The word culture in different languages

Home

About

Blog

Contact Us

Log In

Sign Up

Follow Us

Our Apps

Home>Words that start with C>culture

How to Say Culture in Different LanguagesAdvertisement

Categories:
General

Please find below many ways to say culture in different languages. This is the translation of the word «culture» to over 100 other languages.

Saying culture in European Languages

Saying culture in Asian Languages

Saying culture in Middle-Eastern Languages

Saying culture in African Languages

Saying culture in Austronesian Languages

Saying culture in Other Foreign Languages

abcdefghijklmnopqrstuvwxyz

Saying Culture in European Languages

Language Ways to say culture
Albanian kulturë Edit
Basque kultura Edit
Belarusian культура Edit
Bosnian kultura Edit
Bulgarian култура Edit
Catalan cultura Edit
Corsican cultura Edit
Croatian Kultura Edit
Czech kultura Edit
Danish kultur Edit
Dutch cultuur Edit
Estonian kultuur Edit
Finnish kulttuuri Edit
French Culture Edit
Frisian kultuer Edit
Galician cultura Edit
German Kultur Edit
Greek Πολιτισμός
[Politismós]
Edit
Hungarian kultúra Edit
Icelandic Menning Edit
Irish cultúr Edit
Italian cultura Edit
Latvian kultūra Edit
Lithuanian kultūra Edit
Luxembourgish Kultur Edit
Macedonian култура Edit
Maltese kultura Edit
Norwegian kultur Edit
Polish kultura Edit
Portuguese cultura Edit
Romanian cultură Edit
Russian культура
[kul’tura]
Edit
Scots Gaelic cultar Edit
Serbian култура
[kultura]
Edit
Slovak kultúra Edit
Slovenian kultura Edit
Spanish cultura Edit
Swedish kultur Edit
Tatar культурасы Edit
Ukrainian культура
[kul’tura]
Edit
Welsh diwylliant Edit
Yiddish קולטור Edit

Saying Culture in Asian Languages

Language Ways to say culture
Armenian մշակույթ Edit
Azerbaijani mədəniyyət Edit
Bengali সংস্কৃতি Edit
Chinese Simplified 文化
[wénhuà]
Edit
Chinese Traditional 文化
[wénhuà]
Edit
Georgian კულტურა Edit
Gujarati સંસ્કૃતિ Edit
Hindi संस्कृति Edit
Hmong kab lis kev cai Edit
Japanese 文化 Edit
Kannada ಸಂಸ್ಕೃತಿ Edit
Kazakh мәдениет Edit
Khmer វប្បធម៍ Edit
Korean 문화
[munhwa]
Edit
Kyrgyz маданият Edit
Lao ວັດທະນະທໍາ Edit
Malayalam സംസ്കാരം Edit
Marathi संस्कृती Edit
Mongolian соёл Edit
Myanmar (Burmese) ယဉျကြေးမှု Edit
Nepali संस्कृति Edit
Odia ସଂସ୍କୃତି Edit
Pashto کلتور Edit
Punjabi ਸਭਿਆਚਾਰ Edit
Sindhi ثقافت Edit
Sinhala සංස්කෘතිය Edit
Tajik маданият Edit
Tamil கலாச்சாரம் Edit
Telugu సంస్కృతి Edit
Thai วัฒนธรรม Edit
Turkish kültür Edit
Turkmen medeniýeti Edit
Urdu ثقافت Edit
Uyghur مەدەنىيەت Edit
Uzbek madaniyat Edit
Vietnamese nền văn hóa Edit

Too many ads and languages?

Sign up to remove ads and customize your list of languages

Sign Up

Saying Culture in Middle-Eastern Languages

Language Ways to say culture
Arabic حضاره
[hidaruh]
Edit
Hebrew תַרְבּוּת Edit
Kurdish (Kurmanji) çande Edit
Persian فرهنگ Edit

Saying Culture in African Languages

Language Ways to say culture
Afrikaans kultuur Edit
Amharic ባህል Edit
Chichewa chikhalidwe Edit
Hausa al’ada Edit
Igbo omenala Edit
Kinyarwanda umuco Edit
Sesotho setso Edit
Shona tsika nemagariro Edit
Somali dhaqanka Edit
Swahili utamaduni Edit
Xhosa inkcubeko Edit
Yoruba asa Edit
Zulu isiko Edit

Saying Culture in Austronesian Languages

Language Ways to say culture
Cebuano kultura Edit
Filipino kultura Edit
Hawaiian moʻomeheu Edit
Indonesian budaya Edit
Javanese budaya Edit
Malagasy kolontsaina Edit
Malay budaya Edit
Maori ahurea Edit
Samoan aganuu Edit
Sundanese budaya Edit

Saying Culture in Other Foreign Languages

Language Ways to say culture
Esperanto kulturo Edit
Haitian Creole kilti Edit
Latin cultura Edit

Dictionary Entries near culture

  • cultural significance
  • cultural tradition
  • cultural values
  • culture
  • cultured
  • culvert
  • cum

Cite this Entry

«Culture in Different Languages.» In Different Languages, https://www.indifferentlanguages.com/words/culture. Accessed 12 Apr 2023.

Copy

Copied

Browse Words Alphabetically

Ezoicreport this ad

Culture is one of the most important creations from human beings. One of the primary ways that humans are separated from the rest of living creatures is based on the fact that we have enough organization and awareness to develop unique cultures and communities. 

Even if culture isn’t something that you think about often, it’s something that you interact with every single day. Virtually every person is in a specific social group or a particular group of people, influencing their preferences, tastes, and decisions. Knowing how to appreciate and differentiate between these cultures is both tricky and incredibly important. 

If you want to live the most fruitful life possible in the world today, learning how to integrate and interact with culture is one of the most important steps you can take. This is what culture is, where the word itself comes from, and how it works in the world today. 

What Is Culture? 

The definition of culture (ˈ k ʌ l tʃ ər) in the English dictionary is the arts, customs, achievements, and collective attitudes of a specific social group or location. While there may be similarities between cultures, each culture has its own aspects, quirks, and elements. 

One of the most common ways that cultures are created is through people who share the same ethnicity or physical location. For example, American culture will be different from African or European culture, and New York culture will be different from Chicago culture. Similarly, cultures can differ depending on personal factors, including race, age, religion, and even musical preference. 

Culture is also found inside different organizations and companies. This is typically referred to as corporate culture. At the end of the day, you will have a different set of written or unwritten rules virtually anywhere that need to be followed. These rules can dictate peoples’ attitudes toward life and the rest of the world. 

Anthropologists are the people that study the different aspects of cultures. But you don’t need to get a college degree to look into culture — just looking at the popular culture around you can help you understand what culture is and how it works. Just remember that while your own culture is unique, other cultures are just as valid as your own! 

What Is the Etymology of Culture? 

The word culture is fascinating because it comes from many words in other languages, but all of those words came from the same word. It’s the epitome of a romance language word, and it goes to show that language at large is both deeply interconnected and constantly shifting. 

The word culture began with the Latin colere, which means to tend to or cultivate something. This word was used in the context of farmers tending to their crops and farming the land. As time went on, the term shifted into the Medieval Latin cultura or cultus, which retained essentially the same meaning. 

Over the centuries, this word entered many languages in various forms, including French and English. Around the time Middle English was starting to take shape, the word cultivate was becoming more and more common regarding food and farming. However, as discussions around anthropology and psychology became more and more prominent, the word culture started to be used in its modern contexts. 

Synonyms for Culture

If you looked into a thesaurus for word lists of synonyms for the word culture, you would likely find words including: 

  • Civilization
  • Society
  • Lifestyle
  • Customs
  • Traditions
  • Heritage
  • Values
  • Habits
  • Way of life
  • Ways

How Culture Is Used Today

In the modern world, the ideas and thoughts of culture are incredibly relevant. As the world is slowly getting more and more interconnected, it’s becoming critically important to understand that different cultures are worth understanding. 

If you ever move from one location, organization, or social group to another, you will experience a culture change. It’s important to remember that that isn’t a bad thing — it’s just how the world works! Even if some aspects of life seem different at first, you can get used to them over time. 

