W. Norton & Company. ISBN 0-393-97767-6.
  54. ^ a b Travis J (2004). «Fear not: Scientists are learning how people can unlearn fear». Science News. 165 (3): 42–44. doi:10.2307/4014925. JSTOR 4014925.
  55. ^ von Bohlen und Halbach, O; Dermietzel, R (2006). Neurotransmitters and neuromodulators: handbook of receptors and biological effects. Wiley-VCH. p. 125. ISBN 978-3-527-31307-5.
  56. ^ Hoehn K, Marieb EN (2010). Human Anatomy & Physiology. San Francisco: Benjamin Cummings. ISBN 0-321-60261-7.
  57. ^ Amunts K, Kedo O, Kindler M, Pieperhoff P, Mohlberg H, Shah NJ, et al. (December 2005). «Cytoarchitectonic mapping of the human amygdala, hippocampal region and entorhinal cortex: intersubject variability and probability maps». Anatomy and Embryology. 210 (5–6): 343–52. doi:10.1007/s00429-005-0025-5. PMID 16208455. S2CID 6984617.
  58. ^ Schacter, Daniel L.; Gilbert, Daniel T. and Wegner, Daniel M. (2011) Psychology Study Guide, Worth Publishers, ISBN 1-4292-0615-2.
  59. ^ LeDoux J (October 2003). «The emotional brain, fear, and the amygdala». Cellular and Molecular Neurobiology. 23 (4–5): 727–38. doi:10.1023/A:1025048802629. PMID 14514027. S2CID 3216382.
  60. ^ American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV. Washington, DC. ISBN 0-89042-061-0.
  61. ^ Cheng DT, Knight DC, Smith CN, Stein EA, Helmstetter FJ (February 2003). «Functional MRI of human amygdala activity during Pavlovian fear conditioning: stimulus processing versus response expression» (PDF). Behavioral Neuroscience. 117 (1): 3–10. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.123.4317. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.117.1.3. PMID 12619902. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2008-10-08. Retrieved 2017-10-24.
  62. ^ Berdoy M, Webster JP, Macdonald DW (August 2000). «Fatal attraction in rats infected with Toxoplasma gondii». Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 267 (1452): 1591–4. doi:10.1098/rspb.2000.1182. PMC 1690701. PMID 11007336.
  63. ^ Larkin M (1997). «Amygdala differentiates fear response». The Lancet. 350 (9073): 268. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(05)62234-9. S2CID 54232230.
  64. ^ a b Radua J, Phillips ML, Russell T, Lawrence N, Marshall N, Kalidindi S, et al. (January 2010). «Neural response to specific components of fearful faces in healthy and schizophrenic adults». NeuroImage. 49 (1): 939–46. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.030. PMID 19699306. S2CID 6209163. Archived from the original on 2017-12-01. Retrieved 2019-08-14.
  65. ^ Fear not.» Ski Mar.–Apr. 2009: 15. Gale Canada In Context. Web. 29 Sep. 2011
  66. ^ Moser JC, Brownlee RC, Silverstein R (April 1968). «Alarm pheromones of the ant atta texana». Journal of Insect Physiology. 14 (4): 529–35. doi:10.1016/0022-1910(68)90068-1. PMID 5649232.
  67. ^ Ressler RH, Cialdini RB, Ghoca ML, Kleist SM (August 1968). «Alarm pheromone in the earthworm Lumbricus terrestris». Science. 161 (3841): 597–9. Bibcode:1968Sci…161..597R. doi:10.1126/science.161.3841.597. PMID 5663305. S2CID 30927186.
  68. ^ Rottman SJ, Snowdon CT (December 1972). «Demonstration and analysis of an alarm pheromone in mice». Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 81 (3): 483–90. doi:10.1037/h0033703. PMID 4649187.
  69. ^ Fanselow MS (June 1985). «Odors released by stressed rats produce opioid analgesia in unstressed rats». Behavioral Neuroscience. 99 (3): 589–92. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.99.3.589. PMID 3843728.
  70. ^ Núñez J, Almeida L, Balderrama N, Giurfa M (December 1997). «Alarm pheromone induces stress analgesia via an opioid system in the honeybee». Physiology & Behavior. 63 (1): 75–80. doi:10.1016/s0031-9384(97)00391-0. PMID 9402618. S2CID 8788442.
  71. ^ Abel EL, Bilitzke PJ (August 1990). «A possible alarm substance in the forced swimming test». Physiology & Behavior. 48 (2): 233–9. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(90)90306-o. PMID 2255725. S2CID 22994036.
  72. ^ Abel EL (October 1991). «Alarm substance emitted by rats in the forced-swim test is a low volatile pheromone». Physiology & Behavior. 50 (4): 723–7. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(91)90009-d. PMID 1775546. S2CID 41044786.
  73. ^ Cocke R, Moynihan JA, Cohen N, Grota LJ, Ader R (March 1993). «Exposure to conspecific alarm chemosignals alters immune responses in BALB/c mice». Brain, Behavior, and Immunity. 7 (1): 36–46. doi:10.1006/brbi.1993.1004. PMID 8471798. S2CID 7196539.
  74. ^ Abel EL (June 1994). «The pituitary mediates production or release of an alarm chemosignal in rats». Hormones and Behavior. 28 (2): 139–45. doi:10.1006/hbeh.1994.1011. PMID 7927280. S2CID 11844089.
  75. ^ Kiyokawa Y, Kikusui T, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y (January 2004). «Alarm pheromones with different functions are released from different regions of the body surface of male rats». Chemical Senses. 29 (1): 35–40. doi:10.1093/chemse/bjh004. PMID 14752038.
  76. ^ Kiyokawa Y, Shimozuru M, Kikusui T, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y (February 2006). «Alarm pheromone increases defensive and risk assessment behaviors in male rats». Physiology & Behavior. 87 (2): 383–7. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2005.11.003. PMID 16337975. S2CID 12780994. Archived from the original on 2017-08-30. Retrieved 2017-08-30.
  77. ^ Arakawa H, Arakawa K, Blandino P, Deak T (May 2011). «The role of neuroinflammation in the release of aversive odor cues from footshock-stressed rats: Implications for the neural mechanism of alarm pheromone». Psychoneuroendocrinology. 36 (4): 557–68. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2010.09.001. PMID 20888127. S2CID 24367179.
  78. ^ Kiyokawa Y, Kikusui T, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y (May 2005). «Mapping the neural circuit activated by alarm pheromone perception by c-Fos immunohistochemistry». Brain Research. 1043 (1–2): 145–54. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2005.02.061. PMID 15862528. S2CID 41186952. Archived from the original on 2017-08-31. Retrieved 2017-08-31.
  79. ^ Inagaki H, Kiyokawa Y, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y (February 2010). «The alarm pheromone in male rats as a unique anxiety model: psychopharmacological evidence using anxiolytics». Pharmacology, Biochemistry, and Behavior. 94 (4): 575–9. doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2009.11.013. PMID 19969015. S2CID 28194770.
  80. ^ Zou J, Storm DR, Xia Z (2013). «Conditional deletion of ERK5 MAP kinase in the nervous system impairs pheromone information processing and pheromone-evoked behaviors». PLOS ONE. 8 (10): e76901. Bibcode:2013PLoSO…876901Z. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0076901. PMC 3793934. PMID 24130808.
  81. ^ Cohen S, Wills TA (September 1985). «Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis». Psychological Bulletin. 98 (2): 310–57. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310. PMID 3901065. S2CID 18137066.
  82. ^ Takahashi Y, Kiyokawa Y, Kodama Y, Arata S, Takeuchi Y, Mori Y (March 2013). «Olfactory signals mediate social buffering of conditioned fear responses in male rats». Behavioural Brain Research. 240: 46–51. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2012.11.017. PMID 23183219. S2CID 30938917. Archived from the original on 2017-08-31. Retrieved 2017-08-31.
  83. ^ Rittschof CC, Robinson GE (November 2013). «Manipulation of colony environment modulates honey bee aggression and brain gene expression». Genes, Brain and Behavior. 12 (8): 802–11. doi:10.1111/gbb.12087. PMC 3863782. PMID 24034579.
  84. ^ Ferrer RP, Zimmer RK (October 2012). «Community ecology and the evolution of molecules of keystone significance». The Biological Bulletin. 223 (2): 167–77. doi:10.1086/BBLv223n2p167. PMID 23111129. S2CID 592393.
  85. ^ Lübke KT, Pause BM (January 2014). «Sex-hormone dependent perception of androstenone suggests its involvement in communicating competition and aggression». Physiology & Behavior. 123: 136–41. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.10.016. PMID 24184511. S2CID 25729942.
  86. ^ Prehn A, Ohrt A, Sojka B, Ferstl R, Pause BM (February 2006). «Chemosensory anxiety signals augment the startle reflex in humans». Neuroscience Letters. 394 (2): 127–30. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2005.10.012. PMID 16257486. S2CID 23295966.
  87. ^ Prehn-Kristensen A, Wiesner C, Bergmann TO, Wolff S, Jansen O, Mehdorn HM, et al. (June 2009). «Induction of empathy by the smell of anxiety». PLOS ONE. 4 (6): e5987. Bibcode:2009PLoSO…4.5987P. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0005987. PMC 2695008. PMID 19551135.
  88. ^ Radulescu AR, Mujica-Parodi LR (Jul 2013). «Human gender differences in the perception of conspecific alarm chemosensory cues». PLOS ONE. 8 (7): e68485. Bibcode:2013PLoSO…868485R. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0068485. PMC 3722227. PMID 23894310.
  89. ^ Frey MC, Weyers P, Pauli P, Mühlberger A (June 2012). «Androstadienone in motor reactions of men and women toward angry faces». Perceptual and Motor Skills. 114 (3): 807–25. doi:10.2466/07.16.22.28.PMS.114.3.807-825. PMID 22913022. S2CID 13194791.
  90. ^ a b c van Kampen HS (February 2019). «The principle of consistency and the cause and function of behaviour». Behavioural Processes. 159: 42–54. doi:10.1016/j.beproc.2018.12.013. PMID 30562561. S2CID 56478466.
  91. ^ Hebb DO (September 1946). «On the nature of fear». Psychological Review. 53 (5): 259–76. doi:10.1037/h0061690. PMID 20285975. S2CID 5211697.
  92. ^ Raber J, Arzy S, Bertolus JB, Depue B, Haas HE, Hofmann SG, et al. (October 2019). «Current understanding of fear learning and memory in humans and animal models and the value of a linguistic approach for analyzing fear learning and memory in humans». Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 105: 136–177. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.03.015. PMID 30970272. S2CID 102353770.
  93. ^ Hartley CA, Phelps EA (2013). «Fear Models in Animals and Humans». In Vasa RA, Roy AK (eds.). Pediatric Anxiety Disorders. New York, NY: Springer New York. pp. 3–21. doi:10.1007/978-1-4614-6599-7_1. ISBN 978-1-4614-6598-0.
  94. ^ Luchkina NV, Bolshakov VY (January 2019). «Mechanisms of fear learning and extinction: synaptic plasticity-fear memory connection». Psychopharmacology. 236 (1): 163–182. doi:10.1007/s00213-018-5104-4. PMC 6374177. PMID 30415278.
  95. ^ Maren S (2001). «Neurobiology of Pavlovian fear conditioning». Annual Review of Neuroscience. 24: 897–931. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.897. hdl:2027.42/61939. PMID 11520922.
  96. ^ a b c d e Richter-Levin G, Stork O, Schmidt MV (August 2019). «Animal models of PTSD: a challenge to be met». Molecular Psychiatry. 24 (8): 1135–1156. doi:10.1038/s41380-018-0272-5. PMC 6756084. PMID 30816289.
  97. ^ Zoladz PR, Eisenmann ED, Rose RM, Kohls BA, Johnson BL, Robinson KL, et al. (August 2018). «Predator-based psychosocial stress model of PTSD differentially influences voluntary ethanol consumption depending on methodology». Alcohol. 70: 33–41. doi:10.1016/j.alcohol.2018.01.004. PMID 29775837. S2CID 21734755.
  98. ^ Willner P (February 2017). «The chronic mild stress (CMS) model of depression: History, evaluation and usage». Neurobiology of Stress. 6: 78–93. doi:10.1016/j.ynstr.2016.08.002. PMC 5314424. PMID 28229111.
  99. ^ Abdallah CG, Geha P (February 2017). «Chronic Pain and Chronic Stress: Two Sides of the Same Coin?». Chronic Stress. 1. doi:10.1177/2470547017704763. PMC 5546756. PMID 28795169.
  100. ^ Lisieski MJ, Eagle AL, Conti AC, Liberzon I, Perrine SA (2018-05-15). «Single-Prolonged Stress: A Review of Two Decades of Progress in a Rodent Model of Post-traumatic Stress Disorder». Frontiers in Psychiatry. 9: 196. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00196. PMC 5962709. PMID 29867615.
  101. ^ a b Souza RR, Noble LJ, McIntyre CK (2017-09-11). «Using the Single Prolonged Stress Model to Examine the Pathophysiology of PTSD». Frontiers in Pharmacology. 8: 615. doi:10.3389/fphar.2017.00615. PMC 5600994. PMID 28955225.
  102. ^ Rau V, DeCola JP, Fanselow MS (January 2005). «Stress-induced enhancement of fear learning: an animal model of posttraumatic stress disorder». Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 29 (8): 1207–23. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.010. PMID 16095698. S2CID 5896548.
  103. ^ Rajbhandari AK, Gonzalez ST, Fanselow MS (October 2018). «Stress-Enhanced Fear Learning, a Robust Rodent Model of Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder». Journal of Visualized Experiments (140): 58306. doi:10.3791/58306. PMC 6235522. PMID 30371665.
  104. ^ Rau V, Fanselow MS (March 2009). «Exposure to a stressor produces a long lasting enhancement of fear learning in rats». Stress. 12 (2): 125–33. doi:10.1080/10253890802137320. PMID 18609302. S2CID 15453890.
  105. ^ Sandi C (April 2011). «Healing anxiety disorders with glucocorticoids». Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 108 (16): 6343–4. Bibcode:2011PNAS..108.6343S. doi:10.1073/pnas.1103410108. PMC 3080972. PMID 21482789.
  106. ^ Kolber BJ, Roberts MS, Howell MP, Wozniak DF, Sands MS, Muglia LJ (August 2008). «Central amygdala glucocorticoid receptor action promotes fear-associated CRH activation and conditioning». Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 105 (33): 12004–9. Bibcode:2008PNAS..10512004K. doi:10.1073/pnas.0803216105. PMC 2575312. PMID 18695245.
  107. ^ a b Kaplan JS, Tolin DF (2011). «Exposure therapy for anxiety disorders: Theoretical mechanisms of exposure and treatment strategies». Psychiatric Times. 28 (9): 33–37. ProQuest 894207776.
  108. ^ «Cure Your Fear – Phobia Treatment». FearOf.net. Retrieved 2018-11-28.
  109. ^ «World With No Fear». NPR. 2015-01-16. Archived from the original on 2015-01-27. Retrieved 2015-01-27.
  110. ^ Goldenberg JL, Pyszczynski T, Greenberg J, Solomon S (August 2000). «Fleeing the Body: A Terror Management Perspective on the Problem of Human Corporeality». Personality and Social Psychology Review. 4 (3): 200–218. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0403_1. ISSN 1088-8683. S2CID 31331978.
  111. ^ a b c Fry PS (September 2003). «Perceived self-efficacy domains as predictors of fear of the unknown and fear of dying among older adults». Psychology and Aging. 18 (3): 474–86. doi:10.1037/0882-7974.18.3.474. PMID 14518809.
  112. ^ a b Kagan, Shelly. Lecture 22: Fear of Death Archived 2012-03-09 at the Wayback Machine in PHIL 176: Death Archived 2017-06-09 at the Wayback Machine. Yale Open Course 2007.
  113. ^ Kahoe RD, Dunn RF (1976). «The fear of death and religious attitudes and behavior». Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion. 14 (4): 379–82. doi:10.2307/1384409. JSTOR 1384409.
  114. ^ Bassett JF, Bussard ML (February 2021). «Examining the Complex Relation Among Religion, Morality, and Death Anxiety: Religion Can Be a Source of Comfort and Concern Regarding Fears of Death». Omega. 82 (3): 467–487. doi:10.1177/0030222818819343. PMID 30572785. S2CID 58619649.
  115. ^ Wink P (2006). «Who is afraid of death? Religiousness, spirituality, and death anxiety in late adulthood». Journal of Religion, Spirituality & Aging. 18 (2): 93–110. doi:10.1300/J496v18n02_08. S2CID 144684731.
  116. ^ Strimple RB. «The Fear of the Lord». The Orthodox Presbyterian Church.
  117. ^ Levy J. «How the Crusades Affected Medieval Jews in Europe and Palestine | My Jewish Learning». My Jewish Learning. Retrieved 2018-11-27.
  118. ^ «Fear and Manipulation: Perfect Together | Psychopaths and Love». psychopathsandlove.com. November 2014. Retrieved 2018-11-27.
  119. ^ Lupia, Arthur; Menning, Jesse O. (January 2009). «When Can Politicians Scare Citizens Into Supporting Bad Policies?». American Journal of Political Science. 53 (1): 90–106. doi:10.1111/j.1540-5907.2008.00359.x. ISSN 0092-5853. S2CID 13496294.
  120. ^ «Dystopia facts, information, pictures». www.encyclopedia.com. Archived from the original on 4 March 2017. Retrieved 3 March 2017.
  121. ^ Kyle RG (2012-08-01). Apocalyptic Fever: End-Time Prophecies in Modern America. Wipf and Stock Publishers. ISBN 978-1-62189-410-0. Archived from the original on 25 December 2017. Retrieved 3 March 2017.
  122. ^ Yeung K, Zhang Z (24 January 2014). «The Neverending Apocalypse». The Princeton Buffer. Archived from the original on 4 March 2017. Retrieved 3 March 2017.
  123. ^ Schmidt C (19 November 2014). «Why are Dystopian Films on the Rise Again?». JSTOR Daily. Archived from the original on 4 March 2017. Retrieved 3 March 2017.
  124. ^ a b c Conroy DE, Poczwardowski A, Henschen KP (2001). «Evaluative criteria and consequences associated with failure and success for elite athletes and performing artists». Journal of Applied Sport Psychology. 13 (3): 300–322. doi:10.1080/104132001753144428. S2CID 146422220.
  125. ^ a b c d Lazarus RS (1991). Emotion and Adaptation. New York: Oxford University Press.
  126. ^ Birney RC, Burdick H, Teevan RC (1969). Fear of failure. Van Nostrand-Reinhold Company.
  127. ^ Duda JL (1993). «Goals: A social-cognitive approach to the study of achievement motivation in sport.». In Singer RN, Murphey M, Tennant LK (eds.). Handbook of research on sport psychology. New York: Macmillan. pp. 421–36.
  128. ^ Murray H (1938). Explorations in Personal. New York: Oxford University Press.
  129. ^ a b c Conroy DE, Elliot AJ (2004). «Fear of failure and achievement goals in sport: Addressing the issue of the chicken and the egg». Anxiety, Stress & Coping. 17 (3): 271–85. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.643.3752. doi:10.1080/1061580042000191642. S2CID 15144896.