Example Sentences Using the Word Culture

One of the best ways to learn how to use a word is by seeing it in real-world use. That’s how everyone learns how to speak and communicate, and that specific kind of learning continues throughout a person’s entire life! Here are some examples of the word culture in a sentence: 

Based on the first world fascination with material objects, most people from poorer countries think we have a material culture. 

When I changed jobs last year, I had to go through several weeks of culture shock before figuring out how things worked in my new place! 

The biology department based almost their entire culture on their fervent passion for microorganisms. 

I learned the hard way that when a meeting is set for a particular time at this new job, arriving any later is seen as disrespectful in this culture. 

The gym culture here is intensely focused on ensuring that everyone gets the right amount of nutrients every day. 

Conclusion

If you want to learn more about the English language and how it works, check out our blog here at The Word Counter! We’re constantly creating new articles and posts to help inform people about best practices surrounding complicated grammar, confusing words, and strange phrases. 

If you want to learn more about how you can make your communication as successful as possible, look at some of our latest articles and posts right here! 

Sources: 

  1. Culture Definition & Meaning | Britannica Dictionary
  2. CULTURE | Cambridge English Dictionary
  3. Culture Definition & Meaning | Dictionary.com

mm

Kevin Miller is a growth marketer with an extensive background in Search Engine Optimization, paid acquisition and email marketing. He is also an online editor and writer based out of Los Angeles, CA. He studied at Georgetown University, worked at Google and became infatuated with English Grammar and for years has been diving into the language, demystifying the do’s and don’ts for all who share the same passion! He can be found online here.

Religion and expressive art are important aspects of human culture.

Culture () is an umbrella term which encompasses the social behavior, institutions, and norms found in human societies, as well as the knowledge, beliefs, arts, laws, customs, capabilities, and habits of the individuals in these groups.[1] Culture is often originated from or attributed to a specific region or location.

Humans acquire culture through the learning processes of enculturation and socialization, which is shown by the diversity of cultures across societies.

A cultural norm codifies acceptable conduct in society; it serves as a guideline for behavior, dress, language, and demeanor in a situation, which serves as a template for expectations in a social group.
Accepting only a monoculture in a social group can bear risks, just as a single species can wither in the face of environmental change, for lack of functional responses to the change.[2]
Thus in military culture, valor is counted a typical behavior for an individual and duty, honor, and loyalty to the social group are counted as virtues or functional responses in the continuum of conflict. In the practice of religion, analogous attributes can be identified in a social group.

Cultural change, or repositioning, is the reconstruction of a cultural concept of a society.[3] Cultures are internally affected by both forces encouraging change and forces resisting change. Cultures are externally affected via contact between societies.

Organizations like UNESCO attempt to preserve culture and cultural heritage.

Description

Pygmy music has been polyphonic well before their discovery by non-African explorers of the Baka, Aka, Efe, and other foragers of the Central African forests, in the 1200s, which is at least 200 years before polyphony developed in Europe. Note the multiple lines of singers and dancers. The motifs are independent, with theme and variation interweaving.[4] This type of music is thought to be the first expression of polyphony in world music.

Culture is considered a central concept in anthropology, encompassing the range of phenomena that are transmitted through social learning in human societies. Cultural universals are found in all human societies. These include expressive forms like art, music, dance, ritual, religion, and technologies like tool usage, cooking, shelter, and clothing. The concept of material culture covers the physical expressions of culture, such as technology, architecture and art, whereas the immaterial aspects of culture such as principles of social organization (including practices of political organization and social institutions), mythology, philosophy, literature (both written and oral), and science comprise the intangible cultural heritage of a society.[5]

In the humanities, one sense of culture as an attribute of the individual has been the degree to which they have cultivated a particular level of sophistication in the arts, sciences, education, or manners. The level of cultural sophistication has also sometimes been used to distinguish civilizations from less complex societies. Such hierarchical perspectives on culture are also found in class-based distinctions between a high culture of the social elite and a low culture, popular culture, or folk culture of the lower classes, distinguished by the stratified access to cultural capital. In common parlance, culture is often used to refer specifically to the symbolic markers used by ethnic groups to distinguish themselves visibly from each other such as body modification, clothing or jewelry. Mass culture refers to the mass-produced and mass mediated forms of consumer culture that emerged in the 20th century. Some schools of philosophy, such as Marxism and critical theory, have argued that culture is often used politically as a tool of the elites to manipulate the proletariat and create a false consciousness. Such perspectives are common in the discipline of cultural studies. In the wider social sciences, the theoretical perspective of cultural materialism holds that human symbolic culture arises from the material conditions of human life, as humans create the conditions for physical survival, and that the basis of culture is found in evolved biological dispositions.

When used as a count noun, a «culture» is the set of customs, traditions, and values of a society or community, such as an ethnic group or nation. Culture is the set of knowledge acquired over time. In this sense, multiculturalism values the peaceful coexistence and mutual respect between different cultures inhabiting the same planet. Sometimes «culture» is also used to describe specific practices within a subgroup of a society, a subculture (e.g. «bro culture»), or a counterculture. Within cultural anthropology, the ideology and analytical stance of cultural relativism hold that cultures cannot easily be objectively ranked or evaluated because any evaluation is necessarily situated within the value system of a given culture.

Etymology

The modern term «culture» is based on a term used by the ancient Roman orator Cicero in his Tusculanae Disputationes, where he wrote of a cultivation of the soul or «cultura animi,»[6] using an agricultural metaphor for the development of a philosophical soul, understood teleologically as the highest possible ideal for human development. Samuel Pufendorf took over this metaphor in a modern context, meaning something similar, but no longer assuming that philosophy was man’s natural perfection. His use, and that of many writers after him, «refers to all the ways in which human beings overcome their original barbarism, and through artifice, become fully human.»[7]

In 1986, philosopher Edward S. Casey wrote, «The very word culture meant ‘place tilled’ in Middle English, and the same word goes back to Latin colere, ‘to inhabit, care for, till, worship’ and cultus, ‘A cult, especially a religious one.’ To be cultural, to have a culture, is to inhabit a place sufficiently intensely to cultivate it—to be responsible for it, to respond to it, to attend to it caringly.»[8]

Culture described by Richard Velkley:[7]

… originally meant the cultivation of the soul or mind, acquires most of its later modern meaning in the writings of the 18th-century German thinkers, who were on various levels developing Rousseau’s criticism of «modern liberalism and Enlightenment.» Thus a contrast between «culture» and «civilization» is usually implied in these authors, even when not expressed as such.

In the words of anthropologist E.B. Tylor, it is «that complex whole which includes knowledge, belief, art, morals, law, custom and any other capabilities and habits acquired by man as a member of society.»[9] Alternatively, in a contemporary variant, «Culture is defined as a social domain that emphasizes the practices, discourses and material expressions, which, over time, express the continuities and discontinuities of social meaning of a life held in common.[10]

The Cambridge English Dictionary states that culture is «the way of life, especially the general customs and beliefs, of a particular group of people at a particular time.»[11] Terror management theory posits that culture is a series of activities and worldviews that provide humans with the basis for perceiving themselves as «person[s] of worth within the world of meaning»—raising themselves above the merely physical aspects of existence, in order to deny the animal insignificance and death that Homo sapiens became aware of when they acquired a larger brain.[12][13]

The word is used in a general sense as the evolved ability to categorize and represent experiences with symbols and to act imaginatively and creatively. This ability arose with the evolution of behavioral modernity in humans around 50,000 years ago and is often thought to be unique to humans. However, some other species have demonstrated similar, though much less complicated, abilities for social learning. It is also used to denote the complex networks of practices and accumulated knowledge and ideas that are transmitted through social interaction and exist in specific human groups, or cultures, using the plural form.[citation needed]

Change

The Beatles exemplified changing cultural dynamics, not only in music, but fashion and lifestyle. Over a half century after their emergence, they continue to have a worldwide cultural impact.