Further reading[edit]

  • Bourke J (2005). Fear: A Cultural History. Virago. ISBN 978-1-59376-113-4.
  • Robin C (2004). Fear: The History of a Political Idea. Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-515702-4.
  • Gardner D (2008). Risk: The Science and Politics of Fear. Random House, Inc. ISBN 978-0-7710-3299-8.
  • Plamper J (2012). Fear: Across the Disciplines. University of Pittsburgh Press. ISBN 978-0-8229-6220-5.
  • Wedgwood H (1855). «English Etymologies (Afraid, Affray, Fray)». Transactions of the Philological Society (8).
  • «Fear: A poem».

External links[edit]

Wikiquote has quotations related to Fear.

Wikimedia Commons has media related to Fear.

Look up fear in Wiktionary, the free dictionary.

English[edit]

Pronunciation[edit]

  • (General Australian, Received Pronunciation) IPA(key): /fɪə/
  • (General American) IPA(key): /fɪɚ/, /fɪɹ/
  • Rhymes: -ɪə(ɹ)

Etymology 1[edit]

From Middle English feer, fere, fer, from Old English fǣr, ġefǣr (calamity, sudden danger, peril, sudden attack, terrible sight), from Proto-Germanic *fērō, *fērą (danger), from Proto-Indo-European *per- (to attempt, try, research, risk). Cognate with Dutch gevaar (danger, risk, peril), German Gefahr (danger, risk, hazard), Swedish fara (danger, risk, peril), Latin perīculum (danger, risk, trial), Albanian frikë (fear, danger), Romanian frică. Doublet of peril.

The verb is from Middle English feren, from Old English fǣran (to frighten, raven), from the noun. Cognate with the archaic Dutch verb varen (to fear; to cause fear).

Noun[edit]

fear (countable and uncountable, plural fears)

  1. (uncountable) A strong, uncontrollable, unpleasant emotion or feeling caused by actual or perceived danger or threat.

    He was struck by fear on seeing the snake.

    • 1897 December (indicated as 1898), Winston Churchill, chapter VIII, in The Celebrity: An Episode, New York, N.Y.: The Macmillan Company; London: Macmillan & Co., Ltd., →OCLC:

      I corralled the judge, and we started off across the fields, in no very mild state of fear of that gentleman’s wife, whose vigilance was seldom relaxed.

    • Turning back, then, toward the basement staircase, she began to grope her way through blinding darkness, but had taken only a few uncertain steps when, of a sudden, she stopped short and for a little stood like a stricken thing, quite motionless save that she quaked to her very marrow in the grasp of a great and enervating fear.
    • 1963, Margery Allingham, chapter 18, in The China Governess[1]:

      ‘Then the father has a great fight with his terrible conscience,’ said Munday with granite seriousness. ‘Should he make a row with the police []? Or should he say nothing about it and condone brutality for fear of appearing in the newspapers?’

  2. (countable) A phobia, a sense of fear induced by something or someone.

    Not everybody has the same fears.  I have a fear of ants.

    • 1910, Emerson Hough, chapter I, in The Purchase Price: Or The Cause of Compromise, Indianapolis, Ind.: The Bobbs-Merrill Company, →OCLC:

      Serene, smiling, enigmatic, she faced him with no fear whatever showing in her dark eyes. The clear light of the bright autumn morning had no terrors for youth and health like hers.

  3. (uncountable) Terrified veneration or reverence, particularly towards God, gods, or sovereigns.
Synonyms[edit]
  • (an emotion caused by actual or perceived danger; a sense of fear induced by something or someone): See Thesaurus:fear
  • (terrified veneration): dread
Derived terms[edit]
  • affear
  • fear campaign
  • fear monger
  • fear of heights
  • fear of missing out
  • fearful
  • fearless
  • fearmonger
  • fearnaught
  • fearsome
  • for fear of
  • for fear that
  • geometry of fear
  • germ fear
  • germ-fear
  • height fear
  • height-fear
  • night fear
  • night-fear
  • nightfear
  • no fear
  • put the fear of God into
  • rub the fear of God into
  • snake fear
  • spider fear
  • spider-fear
  • spider-fear
  • stage fear
  • water fear
  • water-fear
  • without fear or favour
Translations[edit]

uncountable: emotion caused by actual or perceived danger or threat

  • Afrikaans: vrees (af)
  • Albanian: frikë (sq) f, druaj (sq)
  • Amharic: ፍርሃት f (fərhat)
  • Arabic: خَوْف (ar) m (ḵawf), وَهَل‎ m (wahal)
    Egyptian Arabic: خوف‎ m (ḵōf)
    Moroccan Arabic: خوف‎ m (ḵəwf), خْلعة‎ f (ḵləʿa)
  • Aragonese: please add this translation if you can
  • Armenian: վախ (hy) (vax), երկյուղ (hy) (erkyuł), ահ (hy) (ah)
  • Assamese: ভয় (bhoy)
  • Asturian: mieu (ast) m
  • Aymara: asxaraña
  • Azerbaijani: qorxu (az)
  • Bashkir: ҡурҡыу (qurqıw)
  • Basque: herstura
  • Bats: ჴერლʻომ (qerlˢom)
  • Belarusian: страх m (strax), бая́знь f (bajáznʹ), бо́язнь f (bójaznʹ), бо́язь f (bójazʹ)
  • Bengali: ভয় (bn) (bhoẏ), ডর (bn) (ḍor), ত্রাস (bn) (traś)
  • Breton: aon (br)
  • Bulgarian: страх (bg) m (strah), боя́зън (bg) f (bojázǎn), опасе́ние (bg) n (opasénie)
  • Burmese: ဘယာ (my) (bha.ya)
  • Catalan: por (ca) f, paüra (ca) f, basarda (ca) f, temor (ca) m or f
  • Cebuano: hadlok
  • Cherokee: ᎤᎾᏰᎯᏍᏗ (unayehisdi)
  • Chinese:
    Mandarin: 恐怖 (zh) (kǒngbù)
    Cantonese: 恐懼恐惧 (hung2 geoi6)
    Classical Chinese: ()
  • Cimbrian: dabòrte
  • Czech: strach (cs) m, bázeň (cs) f (literary), obava (cs) f
  • Danish: angst (da), frygt (da) c
  • Dutch: angst (nl), vrees (nl) f
  • Egyptian: (snḏ m), (snḏw m)
  • Esperanto: timo (eo)
  • Estonian: hirm (et), kartus
  • Even: ҥэл- (ŋəl-)
  • Evenki: ӈэлэтчэми (ŋələtçəmi)
  • Extremaduran: mieu
  • Faroese: ótti m, ræðsla f, angist f, bangilsi n
  • Finnish: pelko (fi), kammo (fi)
  • French: peur (fr) f, crainte (fr) f
  • Friulian: pôre f, timôr
  • Galician: medo (gl) m, receo m, temor (gl) m
  • Georgian: ზარი (zari), შიში (šiši)
  • German: Angst (de) f, Bange (de) f, Schreck (de) m, Furcht (de) f
    Alemannic German: Angscht f, Engschti f
  • Gothic: 𐌰𐌲𐌹𐍃 n (agis), 𐍆𐌰𐌿𐍂𐌷𐍄𐌴𐌹 f (faurhtei)
  • Greek: φόβος (el) m (fóvos)
    Ancient: φόβος m (phóbos)
  • Gujarati: please add this translation if you can
  • Hausa: please add this translation if you can
  • Hawaiian: makaʻu
  • Hebrew: פָּחַד (he) m (pákhad)
  • Higaonon: haduk
  • Hiligaynon: hadlok
  • Hindi: डर (hi) m (ḍar), भय (hi) m (bhay), ख़ौफ़ m (xauf), खौफ (hi) m (khauph), सहम (hi) m (saham)
  • Hungarian: félelem (hu)
  • Icelandic: hræðsla (is) f, beygur (is) m, ótti (is) m
  • Ido: please add this translation if you can
  • Indonesian: takut (id)
  • Interlingua: timor, pavor
  • Irish: eagla (ga) f, scéin f
  • Istriot: tamur, pagura f
  • Italian: paura (it) f, timore (it) m
  • Japanese: 恐れ (ja) (おそれ, osore), 恐怖 (ja) (きょうふ, kyōfu)
  • Javanese: wedi (jv)
  • Kannada: ಭಯ (kn) (bhaya), ಹೆದರಿಕೆ (kn) (hedarike)
  • Kashubian: strach m
  • Kazakh: қорқыныш (qorqynyş), үрей (ürei)
  • Khmer: សេចក្ដីខ្លាច (səchkdəy klaach)
  • Korean: 공포(恐怖) (ko) (gongpo),  (ko) (geop)
  • Kurdish:
    Central Kurdish: ترس (ckb) (tirs)
    Northern Kurdish: tirs (ku) f
  • Kyrgyz: коркунуч (ky) (korkunuç), коркуу (ky) (korkuu)
  • Ladino: espanto, ispantu (Monastir), temor
  • Lao: ຫວາດ (wāt), ຄວາມຢ້ານ (lo) (khuām yān), ຄວາມກົວ (khuām kūa)
  • Latgalian: baime f, baile f
  • Latin: timor m, metus m, pavor m
  • Latvian: bailes pl, bažas pl
  • Ligurian: poîa f, póia f
  • Lithuanian: baimė (lt) f, bijojimas m
  • Lombard: pora (lmo) f, pavura f
  • Low German: Furcht
  • Luxembourgish: Angscht f, Fuercht f
  • Macedonian: страв m (strav)
  • Malay: ketakutan (ms), takut (ms)
  • Malayalam: ഭയം (ml) (bhayaṃ), പേടി (ml) (pēṭi)
  • Maltese: biża f
  • Manx: aggle m
  • Maore Comorian: fazaa class 9, trisidzo
  • Maori: please add this translation if you can
  • Marathi: भीती n (bhītī)
  • Middle English: fer, ferd
  • Mirandese: miedo m
  • Mongolian: аймшиг (mn) (ajmšig)
  • Nanai: нгэлэ-
  • Nepali: डर (ḍar), भय (bhaya), त्रास (trās)
  • Ngazidja Comorian: uhara
  • Norwegian:
    Bokmål: frykt (no) f, redsel (no) m, redsel (no) m
  • Occitan: paur (oc) f
  • Old Church Slavonic:
    Cyrillic: страхъ m (straxŭ), боꙗзнь f (bojaznĭ)
    Glagolitic: ⱄⱅⱃⰰⱈⱏ m (straxŭ)
  • Old East Slavic: страхъ m (straxŭ)
  • Old English: eġe m
  • Old French: peor f
  • Old Javanese: wĕdi
  • Old Norse: ótti m, hræðsla f, uggr m
  • Old Occitan: paor f
  • Oriya: ଡର (or) (ḍôrô)
  • Oromo: sodaa
  • Ossetian: тас (tas)
  • Papiamentu: miedu
  • Pashto: ډار (ps) m
  • Persian: ترس (fa) (tars), بیم (fa) (bim), هراس (fa) (harâs), خوف (fa) (xowf)
  • Plautdietsch: Forcht f
  • Polabian: stroch m
  • Polish: strach (pl) m, niepokój (pl) m, bojaźń (pl) f
  • Portuguese: medo (pt) m, temor (pt) m, receio (pt) m (apprehensive fear)
  • Punjabi: please add this translation if you can
  • Romanian: frică (ro) f, teamă (ro) f
  • Romansch: tema f
  • Russian: страх (ru) m (strax), боя́знь (ru) f (bojáznʹ), опасе́ние (ru) n (opasénije)
  • Rusyn: страх m (strax)
  • Sanskrit: भय (sa) n (bhaya)
  • Sardinian: timoria f
  • Scots: please add this translation if you can
  • Scottish Gaelic: eagal m, uabhas m, oillt f
  • Serbo-Croatian:
    Cyrillic: стра̑х m, бо̏ја̄зан f
    Roman: strȃh (sh) m, bȍjāzan (sh) f
  • Sicilian: paura (scn) f, pagura (scn) f
  • Sinhalese: බය (baya)
  • Slovak: strach m, obava f
  • Slovene: strah (sl) m, bojazen f
  • Somali: please add this translation if you can
  • Sorbian:
    Lower Sorbian: tšach m
    Upper Sorbian: trach m
  • Spanish: miedo (es) m, temor (es) m, pavor (es) m, pavura f
  • Svan: მაყალ (maq̇al)
  • Swahili: woga (sw), hofu (sw)
  • Swedish: skräck (sv), rädsla (sv) c
  • Sylheti: ꠒꠞ (ḍor)
  • Tagalog: takot
  • Tajik: тарс (tg) (tars), ҳарос (tg) (haros), ваҳм (vahm), хавф (tg) (xavf)
  • Tamil: பயம் (ta) (payam)
  • Tatar: курку (qurku)
  • Telugu: భయము (te) (bhayamu)
  • Tetum: ta’uk
  • Divehi: ބިރުވެރިކަމަކީ(biruverikamakī)
  • Thai: ความกลัว (th) (kwaam-gluua)
  • Tibetan: ཞེད་སྣང (zhed snang)
  • Tocharian B: īwate, parskalñe
  • Turkish: korku (tr)
  • Turkmen: gorky, heder
  • Ukrainian: страх (uk) m (strax), боя́знь (uk) f (bojáznʹ), ляк m (ljak)
  • Urdu: ڈر‎ m (ḍar), خوف(xauf), بھی‎ m (bhay)
  • Uyghur: قورقۇش(qorqush)
  • Uzbek: qoʻrquv (uz), doʻq (uz)
  • Vietnamese: sự khiếp đảm, sự sợ hãi
  • Volapük: dred (vo)
  • Walloon: paw (wa) f, peu (wa) f
  • Welsh: ofn (cy)
  • White Hmong: ntshai
  • Yiddish: שרעק‎ m or f (shrek), מורא‎ f (moyre)
  • Yoruba: ẹ̀rù
  • Zhuang: please add this translation if you can