Raimon Panikkar identified 29 ways in which cultural change can be brought about, including growth, development, evolution, involution, renovation, reconception, reform, innovation, revivalism, revolution, mutation, progress, diffusion, osmosis, borrowing, eclecticism, syncretism, modernization, indigenization, and transformation.[14] In this context, modernization could be viewed as adoption of Enlightenment era beliefs and practices, such as science, rationalism, industry, commerce, democracy, and the notion of progress. Rein Raud, building on the work of Umberto Eco, Pierre Bourdieu and Jeffrey C. Alexander, has proposed a model of cultural change based on claims and bids, which are judged by their cognitive adequacy and endorsed or not endorsed by the symbolic authority of the cultural community in question.[15]

Cultural invention has come to mean any innovation that is new and found to be useful to a group of people and expressed in their behavior but which does not exist as a physical object. Humanity is in a global «accelerating culture change period,» driven by the expansion of international commerce, the mass media, and above all, the human population explosion, among other factors. Culture repositioning means the reconstruction of the cultural concept of a society.[16]

Full-length profile portrait of a Turkmen woman, standing on a carpet at the entrance to a yurt, dressed in traditional clothing and jewelry

Cultures are internally affected by both forces encouraging change and forces resisting change. These forces are related to both social structures and natural events, and are involved in the perpetuation of cultural ideas and practices within current structures, which themselves are subject to change.[17]

Social conflict and the development of technologies can produce changes within a society by altering social dynamics and promoting new cultural models, and spurring or enabling generative action. These social shifts may accompany ideological shifts and other types of cultural change. For example, the U.S. feminist movement involved new practices that produced a shift in gender relations, altering both gender and economic structures. Environmental conditions may also enter as factors. For example, after tropical forests returned at the end of the last ice age, plants suitable for domestication were available, leading to the invention of agriculture, which in turn brought about many cultural innovations and shifts in social dynamics.[18]

Cultures are externally affected via contact between societies, which may also produce—or inhibit—social shifts and changes in cultural practices. War or competition over resources may impact technological development or social dynamics. Additionally, cultural ideas may transfer from one society to another, through diffusion or acculturation. In diffusion, the form of something (though not necessarily its meaning) moves from one culture to another. For example, Western restaurant chains and culinary brands sparked curiosity and fascination to the Chinese as China opened its economy to international trade in the late 20th-century.[19] «Stimulus diffusion» (the sharing of ideas) refers to an element of one culture leading to an invention or propagation in another. «Direct borrowing,» on the other hand, tends to refer to technological or tangible diffusion from one culture to another. Diffusion of innovations theory presents a research-based model of why and when individuals and cultures adopt new ideas, practices, and products.[20]

Acculturation has different meanings. Still, in this context, it refers to the replacement of traits of one culture with another, such as what happened to certain Native American tribes and many indigenous peoples across the globe during the process of colonization. Related processes on an individual level include assimilation (adoption of a different culture by an individual) and transculturation. The transnational flow of culture has played a major role in merging different cultures and sharing thoughts, ideas, and beliefs.

Early modern discourses

German Romanticism

Immanuel Kant (1724–1804) formulated an individualist definition of «enlightenment» similar to the concept of bildung: «Enlightenment is man’s emergence from his self-incurred immaturity.»[21] He argued that this immaturity comes not from a lack of understanding, but from a lack of courage to think independently. Against this intellectual cowardice, Kant urged: «Sapere Aude» («Dare to be wise!»). In reaction to Kant, German scholars such as Johann Gottfried Herder (1744–1803) argued that human creativity, which necessarily takes unpredictable and highly diverse forms, is as important as human rationality. Moreover, Herder proposed a collective form of Bildung: «For Herder, Bildung was the totality of experiences that provide a coherent identity, and sense of common destiny, to a people.»[22]

In 1795, the Prussian linguist and philosopher Wilhelm von Humboldt (1767–1835) called for an anthropology that would synthesize Kant’s and Herder’s interests. During the Romantic era, scholars in Germany, especially those concerned with nationalist movements—such as the nationalist struggle to create a «Germany» out of diverse principalities, and the nationalist struggles by ethnic minorities against the Austro-Hungarian Empire—developed a more inclusive notion of culture as «worldview» (Weltanschauung).[23] According to this school of thought, each ethnic group has a distinct worldview that is incommensurable with the worldviews of other groups. Although more inclusive than earlier views, this approach to culture still allowed for distinctions between «civilized» and «primitive» or «tribal» cultures.

In 1860, Adolf Bastian (1826–1905) argued for «the psychic unity of mankind.»[24] He proposed that a scientific comparison of all human societies would reveal that distinct worldviews consisted of the same basic elements. According to Bastian, all human societies share a set of «elementary ideas» (Elementargedanken); different cultures, or different «folk ideas» (Völkergedanken), are local modifications of the elementary ideas.[25] This view paved the way for the modern understanding of culture. Franz Boas (1858–1942) was trained in this tradition, and he brought it with him when he left Germany for the United States.[26]

English Romanticism

British poet and critic Matthew Arnold viewed «culture» as the cultivation of the humanist ideal.

In the 19th century, humanists such as English poet and essayist Matthew Arnold (1822–1888) used the word «culture» to refer to an ideal of individual human refinement, of «the best that has been thought and said in the world.»[27] This concept of culture is also comparable to the German concept of bildung: «…culture being a pursuit of our total perfection by means of getting to know, on all the matters which most concern us, the best which has been thought and said in the world.»[27]

In practice, culture referred to an elite ideal and was associated with such activities as art, classical music, and haute cuisine.[28] As these forms were associated with urban life, «culture» was identified with «civilization» (from Latin: civitas, lit.‘city’). Another facet of the Romantic movement was an interest in folklore, which led to identifying a «culture» among non-elites. This distinction is often characterized as that between high culture, namely that of the ruling social group, and low culture. In other words, the idea of «culture» that developed in Europe during the 18th and early 19th centuries reflected inequalities within European societies.[29]

British anthropologist Edward Tylor was one of the first English-speaking scholars to use the term culture in an inclusive and universal sense.

Matthew Arnold contrasted «culture» with anarchy; other Europeans, following philosophers Thomas Hobbes and Jean-Jacques Rousseau, contrasted «culture» with «the state of nature.» According to Hobbes and Rousseau, the Native Americans who were being conquered by Europeans from the 16th centuries on were living in a state of nature; this opposition was expressed through the contrast between «civilized» and «uncivilized.»[30] According to this way of thinking, one could classify some countries and nations as more civilized than others and some people as more cultured than others. This contrast led to Herbert Spencer’s theory of Social Darwinism and Lewis Henry Morgan’s theory of cultural evolution. Just as some critics have argued that the distinction between high and low cultures is an expression of the conflict between European elites and non-elites, other critics have argued that the distinction between civilized and uncivilized people is an expression of the conflict between European colonial powers and their colonial subjects.

Other 19th-century critics, following Rousseau, have accepted this differentiation between higher and lower culture, but have seen the refinement and sophistication of high culture as corrupting and unnatural developments that obscure and distort people’s essential nature. These critics considered folk music (as produced by «the folk,» i.e., rural, illiterate, peasants) to honestly express a natural way of life, while classical music seemed superficial and decadent. Equally, this view often portrayed indigenous peoples as «noble savages» living authentic and unblemished lives, uncomplicated and uncorrupted by the highly stratified capitalist systems of the West.

In 1870 the anthropologist Edward Tylor (1832–1917) applied these ideas of higher versus lower culture to propose a theory of the evolution of religion. According to this theory, religion evolves from more polytheistic to more monotheistic forms.[31] In the process, he redefined culture as a diverse set of activities characteristic of all human societies. This view paved the way for the modern understanding of religion.