a phobia; sense of fear induced by something or someone

  • Armenian: վախ (hy) (vax)
  • Albanian: drojë (sq) f
  • Asturian: mieu (ast) m
  • Bulgarian: фобия (bg) f (fobija)
  • Catalan: por (ca) f, paüra (ca) f, basarda (ca) f, temor (ca) m or f
  • Chinese:
    Mandarin: 恐怖 (zh) (kǒngbù)
  • Czech: strach (cs) m
  • Danish: angst (da), frygt (da) c
  • Dutch: angst (nl) f, schrik (nl) f
  • Esperanto: timo (eo), fobio
  • Estonian: hirm (et), kartus
  • Finnish: pelko (fi)
  • French: crainte (fr) f, peur (fr) f
  • Galician: medo (gl) m
  • Georgian: შიში (šiši)
  • German: Angst (de) f, Furcht (de) f, Phobie (de) f
  • Greek: φοβία (el) f (fovía)
  • Hawaiian: makaʻu
  • Hindi: घबराहट (hi) f (ghabrāhaṭ)
  • Irish: eagla (ga) f
  • Italian: paura (it) f
  • Japanese: 恐怖症 (kyōfushō)
  • Khmer: សេចក្ដីខ្លាច (səchkdəy klaach)
  • Kurdish:
    Central Kurdish: ترس (ckb) (tirs), ترس و لەرز(tirs û lerz)
    Northern Kurdish: tirs (ku) f
  • Latin: pavor
  • Ligurian: poîa f, póia f
  • Malay: ketakutan (ms)
  • Malayalam: ഭയം (ml) (bhayaṃ)
  • Marathi: घाबरने m or f (ghābarne)
  • Occitan: paur (oc) f
  • Old English: eġe m
  • Old French: peor f
  • Polish: lęk (pl) m
  • Portuguese: fobia (pt) f, medo (pt) m, pavor (pt) m
  • Romanian: fobie (ro)
  • Romansch: tema f
  • Russian: страх (ru) m (strax), боя́знь (ru) f (bojáznʹ), фо́бия (ru) f (fóbija)
  • Sardinian: timoria f
  • Scots: please add this translation if you can
  • Scottish Gaelic: eagal m, uabhas m, oillt f
  • Serbo-Croatian:
    Cyrillic: стра̑х m
    Roman: strȃh (sh) m
  • Slovene: strah (sl) m
  • Spanish: miedo (es) m, pavor (es) m
  • Swahili: kicho (sw) class 7/8
  • Swedish: fruktan (sv), rädsla (sv)
  • Tagalog: takot
  • Zazaki: ters (diq)

extreme veneration or awe

  • Armenian: երկյուղ (hy) (erkyuł)
  • Bulgarian: боязън (bg) f (bojazǎn)
  • Catalan: temor (ca) m, respecte (ca) m
  • Czech: strach (cs) m
  • Danish: frygt (da) c
  • Finnish: pelko (fi)
  • French: crainte (fr) f
  • German: Ehrfurcht (de) f
  • Greek: δέος (el) n (déos)
  • Hindi: परवाह (hi) f (parvāh)
  • Irish: eagla (ga) f
  • Japanese: 畏れ (osore)
  • Kurdish:
    Central Kurdish: ترس (ckb) (tirs)
    Northern Kurdish: tirs (ku) f
  • Malay: ketakutan (ms)
  • Old English: eġe m
  • Persian: پروا (fa) (parvâ)
  • Portuguese: temor (pt) m
  • Romanian: frică (ro) f, spaimă (ro) f
  • Russian: страх (ru) m (strax)
  • Scottish Gaelic: eagal m, uabhas m, oillt f
  • Serbo-Croatian:
    Cyrillic: стра̑х m
    Roman: strȃh (sh) m
  • Slovene: strahospoštovanje n
  • Spanish: temor (es) m, respeto (es) m
  • Swedish: fruktan (sv)

Translations to be checked

  • Albanian: (please verify) frikë (sq) f
  • Breton: (please verify) aon (br) m
  • Esperanto: (please verify) timo (eo)
  • Guaraní: (please verify) kyhyje
  • Hebrew: (please verify) פחד (he) m (pákhad)
  • Indonesian: (please verify) takut (id)
  • Interlingua: (please verify) timor, (please verify) pavor
  • Latin: (please verify) timor, (please verify) pavor, (please verify) metus
  • Maori: (please verify) mataku (mi)
  • Northern Sami: (please verify) ballu
  • Old English: (please verify) eġe
  • Romanian: (please verify) frică (ro) f, (please verify) teamă (ro) f
  • Spanish: (please verify) miedo (es) m, (please verify) temor (es) m
  • Telugu: (please verify) భయం (te) (bhayaṁ)
  • Tupinambá: (please verify) sykyîé
  • Turkish: (please verify) korku (tr)

Verb[edit]

fear (third-person singular simple present fears, present participle fearing, simple past and past participle feared)

  1. (transitive) To be afraid of (something or someone); to consider or expect (something or someone) with alarm.

    I fear the worst will happen.

    • c. 1594 (date written), William Shakespeare, “The Comedie of Errors”, in Mr. William Shakespeares Comedies, Histories, & Tragedies [] (First Folio), London: [] Isaac Iaggard, and Ed[ward] Blount, published 1623, →OCLC, [Act I, scene ii]:

      I greatly fear my money is not safe.

    • At twilight in the summer there is never anybody to fear—man, woman, or cat—in the chambers and at that hour the mice come out. They do not eat parchment or foolscap or red tape, but they eat the luncheon crumbs.
    • 2013 July 19, Mark Tran, “Denied an education by war”, in The Guardian Weekly, volume 189, number 6, page 1:

      One particularly damaging, but often ignored, effect of conflict on education is the proliferation of attacks on schools [] as children, teachers or school buildings become the targets of attacks. Parents fear sending their children to school. Girls are particularly vulnerable to sexual violence.

  2. (intransitive) To feel fear.

    Never fear; help is always near.

  3. (intransitive, used with for) To worry about, to feel concern for, to be afraid for.

    She fears for her son’s safety.

  4. (transitive) To venerate; to feel awe towards.

    People who fear God can be found in Christian churches.

  5. (transitive) To regret.

    I fear I have bad news for you: your husband has died.

  6. (obsolete, transitive) To cause fear to; to frighten.
    • 1485, Sir Thomas Malory, chapter X, in Le Morte Darthur, book V:

      Thenne the knyghte sayd to syre Gawayn / bynde thy wounde or thy blee chaunge / for thou bybledest al thy hors and thy fayre armes / [] / For who someuer is hurte with this blade he shalle neuer be staunched of bledynge / Thenne ansuerd gawayn hit greueth me but lytyl / thy grete wordes shalle not feare me ne lasse my courage

      (please add an English translation of this quote)
    • c. 1590–1592 (date written), William Shakespeare, “The Taming of the Shrew”, in Mr. William Shakespeares Comedies, Histories, & Tragedies [] (First Folio), London: [] Isaac Iaggard, and Ed[ward] Blount, published 1623, →OCLC, [Act I, scene ii]:

      Tush, tush! fear boys with bugs.

    • c. 1603–1604 (date written), William Shakespeare, “Measure for Measure”, in Mr. William Shakespeares Comedies, Histories, & Tragedies [] (First Folio), London: [] Isaac Iaggard, and Ed[ward] Blount, published 1623, →OCLC, [Act II, scene i]:

      We must not make a scarecrow of the law,
      Setting it up to fear the birds of prey,
      And let it keep one shape, till custom make it
      Their perch and not their terror.

  7. (obsolete, transitive) To be anxious or solicitous for.
    • c. 1596–1598 (date written), William Shakespeare, “The Merchant of Venice”, in Mr. William Shakespeares Comedies, Histories, & Tragedies [] (First Folio), London: [] Isaac Iaggard, and Ed[ward] Blount, published 1623, →OCLC, [Act III, scene v]:

      The sins of the father are to be laid upon the children: therefore, I promise ye, I fear you.

  8. (obsolete, transitive) To suspect; to doubt.
    • 1591 (date written), William Shakespeare, “The Second Part of Henry the Sixt, []”, in Mr. William Shakespeares Comedies, Histories, & Tragedies. [] (First Folio), London: [] Isaac Iaggard, and Ed[ward] Blount, published 1623, →OCLC, [Act I, scene iv]:

      Fear you not her courage?

Synonyms[edit]
  • (feel fear about (something)): be afraid of, be frightened of, be scared of, be terrorised/terrorized
  • (venerate; to feel awe towards): be in awe of, revere, venerate
Antonyms[edit]
  • (venerate; to feel awe towards): belittle, contemn
Derived terms[edit]
  • Allah-fearing
  • fear not
  • fools rush in where angels fear to tread
  • God-fearing
  • gods-fearing
  • never fear
Translations[edit]

feel fear about (something)