Anthropology

Petroglyphs in modern-day Gobustan, Azerbaijan, dating back to 10,000 BCE and indicating a thriving culture

Although anthropologists worldwide refer to Tylor’s definition of culture,[32] in the 20th century «culture» emerged as the central and unifying concept of American anthropology, where it most commonly refers to the universal human capacity to classify and encode human experiences symbolically, and to communicate symbolically encoded experiences socially.[33] American anthropology is organized into four fields, each of which plays an important role in research on culture: biological anthropology, linguistic anthropology, cultural anthropology, and in the United States and Canada, archaeology.[34][35][36][37] The term Kulturbrille, or «culture glasses,» coined by German American anthropologist Franz Boas, refers to the «lenses» through which a person sees their own culture. Martin Lindstrom asserts that Kulturbrille, which allow a person to make sense of the culture they inhabit, «can blind us to things outsiders pick up immediately.»[38]

Sociology

An example of folkloric dancing in Colombia

The sociology of culture concerns culture as manifested in society. For sociologist Georg Simmel (1858–1918), culture referred to «the cultivation of individuals through the agency of external forms which have been objectified in the course of history.»[39] As such, culture in the sociological field can be defined as the ways of thinking, the ways of acting, and the material objects that together shape a people’s way of life. Culture can be either of two types, non-material culture or material culture.[5] Non-material culture refers to the non-physical ideas that individuals have about their culture, including values, belief systems, rules, norms, morals, language, organizations, and institutions, while material culture is the physical evidence of a culture in the objects and architecture they make or have made. The term tends to be relevant only in archeological and anthropological studies, but it specifically means all material evidence which can be attributed to culture, past or present.

Cultural sociology first emerged in Weimar Germany (1918–1933), where sociologists such as Alfred Weber used the term Kultursoziologie (‘cultural sociology’). Cultural sociology was then reinvented in the English-speaking world as a product of the cultural turn of the 1960s, which ushered in structuralist and postmodern approaches to social science. This type of cultural sociology may be loosely regarded as an approach incorporating cultural analysis and critical theory. Cultural sociologists tend to reject scientific methods, instead hermeneutically focusing on words, artifacts and symbols.[40] Culture has since become an important concept across many branches of sociology, including resolutely scientific fields like social stratification and social network analysis. As a result, there has been a recent influx of quantitative sociologists to the field. Thus, there is now a growing group of sociologists of culture who are, confusingly, not cultural sociologists. These scholars reject the abstracted postmodern aspects of cultural sociology, and instead, look for a theoretical backing in the more scientific vein of social psychology and cognitive science.[41]

Nowruz is a good sample of popular and folklore culture that is celebrated by people in more than 22 countries with different nations and religions, at the 1st day of spring. It has been celebrated by diverse communities for over 7,000 years.

Early researchers and development of cultural sociology

The sociology of culture grew from the intersection between sociology (as shaped by early theorists like Marx,[42] Durkheim, and Weber) with the growing discipline of anthropology, wherein researchers pioneered ethnographic strategies for describing and analyzing a variety of cultures around the world. Part of the legacy of the early development of the field lingers in the methods (much of cultural, sociological research is qualitative), in the theories (a variety of critical approaches to sociology are central to current research communities), and in the substantive focus of the field. For instance, relationships between popular culture, political control, and social class were early and lasting concerns in the field.

Cultural studies

In the United Kingdom, sociologists and other scholars influenced by Marxism such as Stuart Hall (1932–2014) and Raymond Williams (1921–1988) developed cultural studies. Following nineteenth-century Romantics, they identified culture with consumption goods and leisure activities (such as art, music, film, food, sports, and clothing). They saw patterns of consumption and leisure as determined by relations of production, which led them to focus on class relations and the organization of production.[43][44]

In the United Kingdom, cultural studies focuses largely on the study of popular culture; that is, on the social meanings of mass-produced consumer and leisure goods. Richard Hoggart coined the term in 1964 when he founded the Birmingham Centre for Contemporary Cultural Studies or CCCS.[45] It has since become strongly associated with Stuart Hall,[46] who succeeded Hoggart as Director.[47] Cultural studies in this sense, then, can be viewed as a limited concentration scoped on the intricacies of consumerism, which belongs to a wider culture sometimes referred to as Western civilization or globalism.

From the 1970s onward, Stuart Hall’s pioneering work, along with that of his colleagues Paul Willis, Dick Hebdige, Tony Jefferson, and Angela McRobbie, created an international intellectual movement. As the field developed, it began to combine political economy, communication, sociology, social theory, literary theory, media theory, film/video studies, cultural anthropology, philosophy, museum studies, and art history to study cultural phenomena or cultural texts. In this field researchers often concentrate on how particular phenomena relate to matters of ideology, nationality, ethnicity, social class, and/or gender.[48] Cultural studies is concerned with the meaning and practices of everyday life. These practices comprise the ways people do particular things (such as watching television or eating out) in a given culture. It also studies the meanings and uses people attribute to various objects and practices. Specifically, culture involves those meanings and practices held independently of reason. Watching television to view a public perspective on a historical event should not be thought of as culture unless referring to the medium of television itself, which may have been selected culturally; however, schoolchildren watching television after school with their friends to «fit in» certainly qualifies since there is no grounded reason for one’s participation in this practice.

In the context of cultural studies, a text includes not only written language, but also films, photographs, fashion or hairstyles: the texts of cultural studies comprise all the meaningful artifacts of culture.[49] Similarly, the discipline widens the concept of culture. Culture, for a cultural-studies researcher, not only includes traditional high culture (the culture of ruling social groups)[50] and popular culture, but also everyday meanings and practices. The last two, in fact, have become the main focus of cultural studies. A further and recent approach is comparative cultural studies, based on the disciplines of comparative literature and cultural studies.[51]

Scholars in the United Kingdom and the United States developed somewhat different versions of cultural studies after the late 1970s. The British version of cultural studies had originated in the 1950s and 1960s, mainly under the influence of Richard Hoggart, E.P. Thompson, and Raymond Williams, and later that of Stuart Hall and others at the Centre for Contemporary Cultural Studies at the University of Birmingham. This included overtly political, left-wing views, and criticisms of popular culture as «capitalist» mass culture; it absorbed some of the ideas of the Frankfurt School critique of the «culture industry» (i.e. mass culture). This emerges in the writings of early British cultural-studies scholars and their influences: see the work of (for example) Raymond Williams, Stuart Hall, Paul Willis, and Paul Gilroy.

In the United States, Lindlof and Taylor write, «cultural studies [were] grounded in a pragmatic, liberal-pluralist tradition.»[52] The American version of cultural studies initially concerned itself more with understanding the subjective and appropriative side of audience reactions to, and uses of, mass culture; for example, American cultural-studies advocates wrote about the liberatory aspects of fandom.[citation needed] The distinction between American and British strands, however, has faded.[citation needed] Some researchers, especially in early British cultural studies, apply a Marxist model to the field. This strain of thinking has some influence from the Frankfurt School, but especially from the structuralist Marxism of Louis Althusser and others. The main focus of an orthodox Marxist approach concentrates on the production of meaning. This model assumes a mass production of culture and identifies power as residing with those producing cultural artifacts. In a Marxist view, the mode and relations of production form the economic base of society, which constantly interacts and influences superstructures, such as culture.[53] Other approaches to cultural studies, such as feminist cultural studies and later American developments of the field, distance themselves from this view. They criticize the Marxist assumption of a single, dominant meaning, shared by all, for any cultural product. The non-Marxist approaches suggest that different ways of consuming cultural artifacts affect the meaning of the product. This view comes through in the book Doing Cultural Studies: The Story of the Sony Walkman (by Paul du Gay et al.),[54] which seeks to challenge the notion that those who produce commodities control the meanings that people attribute to them. Feminist cultural analyst, theorist, and art historian Griselda Pollock contributed to cultural studies from viewpoints of art history and psychoanalysis. The writer Julia Kristeva is among influential voices at the turn of the century, contributing to cultural studies from the field of art and psychoanalytical French feminism.[55]

Petrakis and Kostis (2013) divide cultural background variables into two main groups:[56]

  1. The first group covers the variables that represent the «efficiency orientation» of the societies: performance orientation, future orientation, assertiveness, power distance, and uncertainty avoidance.
  2. The second covers the variables that represent the «social orientation» of societies, i.e., the attitudes and lifestyles of their members. These variables include gender egalitarianism, institutional collectivism, in-group collectivism, and human orientation.

In 2016, a new approach to culture was suggested by Rein Raud,[15] who defines culture as the sum of resources available to human beings for making sense of their world and proposes a two-tiered approach, combining the study of texts (all reified meanings in circulation) and cultural practices (all repeatable actions that involve the production, dissemination or transmission of purposes), thus making it possible to re-link anthropological and sociological study of culture with the tradition of textual theory.

Psychology

Cognitive tools suggest a way for people from certain culture to deal with real-life problems, like Suanpan for Chinese to perform mathematical calculation.