  • Albanian: ka frikë
  • Alviri-Vidari: بترسستن(betersesten)
  • Arabic: خَافَ(ḵāfa), خَشِيَ(ḵašiya), هَابَ(hāba), رَهِبَ(rahiba)
    Egyptian Arabic: خاف(ḵāf)
    Moroccan Arabic: خاف(ḵæf), تْخْلع(tḵlæʿ)
  • Aramaic:
    Classical Syriac: ܕܚܠ(dħel)
  • Armenian: վախենալ (hy) (vaxenal), երկյուղել (hy) (erkyułel), երկնչել (hy) (erknčʿel)
  • Asturian: temer, atarrecer (ast)
  • Azerbaijani: qorxmaq (az)
  • Belarusian: бая́цца impf (bajácca), пужа́цца impf (pužácca), пало́хацца impf (palóxacca), страшы́цца impf (strašýcca), ляка́цца impf (ljakácca)
  • Bengali: ভয় করা (bhoẏ kora)
  • Bulgarian: боя́ се impf (bojá se), страху́вам се impf (strahúvam se), пла́ша се (bg) impf (pláša se)
  • Burmese: ကြောက် (my) (krauk)
  • Catalan: témer (ca), tenir por de
  • Cherokee: ᎠᏍᎦᎢᎭ (asgaiha)
  • Chinese:
    Mandarin: 害怕 (zh) (hàipà),  (zh) ()
  • Czech: bát se impf
  • Danish: frygte (da), være bange for
  • Dutch: vrezen (nl)
  • Egyptian: (snḏ)
  • Esperanto: timi (eo)
  • Estonian: kartma
  • Faroese: stúra fyri, ræðast (fo)
  • Finnish: pelätä (fi)
  • French: craindre (fr), avoir peur (fr) (de)
  • Friulian: temê
  • Galician: temer (gl), recear (gl)
  • Georgian: ეშინია (ešinia)
  • German: fürchten (de), Angst haben
  • Gothic: 𐍉𐌲𐌰𐌽 (ōgan)
  • Greek: φοβάμαι (el) (fovámai)
    Ancient: φοβέομαι (phobéomai), δείδω (deídō), ὀκνέω (oknéō) (fear of doing something)
  • Guaraní: (please verify) …rehe kyhyje
  • Haitian Creole:
  • Hebrew: פָּחַד (he) (pakhád), יָרֵא (he) (yaré)
  • Hindi: डरना (hi) (ḍarnā)
  • Hungarian: fél (hu)
  • Icelandic: óttast (is), hræðast
  • Ido: timar (io)
  • Indonesian: takut (id)
  • Interlingua: timer, haber timor
  • Irish: eagla a bheith ort roimh
    Old Irish: ad·ágathar
  • Italian: temere (it), aver paura di
  • Japanese: 恐れる (ja) (おそれる, osoreru), 怖る (おそる, osoru), 怖がる (ja) (こわがる, kowagaru)
  • Kashubian: bòjec
  • Kazakh: қорқу (kk) (qorqu), жасқану (jasqanu)
  • Khmer: ខ្លាច (km) (khlaac)
  • Korean: 두려워하다 (ko) (duryeowohada), 무섭다 (ko) (museopda)
  • Kurdish:
    Central Kurdish: ترسین (ckb) (tirsîn)
    Northern Kurdish: tirsîn (ku)
  • Kyrgyz: коркуу (ky) (korkuu)
  • Lao: ຢ້ານກົວ (yān kūa), ກົວ (kūa), ຂຍາດ (kha nyāt), ຂາມ (khām)
  • Latin: timeō, paveō, veror, vereor (la), formidō, metuō
  • Latvian: baidīties, bīties, bažīties, baiļoties
  • Lithuanian: bijoti, baimintis, būgštauti, nuogąstauti
  • Macedonian: се стравува impf (se stravuva), се плаши impf (se plaši)
  • Malay: takut (ms)
  • Malayalam: ഭയക്കുക (ml) (bhayakkuka), പേടിക്കുക (ml) (pēṭikkuka)
  • Manchu: ᡤᡝᠯᡝᠮᠪᡳ (gelembi), ᠰᡝᠰᡠᠯᠠᠮᠪᡳ (sesulambi), ᠰᡝᠩᡤᡠᠸᡝᠮᠪᡳ (sengguwembi), ᡤᠣᠯᠣᠮᠪᡳ (golombi)
  • Maori: uruwehi
  • Mbyá Guaraní: kyje
  • Middle English: anoyen
  • Mon: ဖေက် (mnw)
  • Mongolian: айх (mn) (ajx)
  • Nepali: डराउनु (ḍarāunu), तर्सिनु (tarsinu), तर्सिनु (tarsinu)
  • Norman: croindre (Guernsey)
  • Northern Sami: ballat
  • Norwegian:
    Bokmål: frykte
  • Occitan: témer (oc), crénher (oc), crentar (oc)
  • Old English: ondrǣdan
  • Old Norse: hræðask, ugga
  • Persian: ترسیدن (fa) (tarsidan)
  • Polish: bać się (pl) impf, lękać się impf
  • Portuguese: temer (pt), ter medo de, recear (pt)
  • Quechua: manchay, mancai, manzai
  • Rapa Nui: mataku
  • Romanian: teme (ro)
  • Romansch: temair, temer, tmair
  • Russian: боя́ться (ru) impf (bojátʹsja) (+ genitive), страши́ться (ru) impf (strašítʹsja) (+ genitive), опаса́ться (ru) impf (opasátʹsja) (+ genitive), пуга́ться (ru) impf (pugátʹsja)
  • Sanskrit: बिभेति (sa) (bibheti), त्रसति (sa) (trasati)
  • Santali: ᱵᱳᱨᱳ (boro)
  • Sardinian: tímere, timi
  • Serbo-Croatian:
    Cyrillic: бо̀јати се impf, пла̏шити се impf
    Roman: bòjati se (sh) impf, plȁšiti se (sh) impf
  • Slovak: báť sa impf
  • Slovene: báti se impf
  • Sorbian:
    Lower Sorbian: bójaś impf
  • Spanish: temer (es), tener miedo de
  • Sundanese: sebér
  • Swahili: -ogopa, -hofu (sw)
  • Swedish: frukta (sv), rädas (sv), (of something being the case or coming to pass) befara (sv)
  • Tagalog: pagtakot
  • Tajik: тарсидан (tarsidan)
  • Tatar: курку (qurku)
  • Thai: กลัว (th) (gluua), หวั่น (th) (wàn), เกรง (th) (greeng), ขาม (th) (kǎam), คร้าม (th) (kráam), ประหวั่น (th) (bprà-wàn), ปอด (th) (bpɔ̀ɔt)
  • Tocharian B: pärsk-
  • Tupinambá: (please verify) …resé sykyîé
  • Turkish: korkmak (tr), ürkmek (tr)
  • Turkmen: gorkmak
  • Ugaritic: 𐎄𐎈𐎍 (dḥl)
  • Ukrainian: боя́тися (uk) impf (bojátysja), ляка́тися impf (ljakátysja), поло́хатися impf (polóxatysja), страши́тися impf (strašýtysja), страха́тися impf (straxátysja)
  • Urdu: ڈرنا(ḍarnā)
  • Uzbek: qoʻrqmoq (uz), hayiqmoq (uz)
  • Venetian: temer
  • Vietnamese: sợ (vi), hãi (vi), sợ hãi (vi)
  • Walloon: awè peu (wa), aveur paw, crinde (wa), ricrinde (wa)
  • Yakut: куттан (kuttan)
  • Yiddish: מוירע האָבן(moyre hobn)

venerate; to feel awe towards

  • Arabic: خَافَ(ḵāfa), خَشِيَ(ḵašiya), رَهِبَ(rahiba), اِتَّقَى(ittaqā)
  • Armenian: վախենալ (hy) (vaxenal)
  • Catalan: témer (ca), respectar (ca)
  • Czech: bát se
  • French: craindre (fr)
  • Galician: temer (gl)
  • German: fürchten (de)
  • Haitian Creole:
  • Hungarian: fél (hu)
  • Japanese: 畏れる (ja) (おそれる, osoreru)
  • Kurdish:
    Central Kurdish: ترسین (ckb) (tirsîn)
    Northern Kurdish: tirsîn (ku)
  • Malay: takut (ms)
  • Portuguese: temer (pt)
  • Swahili: kucha (sw)
  • Swedish: frukta (sv), vörda (sv)
  • Walloon: crinde (wa)

Etymology 2[edit]

From Middle English fere, feore, from Old English fēre (able to go, fit for service), from Proto-Germanic *fōriz (passable), from Proto-Indo-European *per- (to put across, ferry). Cognate with Scots fere, feir (well, active, sound), Middle High German gevüere (able, capable, fit, serviceable), Swedish för (capable, able, stout), Icelandic færr (able). Related to fare.

Adjective[edit]

fear (comparative more fear, superlative most fear)

  1. (dialectal) Able; capable; stout; strong; sound.

    hale and fear

Alternative forms[edit]
  • feer

Anagrams[edit]

  • FERA, Fera, Rafe, fare, reaf

Irish[edit]

Pronunciation[edit]

  • IPA(key): /fʲaɾˠ/
  • (Cois Fharraige) IPA(key): /fʲæɾˠ/

Etymology 1[edit]

Inherited from Old Irish fer, from Proto-Celtic *wiros, from Proto-Indo-European *wiHrós.

Cognate with Welsh gŵr, Breton gour, Cornish gour, Gaulish viros, Latin vir, Sanskrit वीर (vīra), Lithuanian výras, Avestan 𐬬𐬍𐬭𐬀(vīra), and Old English wer.

Noun[edit]

fear m (genitive singular fir, nominative plural fir)

  1. man (adult male)

    Tá an fear ag ól uisce.

    The man is drinking water.

    Sláinte chuig na fir agus go marfuire na mná go deo!

    Health to the men and may the women live forever!
  2. husband, male spouse
Declension[edit]
Derived terms[edit]
  • ardfhear (excellent man)
  • baintreach fir (widower)
  • banaltra fir (male nurse)
  • bunfhear
  • calmfhear
  • ceartfhear
  • cnámhfhear (bones)
  • crannfhear (aging man)
  • dea-fhear
  • dréimire fir (tall thin man)
  • fear adaic
  • fear ascaille (second oarsman)
  • fear banna
  • fear bréige (scarecrow)
  • fear cathaoireach (chairman)
  • fear céile (husband)
  • fear dóiteáin
  • fear falsa (scarecrow)
  • fear fichille (chess piece, chessman)
  • fear ionaid (deputy; vicar)
  • fear margaidh (marketeer; bargain-maker)
  • fear na gcrúb (the Devil; joker)
  • fear siúil
  • fear sneachta (snowman)
  • fear sorcais (circus clown)
  • fear- (man-, male; manly, he-)
  • fearchumhacht
  • fearúil (manly, masculine;, adjective)
  • firín (little man, manikin, diminutive)
  • fuathaitheoir fear (misandrist)
  • murúch fir (merman)
  • ógfhear (young man)
  • saorfhear
  • seanfhear (old man)
  • slatfhear
  • tréanfhear

Etymology 2[edit]

From Middle Irish feraid, from Old Irish feraid.