Starting in the 1990s,[57]: 31  psychological research on culture influence began to grow and challenge the universality assumed in general psychology.[58]: 158–168 [59] Culture psychologists began to try to explore the relationship between emotions and culture, and answer whether the human mind is independent from culture. For example, people from collectivistic cultures, such as the Japanese, suppress their positive emotions more than their American counterparts.[60] Culture may affect the way that people experience and express emotions. On the other hand, some researchers try to look for differences between people’s personalities across cultures.[61][62] As different cultures dictate distinctive norms, culture shock is also studied to understand how people react when they are confronted with other cultures. Cognitive tools may not be accessible or they may function differently cross culture.[57]: 19  For example, people who are raised in a culture with an abacus are trained with distinctive reasoning style.[63] Cultural lenses may also make people view the same outcome of events differently. Westerners are more motivated by their successes than their failures, while East Asians are better motivated by the avoidance of failure.[64] Culture is important for psychologists to consider when understanding the human mental operation.

Protection of culture

There are a number of international agreements and national laws relating to the protection of culture and cultural heritage. UNESCO and its partner organizations such as Blue Shield International coordinate international protection and local implementation.[65][66]
Basically, the Hague Convention for the Protection of Cultural Property in the Event of Armed Conflict and the UNESCO Convention for the Protection of Cultural Diversity deal with the protection of culture. Article 27 of the Universal Declaration of Human Rights deals with cultural heritage in two ways: it gives people the right to participate in cultural life on the one hand and the right to the protection of their contributions to cultural life on the other.[67]

The protection of culture and cultural goods is increasingly taking up a large area nationally and internationally. Under international law, the UN and UNESCO try to set up and enforce rules for this. The aim is not to protect a person’s property, but rather to preserve the cultural heritage of humanity, especially in the event of war and armed conflict. According to Karl von Habsburg, President of Blue Shield International, the destruction of cultural assets is also part of psychological warfare. The target of the attack is the identity of the opponent, which is why symbolic cultural assets become a main target. It is also intended to affect the particularly sensitive cultural memory, the growing cultural diversity and the economic basis (such as tourism) of a state, region or municipality.[68][69][70]

Another important issue today is the impact of tourism on the various forms of culture. On the one hand, this can be physical impact on individual objects or the destruction caused by increasing environmental pollution and, on the other hand, socio-cultural effects on society.[71][72][73]

See also

  • Animal culture
  • Anthropology
  • Cultural area
  • Cultural studies
  • Cultural tourism
  • Culture 21 – United Nations plan of action
  • Honour § Cultures of honour and cultures of law
  • Outline of culture
  • Recombinant culture
  • Semiotics of culture

References

  1. ^ Tylor, Edward. (1871). Primitive Culture. Vol 1. New York: J.P. Putnam’s Son
  2. ^ Jackson, Y. Encyclopedia of Multicultural Psychology, p. 203
  3. ^ Chigbu, Uchendu Eugene (November 24, 2014). «Repositioning culture for development: women and development in a Nigerian rural community». Community, Work & Family. 18 (3): 334–350. doi:10.1080/13668803.2014.981506. ISSN 1366-8803. S2CID 144448501. Archived from the original on July 30, 2022. Retrieved May 29, 2017.
  4. ^ Michael Obert (2013) Song from the Forest
  5. ^ a b Macionis, John J; Gerber, Linda Marie (2011). Sociology. Toronto: Pearson Prentice Hall. p. 53. ISBN 978-0-13-700161-3. OCLC 652430995.
  6. ^ Cicéron, Marcus Tullius Cicero; Bouhier, Jean (1812). Tusculanes (in French). Nismes: J. Gaude. p. 273. OCLC 457735057. Archived from the original on September 2, 2018. Retrieved March 3, 2018.
  7. ^ a b Velkley, Richard L (2002). «The Tension in the Beautiful: On Culture and Civilization in Rousseau and German Philosophy». Being after Rousseau: philosophy and culture in question. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. pp. 11–30. ISBN 978-0-226-85256-0. OCLC 47930775.
  8. ^ Sorrells, Kathryn (2015). Intercultural Communication: Globalization and Social Justice. Los Angeles: Sage. ISBN 978-1-4129-2744-4.[page needed]
  9. ^ Tylor 1974, 1.
  10. ^ James, Paul; Magee, Liam; Scerri, Andy; Steger, Manfred (2015). Urban Sustainability in Theory and Practice: Circles of Sustainability. London: Routledge. p. 53. ISBN 978-1-138-02572-1. OCLC 942553107. Archived from the original on June 26, 2017. Retrieved May 29, 2017.
  11. ^ «Meaning of «culture»«. Cambridge English Dictionary. Archived from the original on August 15, 2015. Retrieved July 26, 2015.
  12. ^ Pyszczynski, Tom; Solomon, Sheldon; Greenberg, Jeff (2015). Thirty Years of Terror Management Theory. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology. Vol. 52. pp. 1–70. doi:10.1016/bs.aesp.2015.03.001. ISBN 978-0-12-802247-4.
  13. ^ Greenberg, Jeff; Koole, Sander L.; Pyszczynski, Tom (2013). Handbook of Experimental Existential Psychology. Guilford Publications. ISBN 978-1-4625-1479-3. Archived from the original on December 31, 2016. Retrieved June 1, 2016.
  14. ^ Panikkar, Raimon (1991). Pathil, Kuncheria (ed.). Religious Pluralism: An Indian Christian Perspective. ISPCK. pp. 252–99. ISBN 978-81-7214-005-2. OCLC 25410539.
  15. ^ a b Rein, Raud (August 29, 2016). Meaning in Action: Outline of an Integral Theory of Culture. Cambridge: Polity. ISBN 978-1-5095-1124-2. OCLC 944339574.
  16. ^ Chigbu, Uchendu Eugene (July 3, 2015). «Repositioning culture for development: women and development in a Nigerian rural community». Community, Work & Family. 18 (3): 334–50. doi:10.1080/13668803.2014.981506. ISSN 1366-8803. S2CID 144448501.
  17. ^ O’Neil, Dennis (2006). «Culture Change: Processes of Change». Culture Change. Palomar College. Archived from the original on October 27, 2016. Retrieved October 29, 2016.
  18. ^ Pringle, Heather (November 20, 1998). «The Slow Birth of Agriculture». Science. 282 (5393): 1446. doi:10.1126/science.282.5393.1446. ISSN 0036-8075. S2CID 128522781.
  19. ^ Wei, Clarissa (March 20, 2018). «Why China Loves American Chain Restaurants So Much». Eater. Archived from the original on March 30, 2019. Retrieved September 29, 2019.
  20. ^ Stephen Wolfram (May 16, 2017) A New Kind of Science: A 15-Year View Archived February 28, 2021, at the Wayback Machine As applied to the computational universe
  21. ^ Kant, Immanuel. 1784. «Answering the Question: What is Enlightenment?» (German: Beantwortung der Frage: Was ist Aufklärung?) Berlinische Monatsschrift, December (Berlin Monthly)
  22. ^ Eldridge, Michael. «The German Bildung Tradition». UNC Charlotte. Archived from the original on January 23, 2009. Retrieved May 29, 2017.
  23. ^ Underhill, James W. (2009). Humboldt, Worldview, and Language. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press.
  24. ^ Köpping, Klaus-Peter (2005). Adolf Bastian and the psychic unity of mankind. Lit Verlag. ISBN 978-3-8258-3989-5. OCLC 977343058.
  25. ^ Ellis, Ian. «Biography of Adolf Bastian, ethnologist». Today in Science History. Archived from the original on August 6, 2017. Retrieved May 29, 2017.
  26. ^ Liron, Tal (2003). Franz Boas and the discovery of culture (PDF) (Thesis). OCLC 52888196. Archived from the original (PDF) on January 2, 2017. Retrieved October 30, 2016.
  27. ^ a b Arnold, Matthew (1869). «Culture and Anarchy». Archived from the original on January 6, 2017. Retrieved May 29, 2017.
  28. ^ Williams (1983), p. 90. Cited in Roy, Shuker (1997). Understanding popular music. Routledge. p. 5. ISBN 978-0-415-10723-5. OCLC 245910934. argues that contemporary definitions of culture fall into three possibilities or mixture of the following three:
    • «a general process of intellectual, spiritual, and aesthetic development.»
    • «a particular way of life, whether of a people, period or a group.»
    • «the works and practices of intellectual and especially artistic activity.»

  29. ^ Bakhtin 1981, p. 4
  30. ^ Dunne, Timothy; Reus-Smit, Christian (2017). The globalization of international society. Oxford. pp. 102–121. ISBN 978-0-19-251193-5.
  31. ^ McClenon, pp. 528–29
  32. ^ Angioni, Giulio (1973). Tre saggi sull’antropologia dell’età coloniale (in Italian). OCLC 641869481.
  33. ^ Teslow, Tracy (March 10, 2016). Constructing race: the science of bodies and cultures in American anthropology. ISBN 978-1-316-60338-3. OCLC 980557304. Archived from the original on September 1, 2021. Retrieved July 10, 2021.
  34. ^ «anthropology». Lexico UK English Dictionary. Oxford University Press. Archived from the original on January 22, 2020.
  35. ^ Fernandez, James W.; Hanchett, Suzanne L.; Jeganathan, Pradeep; Nicholas, Ralph W.; Robotham, Donald Keith; Smith, Eric A. (August 31, 2015). «anthropology | Britannica.com». Britannica.com. Encyclopedia Britannica. Archived from the original on October 30, 2016. Retrieved October 30, 2016.
  36. ^ «What is Anthropology? – Advance Your Career». American Anthropological Association. Archived from the original on October 26, 2016. Retrieved October 30, 2016.
  37. ^ Haviland, William A.; McBride, Bunny; Prins, Harald E.L.; Walrath, Dana (2011). Cultural Anthropology: The Human Challenge. Wadsworth/Cengage Learning. ISBN 978-0-495-81082-7. OCLC 731048150.
  38. ^ Lindström, Martin (2016). Small data: the tiny clues that uncover huge trends. London: St. Martin’s Press. ISBN 978-1-250-08068-4. OCLC 921994909.
  39. ^ Simmel, Georg (1971). Levine, Donald N (ed.). Georg Simmel on individuality and social forms: selected writings. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. p. xix. ISBN 978-0-226-75776-6. OCLC 951272809. Archived from the original on September 12, 2017. Retrieved May 29, 2017.
  40. ^ Sokal, Alan D. (June 5, 1996). «A Physicist Experiments with Cultural Studies». Lingua Franca. Archived from the original on March 26, 2007. Retrieved October 28, 2016. Physicist Alan Sokal published a paper in a journal of cultural sociology stating that gravity was a social construct that should be examined hermeneutically. See Sokal affair for further details.
  41. ^ Griswold, Wendy (1987). «A Methodological Framework for the Sociology of Culture». Sociological Methodology. 17: 1–35. doi:10.2307/271027. ISSN 0081-1750. JSTOR 271027.
  42. ^ Berlin, Isaiah; Ryan, Alan (2002). Karl Marx: His Life and Environment. New York: Oxford University Press. p. 130. ISBN 978-0-19-510326-7. OCLC 611127754.
  43. ^ Williams, Raymond (1983), Keywords: A Vocabulary of Culture and Society, New York: Oxford University Press, pp. 87–93, 236–38, OCLC 906396817
  44. ^ Berger, John (1972). Ways of seeing. Peter Smithn. ISBN 978-0-563-12244-9. OCLC 780459348.
  45. ^ «Studying Culture – Reflections and Assessment: An Interview with Richard Hoggart». Media, Culture & Society. 13.
  46. ^ Adams, Tim (September 23, 2007). «Cultural hallmark». The Guardian. ISSN 0261-3077. Archived from the original on October 31, 2016. Retrieved October 30, 2016.
  47. ^ James, Procter (2004). Stuart Hall. Routledge. ISBN 978-0-415-26267-5. OCLC 318376213.
  48. ^ Sardar, Ziauddin; Van Loon, Borin; Appignanesi, Richard (1994). Introducing Cultural Studies. New York: Totem Books. ISBN 978-1-84046-587-7. OCLC 937991291.
  49. ^ Fiske, John; Turner, Graeme; Hodge, Robert Ian Vere (1987). Myths of Oz: reading Australian popular culture. London: Allen and Unwin. ISBN 978-0-04-330391-7. OCLC 883364628.
  50. ^ Bakhtin, Mikhail Mikhaĭlovich; Holquist, Michael (1981). The dialogic imagination four essays. Austin: University of Texas Press. p. 4. OCLC 872436352. Archived from the original on September 1, 2021. Retrieved September 1, 2021.
  51. ^ «Comparative Cultural Studies». Purdue University Press. 2015. Archived from the original on August 5, 2012. Retrieved October 30, 2016.
  52. ^ Lindlof, Thomas R; Taylor, Bryan C (2002). Qualitative Communication Research Methods (2nd ed.). Sage. p. 60. ISBN 978-0-7619-2493-7. OCLC 780825710.
  53. ^ Gonick, Cy (February 7, 2006). «Marxism». The Canadian Encyclopedia. Historica Canada. Archived from the original on October 7, 2019. Retrieved October 7, 2019.
  54. ^ du Gay, Paul, ed. (1997). Doing Cultural Studies: The Story of the Sony Walkman. Sage. ISBN 978-0-7619-5402-6. OCLC 949857570. Archived from the original on September 26, 2015. Retrieved July 2, 2015.
  55. ^ MacKenzie, Gina (August 21, 2018). «Julia Kristeva». Oxford Bibliographies. Oxford University Press. Archived from the original on July 12, 2019. Retrieved September 29, 2019.
  56. ^ Petrakis, Panagiotis; Kostis, Pantelis (December 1, 2013). «Economic growth and cultural change». The Journal of Socio-Economics. 47: 147–57. doi:10.1016/j.socec.2013.02.011.
  57. ^ a b Heine, Steven J. (2015). Cultural psychology. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews. Cognitive Science. Vol. 1 (Third ed.). New York, NY. pp. 254–266. doi:10.1002/wcs.7. ISBN 9780393263985. OCLC 911004797. PMID 26271239.
  58. ^ Myers, David G. (2010). Social psychology (Tenth ed.). New York, NY. ISBN 9780073370668. OCLC 667213323.
  59. ^ Norenzayan, Ara; Heine, Steven J. (September 2005). «Psychological universals: what are they and how can we know?». Psychological Bulletin. 131 (5): 763–784. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.131.5.763. ISSN 0033-2909. PMID 16187859.
  60. ^ Miyahara, Akira. «Toward Theorizing Japanese Communication Competence from a Non-Western Perspective». American Communication Journal. 3 (3).
  61. ^ McCrae, Robert R.; Costa, Paul T.; de Lima, Margarida Pedroso; Simões, António; Ostendorf, Fritz; Angleitner, Alois; Marušić, Iris; Bratko, Denis; Caprara, Gian Vittorio; Barbaranelli, Claudio; Chae, Joon-Ho; Piedmont, Ralph L. (1999). «Age differences in personality across the adult life span: Parallels in five cultures». Developmental Psychology. American Psychological Association (APA). 35 (2): 466–477. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.2.466. ISSN 1939-0599. PMID 10082017.
  62. ^ Cheung, F. M.; Leung, K.; Fan, R. M.; Song, W.S.; Zhang, J. X.; Zhang, H. P. (March 1996). «Development of the Chinese Personality Assessment Inventory». Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 27 (2): 181–199. doi:10.1177/0022022196272003. S2CID 145134209.
  63. ^ Baillargeon, Rene (2002), «The Acquisition of Physical Knowledge in Infancy: A Summary in Eight Lessons», Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Cognitive Development, Blackwell Publishers Ltd, pp. 47–83, doi:10.1002/9780470996652.ch3, ISBN 9780470996652
  64. ^ Heine, Steven J.; Kitayama, Shinobu; Lehman, Darrin R. (2001). «Cultural Differences in Self-Evaluation». Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology. 32 (4): 434–443. doi:10.1177/0022022101032004004. ISSN 0022-0221. S2CID 40475406.
  65. ^ Roger O’Keefe, Camille Péron, Tofig Musayev, Gianluca Ferrari «Protection of Cultural Property. Military Manual.» UNESCO, 2016, p 73.
  66. ^ UNESCO Director-General calls for stronger cooperation for heritage protection at the Blue Shield International General Assembly. UNESCO, September 13, 2017.
  67. ^ «UNESCO Legal Instruments: Second Protocol to the Hague Convention of 1954 for the Protection of Cultural Property in the Event of Armed Conflict 1999». Archived from the original on August 25, 2021. Retrieved September 1, 2021.
  68. ^ Gerold Keusch «Kulturschutz in der Ära der Identitätskriege» In: Truppendienst — Magazin des Österreichischen Bundesheeres, October 24, 2018.
  69. ^ «Karl von Habsburg auf Mission im Libanon» (in German). April 28, 2019. Archived from the original on May 26, 2020. Retrieved December 13, 2020.
  70. ^ Corine Wegener; Marjan Otter (Spring 2008), «Cultural Property at War: Protecting Heritage during Armed Conflict», The Getty Conservation Institute Newsletter, The Getty Conservation Institute, vol. 23, no. 1;
    Eden Stiffman (May 11, 2015), «Cultural Preservation in Disasters, War Zones. Presents Big Challenges», The Chronicle Of Philanthropy;
    Hans Haider (June 29, 2012), «Missbrauch von Kulturgütern ist strafbar», Wiener Zeitung
  71. ^ Shepard, Robert (August 2002). «Commodification, culture and tourism». Tourist Studies. 2 (2): 183–201. doi:10.1177/146879702761936653. S2CID 55744323.
  72. ^ Coye, N. dir. (2011), Lascaux et la conservation en milieu souterrain: actes du symposium international (Paris, 26-27 fév. 2009) = Lascaux and Preservation Issues in Subterranean Environments: Proceedings of the International Symposium (Paris, February 26 and 27), Éditions de la Maison des sciences de l’homme, 360 p.
  73. ^ Jaafar, Mastura; Rasoolimanesh, S Mostafa; Ismail, Safura (2017). «Perceived sociocultural impacts of tourism and community participation: A case study of Langkawi Island». Tourism and Hospitality Research. 17 (2): 123–134. doi:10.1177/1467358415610373. S2CID 157784805.