Verb[edit]

fear (present analytic fearann, future analytic fearfaidh, verbal noun fearadh, past participle feartha)

  1. (transitive) grant, provide
  2. (transitive) pour out, give forth, shed
  3. (transitive) wage
  4. (transitive) perform, execute; hold, observe
  5. (transitive) affect; benefit
  6. (transitive) excrete
Conjugation[edit]
singular plural relative autonomous
first second third first second third
indicative present fearaim fearann tú;
fearair
fearann sé, sí fearaimid fearann sibh fearann siad;
fearaid
a fhearann; a fhearas /
a bhfearann*; a bhfearas*
feartar
past d’fhear mé; d’fhearas /
fhear mé‡; fhearas
d’fhear tú; d’fhearais /
fhear tú; fhearais
d’fhear sé, sí /
fhear sé, sí‡
d’fhearamar; d’fhear muid /
fhearamar; fhear muid‡
d’fhear sibh; d’fhearabhair /
fhear sibh;
fhearabhair
d’fhear siad; d’fhearadar /
fhear siad; fhearadar
a d’fhear /
ar fhear*
fearadh
past habitual d’fhearainn /
fhearainn‡; bhfearainn‡‡
d’fheartá /
fheartá‡; bhfeartᇇ
d’fhearadh sé, sí /
fhearadh sé, sí‡; bhfearadh sé, s퇇
d’fhearaimis; d’fhearadh muid /
fhearaimis; fhearadh muid‡; bhfearaimis‡‡; bhfearadh muid‡‡
d’fhearadh sibh /
fhearadh sibh‡; bhfearadh sibh‡‡
d’fhearaidís; d’fhearadh siad /
fhearaidís; fhearadh siad‡; bhfearaidís‡‡; bhfearadh siad‡‡
a d’fhearadh /
a bhfearadh*
d’fheartaí /
fheartaí‡; bhfearta퇇
future fearfaidh mé;
fearfad
fearfaidh tú;
fearfair
fearfaidh sé, sí fearfaimid;
fearfaidh muid
fearfaidh sibh fearfaidh siad;
fearfaid
a fhearfaidh; a fhearfas /
a bhfearfaidh*; a bhfearfas*
fearfar
conditional d’fhearfainn / fhearfainn‡; bhfearfainn‡‡ d’fhearfá / fhearfá‡; bhfearfᇇ d’fhearfadh sé, sí / fhearfadh sé, sí‡; bhfearfadh sé, s퇇 d’fhearfaimis; d’fhearfadh muid / fhearfaimis‡; fhearfadh muid‡; bhfearfaimis‡‡; bhfearfadh muid‡‡ d’fhearfadh sibh / fhearfadh sibh‡; bhfearfadh sibh‡‡ d’fhearfaidís; d’fhearfadh siad / fhearfaidís‡; fhearfadh siad‡; bhfearfaidís‡‡; bhfearfadh siad‡‡ a d’fhearfadh /
a bhfearfadh*
d’fhearfaí / fhearfaí‡; bhfearfa퇇
subjunctive present go bhfeara mé;
go bhfearad
go bhfeara tú;
go bhfearair
go bhfeara sé, sí go bhfearaimid;
go bhfeara muid
go bhfeara sibh go bhfeara siad;
go bhfearaid
go bhfeartar
past bhfearainn bhfeartá bhfearadh sé, sí bhfearaimis;
bhfearadh muid
bhfearadh sibh bhfearaidís;
bhfearadh siad
bhfeartaí
imperative fearaim fear fearadh sé, sí fearaimis fearaigí;
fearaidh
fearaidís feartar
verbal noun fearadh
past participle feartha

* Indirect relative
† Archaic or dialect form
‡ Dependent form
‡‡ Dependent form used with particles that trigger eclipsis

Mutation[edit]

Irish mutation
Radical Lenition Eclipsis
fear fhear bhfear
Note: Some of these forms may be hypothetical. Not every possible mutated form of every word actually occurs.

Further reading[edit]

  • Ó Dónaill, Niall (1977), “fear”, in Foclóir Gaeilge–Béarla, Dublin: An Gúm, →ISBN
  • G. Toner, M. Ní Mhaonaigh, S. Arbuthnot, D. Wodtko, M.-L. Theuerkauf, editors (2019), “1 fer”, in eDIL: Electronic Dictionary of the Irish Language
  • de Bhaldraithe, Tomás (1977) Gaeilge Chois Fhairrge: An Deilbhíocht (in Irish), 2nd edition, Dublin Institute for Advanced Studies, § 5 and p. 339
  • Entries containing “fear” in English-Irish Dictionary, An Gúm, 1959, by Tomás de Bhaldraithe.
  • Entries containing “fear” in New English-Irish Dictionary by Foras na Gaeilge.
  • Sjoestedt, M. L. (1931) Phonétique d’un parler irlandais de Kerry (in French), Paris: Librairie Ernest Leroux, page 30

Scots[edit]

Noun[edit]

fear (plural fears)

  1. fear

Verb[edit]

fear (third-person singular simple present fears, present participle fearin, simple past feart, past participle feart)

  1. to fear
  2. to frighten, scare

Scottish Gaelic[edit]

Etymology[edit]

Inherited from Old Irish fer, from Proto-Celtic *wiros, from Proto-Indo-European *wiHrós.

Pronunciation[edit]

  • IPA(key): /fɛɾ/

Noun[edit]

fear m (genitive singular fir, plural fir)

  1. man
  2. husband, male spouse

Declension[edit]

Derived terms[edit]

  • fear an taighe
  • fear-
  • fear-taighe
  • fireann
  • fireannach
  • fireannta

Pronoun[edit]

fear (genitive fir)

  1. somebody, something, one

Usage notes[edit]

  • Used when referring to a singular masculine subject.
  • For feminine subjects is used. Alternatively, neach can be used for either gender.
  • In the plural feadhainn is used for both genders.

Derived terms[edit]

  • feareigin
  • fear mu seach

Mutation[edit]

Scottish Gaelic mutation
Radical Lenition
fear fhear
Note: Some of these forms may be hypothetical. Not every possible mutated form of every word actually occurs.

See also[edit]

  • bean

Further reading[edit]

  • G. Toner, M. Ní Mhaonaigh, S. Arbuthnot, D. Wodtko, M.-L. Theuerkauf, editors (2019), “1 fer”, in eDIL: Electronic Dictionary of the Irish Language

West Frisian[edit]

Etymology 1[edit]

From Old Frisian fethere, from Proto-West Germanic *feþru, from Proto-Germanic *feþrō, from Proto-Indo-European *péth₂r̥.

Cognate with English feather, Greek φτερό (fteró, wing, feather), Latin penna (wing, feather) and Irish éan (bird).

Noun[edit]

fear c (plural fearren, diminutive fearke)

  1. feather
  2. spring (mechanical device)
Further reading[edit]
  • “fear (I)”, in Wurdboek fan de Fryske taal (in Dutch), 2011

Etymology 2[edit]

From Proto-Germanic *farjǭ. Cognate with Dutch veer, English ferry.

Noun[edit]

fear n (plural fearen)

  1. ferry
Further reading[edit]
  • “fear (II)”, in Wurdboek fan de Fryske taal (in Dutch), 2011

Etymology 3[edit]

From Old Frisian *farn, from Proto-West Germanic *farn.

Noun[edit]

fear c (plural fearen)

  1. fern
Further reading[edit]
  • “fear (III)”, in Wurdboek fan de Fryske taal (in Dutch), 2011

Etymology 4[edit]

From Old Frisian *farch, from Proto-West Germanic *farh. Cognate with English farrow.

Adjective[edit]

fear

  1. farrow
Inflection[edit]
Inflection of fear
uninflected fear
inflected feare
comparative
positive
predicative/adverbial fear
indefinite c. sing. feare
n. sing. fear
plural feare
definite feare
partitive fears
Further reading[edit]
  • “fear (V)”, in Wurdboek fan de Fryske taal (in Dutch), 2011

  • Top Definitions
  • Synonyms
  • Quiz
  • Related Content
  • Examples
  • British
  • Idioms And Phrases

This shows grade level based on the word’s complexity.

This shows grade level based on the word’s complexity.


noun

a distressing emotion aroused by impending danger, evil, pain, etc., whether the threat is real or imagined; the feeling or condition of being afraid.

a specific instance of or propensity for such a feeling: an abnormal fear of heights.

concern or anxiety; solicitude: a fear for someone’s safety.

reverential awe, especially toward God: the fear of God.

something that causes feelings of dread or apprehension; something a person is afraid of: Cancer is a common fear.

anticipation of the possibility that something unpleasant will occur: Having grown up during the Great Depression, he had a constant fear of running out of money.

verb (used with object)

to regard with fear; be afraid of.

to have reverential awe of.

to consider or anticipate (something unpleasant) with a feeling of dread or alarm: It’s about to snow again, I fear.

Archaic. to experience fear in (oneself): I fear me he will ne’er forgive us.

verb (used without object)

to have fear; be afraid: I’ll go with you, so do not fear!

to feel apprehensive or uneasy (usually followed by for): In this time of economic instability, I fear for my children’s future.

OTHER WORDS FOR fear

1 foreboding, apprehension, consternation, dismay, dread, terror, fright, panic, horror, trepidation, qualm.

See synonyms for fear on Thesaurus.com

QUIZ

CAN YOU ANSWER THESE COMMON GRAMMAR DEBATES?

There are grammar debates that never die; and the ones highlighted in the questions in this quiz are sure to rile everyone up once again. Do you know how to answer the questions that cause some of the greatest grammar debates?

Which sentence is correct?

Idioms about fear

    for fear of / that, in order to prevent or avoid the risk of: She is afraid to say anything for fear of the consequences.

    put the fear of God in / into, to cause to be greatly afraid.

Origin of fear

First recorded before 900; Middle English fere, Old English fær “sudden attack or danger”; cognate with Old Saxon fār “ambush,” Dutch gevaar, German Gefahr “danger,” Old Norse fār “disaster”

synonym study for fear

Fear, alarm, dread all imply a painful emotion experienced when one is confronted by threatening danger or evil. Alarm implies an agitation of the feelings caused by awakening to imminent danger; it names a feeling of fright or panic: He started up in alarm. Fear and dread usually refer more to a condition or state than to an event. Fear is often applied to an attitude toward something, which, when experienced, will cause the sensation of fright: fear of falling. Dread suggests anticipation of something, usually a particular event, which, when experienced, will be disagreeable rather than frightening: She lives in dread of losing her money. The same is often true of fear, when used in a negative statement: She has no fear of losing her money.

popular references for fear

Fear and Trembling: A philosophical exploration of faith and ethics by Sören Kierkegaard. Published in 1843 under the pseudonym Johannes de Silentio.
Fear and Loathing in Las Vegas: A Savage Journey to the Heart of the American Dream: A roman à clef by gonzo journalist Hunter S. Thompson, illustrated by Ralph Steadman. First printed in 1971 as a two-part series in Rolling Stone magazine, and as a novel in 1972.
Fear and Loathing in Las Vegas: A film adaptation of the Hunter S. Thompson book, directed by Terry Gilliam. Released in 1998.
The Culture of Fear: Why Americans Are Afraid of the Wrong Things: 1999 book by sociologist Barry Glassner, examining why Americans’ fears are misplaced and exaggerated. Revised and updated in 2010.
Fear Factor: An American reality game show (2001–2006) in which contestants had to complete a series of dangerous, disgusting, or otherwise fear-inducing stunts.
—No FEAR Act: The Notification and Federal Employee Antidiscrimination and Retaliation Act of 2002 (Public Law 107–174). The act, signed into law by President George W. Bush in 2002, increases Federal agency accountability for acts of discrimination or reprisal against employees.

OTHER WORDS FROM fear

self-fearing, adjectiveun·fear·ing, adjective

Words nearby fear

FDP, FDR, Fe, feal, fealty, fear, feared, fearful, fearfully, fearless, fearnought

Other definitions for fear (2 of 2)


noun

a river in SE North Carolina. 202 miles (325 km) long.