Further reading

Books

  • Barker, C. (2004). The Sage dictionary of cultural studies. Sage.
  • Terrence Deacon (1997). The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain. New York and London: W.W. Norton. ISBN 9780393038385.
  • Ralph L. Holloway Jr. (1969). «Culture: A Human domain». Current Anthropology. 10 (4): 395–412. doi:10.1086/201036. S2CID 144502900.
  • Dell Hymes (1969). Reinventing Anthropology.
  • James, Paul; Szeman, Imre (2010). Globalization and Culture, Vol. 3: Global-Local Consumption. London: Sage Publications.
  • Michael Tomasello (1999). «The Human Adaptation for Culture». Annual Review of Anthropology. 28: 509–29. doi:10.1146/annurev.anthro.28.1.509.
  • Whorf, Benjamin Lee (1941). «The relation of habitual thought and behavior to language». Language, Culture, and Personality: Essays in Honor of Edward Sapir.
  • Walter Taylor (1948). A Study of Archeology. Memoir 69, American Anthropological Association. Carbondale IL: Southern Illinois University Press.
  • «Adolf Bastian», Encyclopædia Britannica Online, January 27, 2009
  • Ankerl, Guy (2000) [2000]. Global communication without universal civilization, vol.1: Coexisting contemporary civilizations: Arabo-Muslim, Bharati, Chinese, and Western. INU societal research. Geneva: INU Press. ISBN 978-2-88155-004-1.
  • Arnold, Matthew. 1869. Culture and Anarchy. Archived November 18, 2017, at the Wayback Machine New York: Macmillan. Third edition, 1882, available online. Retrieved: 2006-06-28.
  • Bakhtin, M.M. (1981) The Dialogic Imagination: Four Essays. Ed. Michael Holquist. Trans. Caryl Press. ISBN 978-0-252-06445-6.
  • Barzilai, Gad. 2003. Communities and Law: Politics and Cultures of Legal Identities University of Michigan Press. ISBN 0-472-11315-1
  • Benedict, Ruth (1934). Patterns of Culture. Boston: Houghton Mifflin Company.
  • Bourdieu, Pierre. 1977. Outline of a Theory of Practice. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-29164-4
  • Michael C. Carhart, The Science of Culture in Enlightenment Germany, Cambridge, Harvard University press, 2007.
  • Cohen, Anthony P. 1985. The Symbolic Construction of Community. Routledge: New York,
  • Dawkins, R. 1982. The Extended Phenotype: The Long Reach of the Gene. Paperback ed., 1999. Oxford Paperbacks. ISBN 978-0-19-288051-2
  • Findley & Rothney. Twentieth-Century World (Houghton Mifflin, 1986)
  • Geertz, Clifford. 1973. The Interpretation of Cultures: Selected Essays. New York. ISBN 978-0-465-09719-7.
  • Geertz, Clifford (1957). «Ritual and Social Change: A Javanese Example». American Anthropologist. 59: 32–54. doi:10.1525/aa.1957.59.1.02a00040.
  • Goodall, J. 1986. The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-11649-8
  • Hoult, T.F., ed. 1969. Dictionary of Modern Sociology. Totowa, New Jersey, United States: Littlefield, Adams & Co.
  • Jary, D. and J. Jary. 1991. The HarperCollins Dictionary of Sociology. New York: HarperCollins. ISBN 0-06-271543-7
  • Keiser, R. Lincoln 1969. The Vice Lords: Warriors of the Streets. Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. ISBN 978-0-03-080361-1.
  • Kroeber, A.L. and C. Kluckhohn, 1952. Culture: A Critical Review of Concepts and Definitions. Cambridge, Massachusetts: Peabody Museum
  • Kim, Uichol (2001). «Culture, science and indigenous psychologies: An integrated analysis.» In D. Matsumoto (Ed.), Handbook of culture and psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press
  • McClenon, James. «Tylor, Edward B(urnett)». Encyclopedia of Religion and Society. Ed. William Swatos and Peter Kivisto. Walnut Creek: AltaMira, 1998. 528–29.
  • Middleton, R. 1990. Studying Popular Music. Philadelphia: Open University Press. ISBN 978-0-335-15275-9.
  • O’Neil, D. 2006. Cultural Anthropology Tutorials Archived December 4, 2004, at the Wayback Machine, Behavioral Sciences Department, Palomar College, San Marco, California. Retrieved: 2006-07-10.
  • Reagan, Ronald. «Final Radio Address to the Nation» Archived January 30, 2016, at the Wayback Machine, January 14, 1989. Retrieved June 3, 2006.
  • Reese, W.L. 1980. Dictionary of Philosophy and Religion: Eastern and Western Thought. New Jersey U.S., Sussex, U.K: Humanities Press.
  • Tylor, E.B. (1974) [1871]. Primitive culture: researches into the development of mythology, philosophy, religion, art, and custom. New York: Gordon Press. ISBN 978-0-87968-091-6.
  • UNESCO. 2002. Universal Declaration on Cultural Diversity, issued on International Mother Language Day, February 21, 2002. Retrieved: 2006-06-23.
  • White, L. 1949. The Science of Culture: A study of man and civilization. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
  • Wilson, Edward O. (1998). Consilience: The Unity of Knowledge. Vintage: New York. ISBN 978-0-679-76867-8.
  • Wolfram, Stephen. 2002 A New Kind of Science. Wolfram Media, Inc. ISBN 978-1-57955-008-0.

Articles

  • The Meaning of «Culture» (2014-12-27), Joshua Rothman, The New Yorker

External links

  • Cultura: International Journal of Philosophy of Culture and Axiology
  • What is Culture?

Culture means the patterns and characteristics of human behavior, and all that entails in terms of religion, beliefs, social norms, arts, customs, and habits

The word “culture” is used in different ways by different people.

To some, it might mean a string quartet and the use of multiple utensils at dinner. To others, it might be used in a vague way when planning a holiday overseas. If you are a scientist it means a petri dish full of microorganisms.

This is something every human experience and the way you experience it can define your life.