Cape, a cape at its mouth.

Dictionary.com Unabridged
Based on the Random House Unabridged Dictionary, © Random House, Inc. 2023

Words related to fear

alarm, angst, anxiety, apprehension, awe, concern, despair, dismay, doubt, dread, horror, jitters, panic, scare, suspicion, terror, unease, uneasiness, worry, anticipate

How to use fear in a sentence

  • Others voice fear that their taxes would be raised under Biden.

  • There’s a palpable fear among Democrats that voters will blame them on Election Day should they appear to be putting their own re-elections ahead of what’s good for Americans.

  • She was asked by senior Pence aides, she said, to help on an op-ed for the Wall Street Journal that minimized the fears of a second coronavirus wave and touted the administration’s work on the virus as a success story.

  • Cleopatra and Toliver conquer obstacles and fears under Bell’s tutelage and draw connections between the trail and life.

  • That might seem tangential to solving your back pain, but the truth is that a large part of overcoming that discomfort is about overcoming the fear of being in pain.

  • Before anti-vaxxers, there were anti-fluoriders: a group who spread fear about the anti-tooth decay agent added to drinking water.

  • The fear of violence should not determine what one does or does not say.

  • Stephanie Giorgio, a classical musician, credits The Class for helping her cope with anxiety, focus, fear, and self-doubt.

  • The choice between freedom and fear is not difficult when seen with perspective.

  • The decision not to run the cartoons is motivated by nothing more than fear: either fear of offending or fear of retaliation.

  • Truth is a torch, but one of enormous size; so that we slink past it in rather a blinking fashion for fear it should burn us.

  • To others the fierce desire for social justice obliterates all fear of a general catastrophe.

  • Her heart fluttered violently with fear as she saw that he stepped out after her, and walked by her side toward the house.

  • And for fear of being ill spoken of weep bitterly for a day, and then comfort thyself in thy sadness.

  • The water suggested the fear that he must be nearing the open sea, and he became supernaturally grave.

British Dictionary definitions for fear


noun

a feeling of distress, apprehension, or alarm caused by impending danger, pain, etc

a cause of this feeling

awe; reverencefear of God

concern; anxiety

possibility; chancethere is no fear of that happening

for fear of, for fear that or for fear lest to forestall or avoid

no fear certainly not

put the fear of God into to frighten

verb

to be afraid (to do something) or of (a person or thing); dread

(tr) to revere; respect

(tr; takes a clause as object) to be sorry: used to lessen the effect of an unpleasant statementI fear that you have not won

(intr foll by for) to feel anxiety about something

Derived forms of fear

fearer, nounfearless, adjectivefearlessly, adverbfearlessness, noun

Word Origin for fear

Old English fǣr; related to Old High German fāra, Old Norse fār hostility, Latin perīculum danger

Collins English Dictionary — Complete & Unabridged 2012 Digital Edition
© William Collins Sons & Co. Ltd. 1979, 1986 © HarperCollins
Publishers 1998, 2000, 2003, 2005, 2006, 2007, 2009, 2012

Other Idioms and Phrases with fear


see fools rush in where angels fear to tread; for fear of; never fear; put the fear of god in.

The American Heritage® Idioms Dictionary
Copyright © 2002, 2001, 1995 by Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company. Published by Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company.

Noun



He was trembling with fear.



unable to walk the streets without fear of being mugged



They regarded their enemies with fear and hatred.



I’ve been trying to overcome my fear of flying.



The doctor’s diagnosis confirmed our worst fears.



The government is trying to allay fears of a recession.



Employees expressed fears that the company would go out of business.



He told us about all his hopes and fears.



She has a morbid fear of cats.

Verb



He was a cruel king who was feared and hated by his subjects.



There’s no need to fear.

See More

Recent Examples on the Web



The rocket fire raised fears of a wider conflagration as Jews began the week-long Passover holiday, hundreds of Christians in the Old City gathered for Holy Thursday at the Holy Sepulcher to mark the Last Supper, and Muslims marked the Ramadan holy month.


Sam Mcneil, BostonGlobe.com, 6 Apr. 2023





Palestinian militants in the Gaza Strip renewed their rocket fire at Israel, raising fears of a wider conflagration.


Isabel Debre And Fares Akram, Anchorage Daily News, 6 Apr. 2023





What is your biggest fear about motherhood?


Joelle Goldstein, Peoplemag, 5 Apr. 2023





Ironically, fear of NATO expansion was one of the motives Putin cited for attacking Ukraine.


Tracy Wilkinson, Los Angeles Times, 4 Apr. 2023





Opinion polls suggest Trump was broadly unpopular across the world throughout his time in office, with the Jan. 6, 2021, assault on the Capitol raising fears for the future of U.S. democracy.


Patrick Smith, NBC News, 4 Apr. 2023





The great fear when the Citizens United decision was handed down was that S&P 500 corporations would use it to seize control of American politics.


Timothy Noah, The New Republic, 3 Apr. 2023





While the reduction of school violence should mean one less thing that parents have to worry about, the fear of mass shootings is real and valid, making the absence of worry seem impossible.


Ron Avi Astor, CNN, 3 Apr. 2023





The fear now is that record walls of snow in the southern Sierra Nevada will liquefy in the intensifying spring heat into a downhill torrent that will inundate the Central Valley.


Shawn Hubler, New York Times, 2 Apr. 2023




Progressives pointed to those successful rebuttals to suggest that candidates no longer need to fear GOP attacks on crime.


Tal Axelrod, ABC News, 6 Apr. 2023





Coyotes have been found on second floor balconies, near a school bus stop, have killed pets and don’t seem to fear humans, residents have told legislators.


Sue Kiesewetter, The Enquirer, 6 Apr. 2023





All that processing may seem daunting for people who fear GMOs.


Claire Bugos, Verywell Health, 4 Apr. 2023





There’s honestly nothing to fear.


Gabe Lacques, USA TODAY, 3 Apr. 2023





In the age of the HPV vaccine, eventually no woman will need to fear cervical cancer again.


Dr. Keith Roach, oregonlive, 31 Mar. 2023





Unlike her beloved White Lotus character, Jennifer Coolidge had nothing to fear among friends at the 34th annual GLAAD Media Awards, held Thursday at the Beverly Hilton in Los Angeles.


Janine Rubenstein, Peoplemag, 31 Mar. 2023





That led investors to fear a rash of mortgage defaults, and the values of securities backed by mortgages plunged.


Rodney Ramcharan, Fortune, 30 Mar. 2023





Congress earlier this month in a bipartisan vote passed a resolution blocking D.C.’s criminal code overhaul, which Biden signed — angering local advocates for D.C. statehood and causing local Democratic officials to fear more legislation could be in jeopardy.


Meagan Flynn, Washington Post, 30 Mar. 2023



See More

These examples are programmatically compiled from various online sources to illustrate current usage of the word ‘fear.’ Any opinions expressed in the examples do not represent those of Merriam-Webster or its editors. Send us feedback about these examples.

Princeton’s WordNetRate this definition:5.0 / 3 votes

  1. fear, fearfulness, frightnoun

    an emotion experienced in anticipation of some specific pain or danger (usually accompanied by a desire to flee or fight)

  2. concern, care, fearnoun

    an anxious feeling

    «care had aged him»; «they hushed it up out of fear of public reaction»

  3. fear, reverence, awe, venerationverb

    a feeling of profound respect for someone or something

    «the fear of God»; «the Chinese reverence for the dead»; «the French treat food with gentle reverence»; «his respect for the law bordered on veneration»

  4. fearverb

    be afraid or feel anxious or apprehensive about a possible or probable situation or event

    «I fear she might get aggressive»

  5. fear, dreadverb

    be afraid or scared of; be frightened of

    «I fear the winters in Moscow»; «We should not fear the Communists!»

  6. fearverb

    be sorry; used to introduce an unpleasant statement

    «I fear I won’t make it to your wedding party»

  7. fearverb

    be uneasy or apprehensive about

    «I fear the results of the final exams»

  8. reverence, fear, revere, venerateverb

    regard with feelings of respect and reverence; consider hallowed or exalted or be in awe of

    «Fear God as your father»; «We venerate genius»

Samuel Johnson’s DictionaryRate this definition:0.0 / 0 votes

  1. FEARnoun

    Etymology: fearan, Sax. to fear; vaer, Dut. feakle, Erse.

    1. Dread; horrour; painful apprehension of danger.

    Fear is an uneasiness of the mind, upon the thought of future evil likely to befal us.
    John Locke.

    Trembling fear still to and fro did fly,
    And found no place where safe she shrowd him might.
    F. Q.

    For fear was upon them, because of the people of those countries.
    Ezra iii. 3.

    What then remains? Are we depriv’d of will?
    Must we not wish, for fear of wishing ill?
    John Dryden, Juv.

    Fear, in general, is that passion of our nature whereby we are excited to provide for our security upon the approach of evil.
    John Rogers, Sermon 1.

    2. Awe; dejection of mind at the presence of any person or thing.

    And the fear of you, and the dread of you, shall be upon every beast.
    Gen. ix. 2.

    3. Anxiety; solicitude.

    The greatest and principal fear was for the holy temple.
    2 Mac. xv. 18.

    4. That which causes fear.

    Antony, stay not by his side:
    Thy demon, that’s the spirit that keeps thee, is
    Noble, courageous, high, unmatchable,
    Where Cæsar’s is not; but near him, thy angel
    Becomes a fear, as being o’erpower’d.
    William Shakespeare, Ant. and Cleop.

    5. The object of fear.

    Except the God of Abraham and the fear of Isaac had been with me.
    Gen. xxxi. 42.

    6. Something hung up to scare deer by its colour or noise.

    He who fleeth from the noise of the fear shall fall into the pit, and he that cometh up out of the midst of the pit shall be taken in the snare.
    Is. xxiv. 18.

  2. Fearnoun

    A companion. Obsolete.

    Etymology: fearan, Sax. to fear; vaer, Dut. feakle, Erse.

    But fair Charissa to a lovely fear
    Was linked, and by him had many pledges dear.
    Fairy Qu.

  3. To Fearverb

    Etymology: fearan, Saxon.

    1. To dread; to consider with apprehensions of terrour; to be afraid of.

    Now, for my life, Hortensio fears his widow.
    —— Then never trust me if I be afraid.
    —— You are very sensible, yet you miss my sense;
    I mean Hortensio is afraid of you.
    William Shakespeare, Tam. of the Shrew.

    To fear the foe, since fear oppresseth strength,
    Gives, in your weakness, strength unto your foe.
    William Shakespeare, R. II.

    There shall rise up a kingdom, and it shall be feared above all the kingdoms before it.
    2 Esdr. xii. 13.

    When I view the beauties of thy face,
    I fear not death, nor dangers, nor disgrace.
    Dryden.

    2. To fright; to terrify; to make afraid.

    The inhabitants, being feared with the Spaniards landing and burning, fled from their dwellings.
    Carew.

    If he be taken, he shall never more
    Be fear’d of doing harm: make your own purpose
    How in my strength you please.
    William Shakespeare, King Lear.

    We must not make a scarecrow of the law,
    Setting it up to fear the birds of prey.
    William Shakespeare, Meas. for Meas.

    Some, sitting on the hatches, would seem there,
    With hideous gazing, to fear away fear.
    John Donne.

  4. To Fearverb

    1. To live in horrour; to be afraid.

    Well you may fear too far.
    —— Safer than trust too far:
    Let me still take away the harms I fear,
    Not fear still to be harm’d.
    William Shakespeare, King Lear.

    2. To be anxious.

    If any such be here, if any fear
    Less for his person than an ill report;
    If any think brave death outweighs bad life.
    William Shakespeare, Coriolan.

    Then let the greedy merchant fear
    For his ill-gotten gain;
    And pray to gods that will not hear,
    While the debating winds and billows bear
    His wealth into the main.
    John Dryden, Horace.

    See, pious king, with diff’rent strife,
    Thy struggling Albion’s bosom torn:
    So much she fears for William’s life,
    That Mary’s fate she dare not mourn.
    Matthew Prior.

WikipediaRate this definition:0.0 / 0 votes

  1. Fear

    Fear is an intensely unpleasant emotion in response to perceiving or recognizing a danger or threat. Fear causes physiological changes that may produce behavioral reactions such as mounting an aggressive response or fleeing the threat. Fear in human beings may occur in response to a certain stimulus occurring in the present, or in anticipation or expectation of a future threat perceived as a risk to oneself. The fear response arises from the perception of danger leading to confrontation with or escape from/avoiding the threat (also known as the fight-or-flight response), which in extreme cases of fear (horror and terror) can be a freeze response or paralysis.
    In humans and other animals, fear is modulated by the process of cognition and learning. Thus, fear is judged as rational or appropriate and irrational or inappropriate. An irrational fear is called a phobia.
    Fear is closely related to the emotion anxiety, which occurs as the result of threats that are perceived to be uncontrollable or unavoidable. The fear response serves survival by engendering appropriate behavioral responses, so it has been preserved throughout evolution. Sociological and organizational research also suggests that individuals’ fears are not solely dependent on their nature but are also shaped by their social relations and culture, which guide their understanding of when and how much fear to feel.Fear is sometimes incorrectly considered the opposite of courage. For the reason that courage is a willingness to face adversity, fear is an example of a condition that makes the exercise of courage possible.

Webster DictionaryRate this definition:0.0 / 0 votes

  1. Fearnoun

    a variant of Fere, a mate, a companion

  2. Fearnoun

    a painful emotion or passion excited by the expectation of evil, or the apprehension of impending danger; apprehension; anxiety; solicitude; alarm; dread

  3. Fearnoun

    apprehension of incurring, or solicitude to avoid, God’s wrath; the trembling and awful reverence felt toward the Supreme Belng

  4. Fearnoun

    respectful reverence for men of authority or worth

  5. Fearnoun

    that which causes, or which is the object of, apprehension or alarm; source or occasion of terror; danger; dreadfulness

  6. Fearnoun

    to feel a painful apprehension of; to be afraid of; to consider or expect with emotion of alarm or solicitude

  7. Fearnoun

    to have a reverential awe of; to solicitous to avoid the displeasure of

  8. Fearnoun

    to be anxious or solicitous for

  9. Fearnoun

    to suspect; to doubt

  10. Fearnoun

    to affright; to terrify; to drive away or prevent approach of by fear

  11. Fearverb

    to be in apprehension of evil; to be afraid; to feel anxiety on account of some expected evil

  12. Etymology: [OE. fer, feer, fere, AS. fr a coming suddenly upon, fear, danger; akin to D. vaar, OHG. fra danger, G. gefahr, Icel. fr harm, mischief, plague, and to E. fare, peril. See Fare.]

FreebaseRate this definition:5.0 / 1 vote

  1. Fear

    Fear is an emotion induced by a perceived threat which causes entities to quickly pull far away from it and usually hide. It is a basic survival mechanism occurring in response to a specific stimulus, such as pain or the threat of danger. In short, fear is the ability to recognize danger leading to an urge to confront it or flee from it but in extreme cases of fear a freeze or paralysis response is possible. Some psychologists such as John B. Watson, Robert Plutchik, and Paul Ekman have suggested that there is only a small set of basic or innate emotions and that fear is one of them. This hypothesized set includes such emotions as joy, sadness, fright, dread, horror, panic, anxiety, acute stress reaction and anger. Fear should be distinguished from the emotion anxiety, which typically occurs without any certain or immediate external threat.
    Fear is frequently related to the specific behaviors of escape and avoidance, whereas anxiety is the result of threats which are perceived to be uncontrollable or unavoidable. It is worth noting that fear almost always relates to future events, such as worsening of a situation, or continuation of a situation that is unacceptable. Fear can also be an instant reaction to something presently happening. All people have an instinctual response to potential danger, which is in fact important to the survival of all species. The reactions elicited from fear are seen through advantages in evolution. Fear can be a manipulating and controlling factor in an individual’s life.

Chambers 20th Century DictionaryRate this definition:0.0 / 0 votes

  1. Fear

    fēr, n. a painful emotion excited by danger: apprehension of danger or pain: alarm: the object of fear: aptness to cause fear: (B.) deep reverence: piety towards God.—v.t. to regard with fear: to expect with alarm: (B.) to stand in awe of: to venerate: (obs.) to terrify: to make afraid.—v.i. to be afraid: to be in doubt.—adj. Fear′ful, timorous: exciting intense fear: terrible.—adv. Fear′fully.—n. Fear′fulness.—adj. Fear′less, without fear: daring: brave.—adv. Fear′lessly.—ns. Fear′lessness; Fear′nought (same as Dreadnaught).—adj. Fear′some, causing fear, frightful.—adv. Fear′somely. [A.S. fǽr, fear, fǽran, to terrify; cf. Ger. gefahr, Ice. fár, harm, mischief.]

  2. Fear

    fēr, n. (Spens.) a companion. [See Fere.]

The Roycroft DictionaryRate this definition:0.0 / 0 votes

  1. fear

    1. A club used by priests, presidents, kings and policemen to keep the people from recovering stolen goods. 2. The thought of admitted inferiority. 3. The rock on which we split.

U.S. National Library of MedicineRate this definition:0.0 / 0 votes

  1. Fear

    The affective response to an actual current external danger which subsides with the elimination of the threatening condition.

Surnames Frequency by Census RecordsRate this definition:0.0 / 0 votes

  1. FEAR

    According to the U.S. Census Bureau, Fear is ranked #18502 in terms of the most common surnames in America.

    The Fear surname appeared 1,493 times in the 2010 census and if you were to sample 100,000 people in the United States, approximately 1 would have the surname Fear.

    94.5% or 1,412 total occurrences were White.
    2% or 30 total occurrences were of Hispanic origin.
    1.1% or 17 total occurrences were of two or more races.
    1% or 16 total occurrences were Black.
    0.6% or 10 total occurrences were American Indian or Alaskan Native.
    0.5% or 8 total occurrences were Asian.

Matched Categories

    • Anxiety
    • Emotion
    • Regret
    • Respect
    • Worry

British National Corpus

  1. Spoken Corpus Frequency

    Rank popularity for the word ‘fear’ in Spoken Corpus Frequency: #1594

  2. Written Corpus Frequency

    Rank popularity for the word ‘fear’ in Written Corpus Frequency: #2958

  3. Nouns Frequency

    Rank popularity for the word ‘fear’ in Nouns Frequency: #486

  4. Verbs Frequency

    Rank popularity for the word ‘fear’ in Verbs Frequency: #383

How to pronounce fear?

How to say fear in sign language?

Numerology

  1. Chaldean Numerology

    The numerical value of fear in Chaldean Numerology is: 7

  2. Pythagorean Numerology

    The numerical value of fear in Pythagorean Numerology is: 3

Examples of fear in a Sentence

  1. Lana Theis:

    The holes in our law are massive, these victims face the real possibility that their assailant would return to their same high school. I can’t imagine the pain and fear these victims and their families went through. The possibility of having to share space with the person who assaulted them is unconscionable.

  2. Sophocles:

    To him who is in fear everything rustles.

  3. Jonathan Blake:

    There was this absolute fear about what was befalling seemingly healthy gay men, who would suddenly take ill for no apparent reason and die in appalling circumstances, your head is messed up, you have this killer virus coursing through your veins, you feel like a pariah… HIV was considered a gay plague that we (gay men) had brought upon ourselves — it was horrid.

  4. John Kilduff:

    The fear of the unknown is going to be supportive to crude oil prices, king Abdullah was the architect of the current strategy to keep production high and force out smaller players instead of cutting.

  5. Mohammad Reza Bahonar:

    In the past year, the reach of sanctions has been broken and the ‘Fear Iran’ campaign has failed. So this is for our team of negotiators: good job. More power to you!

Popularity rank by frequency of use


Translation

Find a translation for the fear definition in other languages:

Select another language:

  • — Select —
  • 简体中文 (Chinese — Simplified)
  • 繁體中文 (Chinese — Traditional)
  • Español (Spanish)
  • Esperanto (Esperanto)
  • 日本語 (Japanese)
  • Português (Portuguese)
  • Deutsch (German)
  • العربية (Arabic)
  • Français (French)
  • Русский (Russian)
  • ಕನ್ನಡ (Kannada)
  • 한국어 (Korean)
  • עברית (Hebrew)
  • Gaeilge (Irish)
  • Українська (Ukrainian)
  • اردو (Urdu)
  • Magyar (Hungarian)
  • मानक हिन्दी (Hindi)
  • Indonesia (Indonesian)
  • Italiano (Italian)
  • தமிழ் (Tamil)
  • Türkçe (Turkish)
  • తెలుగు (Telugu)
  • ภาษาไทย (Thai)
  • Tiếng Việt (Vietnamese)
  • Čeština (Czech)
  • Polski (Polish)
  • Bahasa Indonesia (Indonesian)
  • Românește (Romanian)
  • Nederlands (Dutch)
  • Ελληνικά (Greek)
  • Latinum (Latin)
  • Svenska (Swedish)
  • Dansk (Danish)
  • Suomi (Finnish)
  • فارسی (Persian)
  • ייִדיש (Yiddish)
  • հայերեն (Armenian)
  • Norsk (Norwegian)
  • English (English)

Word of the Day

Would you like us to send you a FREE new word definition delivered to your inbox daily?


Citation

Use the citation below to add this definition to your bibliography:

Are we missing a good definition for fear? Don’t keep it to yourself…

Понравилась статья? Поделить с друзьями:
  • The word favorite in french
  • The word exceeded means
  • The word father in greek
  • The word example in latin
  • The word father in german