Culture is shared. Culture is learned, and it is not biological.

Rather, it might be said that it is developed as we seek to satisfy our biological needs. It belongs to us, to our families, our peers, our art, and institutions.

What is Culture?

Culture means the patterns and characteristics of human behavior.  Culture is one collective term of religion, beliefs, social norms, arts, customs, and habits that we possess

The interesting part is that culture, as a term, almost eludes absolute definition.

Because it is something intrinsic to our humanity, perhaps, and humans, as a rule, also elude definition. That has not stopped some of history’s brightest minds from attempting to define it, however.

Islamic Art New York Culture what is culture

Islamic Art New York Culture

The first person to use the term “culture” in the way we currently understand it was  Edward B. Tylor, an anthropologist,

He explained culture as “that complex whole which includes knowledge, belief, art, law, morals, custom, and any other capabilities and habits acquired by man as a member of society.” ( Primitive Culture, 1871).

The Famous Definitions of Culture

Geert Hofstede said

“Culture is the collective programming of the human mind that distinguishes the members of one human group from those of another. Culture in this sense is a system of collectively held values.”

Linton said

“A culture is a configuration of learned behaviors and results of behavior whose component elements are shared and transmitted by the members of a particular society”

In L.A. Samovar & R.E. Porter (Eds.), Communication Between Cultures. Belmont, CA: Wadsworth. refers

“Culture is the collective programming of the mind which distinguishes the members of one category of people from another.” – National cultures and corporate cultures.

Edgar Schein quoted

“Culture is the deeper level of basic assumptions and beliefs that are shared by members of an organization, that operate unconsciously and define in a basic ‘taken for granted’ fashion an organization’s view of its self and its environment.”

What is Culture in Anthropology?

Anthropology is the study of humanity, including prehistoric origins and contemporary human diversity. Often, it is confused with many other disciplines around humanity, history, sociology, etc., anthropology is far broader in scope

The culture of a society pervades it to its very roots.

  • Biological anthropology — the study of the biological side of human including the evolution
  • Archaeology – the study of past human cultures through their material remains.
  • Linguistic anthropology – the study of human communication, including its origins, history, and variation, and change.
  • Cultural anthropology – the study of living peoples and their cultures, including variation and change.

The fourth discipline – cultural anthropology – defines the culture to a deeper level by analyzing various two key aspects of culture

  1. Diversity – Refers to the distinctive behaviors of humans and societies
  2. Change – Refers to the evolution of these distinct behaviors and humans adapted to it.

Famous Renaissance painting The Burial of the Count of Orgaz by El Greco

The Burial of the Count of Orgaz by El Greco

Overall, cultural anthropology refers to how culture affects the way people live, the way they interact, the art they make, the jobs they hold, their beliefs, and relationships.

And yet an archaeologist digging up an ancient site, finding wall stubs and pottery fragments, could never say that they have dug up “culture”.

The results of the culture are there for all to see: the patterns on the pottery, the places of worship, the way a family home was set up. But they are remains, nothing more.

Culture belongs to life itself.

The relation between culture and society

It can be a little difficult to draw the lines between culture and society. Both involve the way we live, both involve beliefs and systems, both are formed by groups of people.

Virgin of the Rocks Painting by Leonardo da Vinci.

Virgin of the Rocks Painting by Leonardo da Vinci.

A society is a group of organisms that interact with one another. This might mean a school of fish, a flock of birds, a beehive, and so on. Human societies are similar, as they are groups of individuals who interact with one another, though not always directly. In human societies, however, the behavior of the group is not just determined by survival, but by history, tradition, and expectation.

Yet people living in a single society can have different cultures. So society and culture are not the same things – but they are linked.

If culture is a pattern of people’s behavior, and if people live in societies, then, of course, they are going to be tied together at multiple points.

And culture cannot exist without society, without people coming together and exchanging ideas and experiences. Without groups of people living together, why would we ever have needed to develop language or politics? You cannot have one without the other.

Culture is all about Learned Behaviors

Culture is not something that we are born knowing. No baby is born being able to understand art, or speaking the language of its parents. Yet what it does possess is a desire to communicate and be understood – a desire it generally seeks to fill by screaming, which works out just fine, to begin with. But then, it learns that different noises mean different things, and so language begins to be learned.

egyptian art depicted by Rosetta Stone

Rosetta Stone

Because of this, culture is also something that accumulates. It is built on overtime.

It’s not as though a group of people in 1000BC sat down and discussed whether they were going to use forks or chopsticks, or whether they were going to teach math in school. These things developed slowly – and now, millennia later, schoolchildren are learning mathematical concepts developed by ancient Greeks.

Art and Culture – A soulful connection

Art is yet another concept that is very difficult to define.

Abby Willowroot  – “Art speaks the soul of its culture”

But when it comes to a shared understanding of art within a group of people, one could say that art is the physical manifestation of the culture to which it belongs – to the point that sometimes it almost seems impossible to separate the culture from its art.

Indian Paintings

Indian Paintings

If you pass a wedding venue and see it crowded with paper swans, it doesn’t matter if you are in Texas, Perth or Abu Dhabi, you will immediately recognize the sight as belonging to the culture of Japan.

Geometric patterns with bright colors and striking contrast might bring to mind traditional Kenyan textiles, even if seen in a window in Prague.

“Scandinavian interior design” could be found in a desert.

In addition to this, there is a reason great art movements tend to find their momentum in cities.

That’s where you can find the most people, packed in closely together – and, as a result, that’s where the cultures to which they belong become the richest, the densest, the most likely to turn into something new.

And sometimes, finding themselves so close to other cultures, they find themselves rubbing together and creating sparks.

An Adaptive Mechanism

When we look at the human experience in all its needs and forms, culture can sometimes seem like something of an extra.

True, humans create art, and language, and politics.

But these things, while adding to the richness, complexity, or possibilities of our lives, do not seem to be necessary for survival.

After all, a person could live in a hut on a hill for their entire lives and never see another human being.

They might never learn a language, create art, or develop an understanding of authority; as long as they can hunt and gather, they will do just fine.

And yet, if you look at cultures across the world, there seem to be very obvious differences between them that have sprung from a need to adapt.

For example, humans are warm-blooded creatures, which was fine when we were all living in subtropical conditions a few million years ago, but when you look further afield and forward in time, you see the mechanisms humans have put in place to survive the environments they moved to.

Thus we have architecture and communal planning.

Unlike other organisms, we did not wait for evolutionary adaptation to allow us to thrive in these new climates. Instead, we invented things to help us – things which became a part of the cultures which developed them.

From the clothes we wear to the food we eat, to the shape of our roofs, we can see how each culture was affected by humanity’s need for survival.

And, let’s be honest, it worked – we have dominated the planet with our technology and subsequent population growth. (Whether that is a good thing or not is quite another matter.)

Culture’s relation to Nature

Depending on the way we have defined culture, it can be argued that humans are not the only species to have developed it.

Not that we’re going to find any other animals that create paper cranes for their weddings, but using the broad and relatively simplistic definition of a complex pattern of learned behavior, we can see examples of culture in other species.

Chimpanzees, along with other intelligent primates, seem to be the closest contenders for this.

The young chimpanzees learn from the older ones – whether hunting or gathering skills, communication, or sexual education.

This is a fascinating addition to any discussions one might have regarding culture.

It opens up the possibility that culture is not strictly something that belongs to humans, but perhaps that it is the skill we have developed above all other animals.

We can be outrun, out-swam or out-fought by any number of other species. But our patterns of behavior, in terms of complexity and possibility, leave them all behind.

Culture, from a historical perspective..

The following extract from Kevin Avruch, famous anthropologist and sociologist

A great deal of the problem [of understanding the idea of culture] is caused by the different usages of the word as it was increasingly used in the nineteenth century. Broadly talking, it had been found in three ways (most of that can be found nowadays at the same time). Initial, as stated in Matthew Arnolds’ Culture and Anarchy (1867), cultures are known as special intellectual or imaginative endeavors or items, what right now we might get in touch with “high culture” in contrast to “popular culture” (or “folkways”).

From this classification, only a portion – typically a small one – associated with a sociable team “has” culture. (The rest are possible resources for anarchy!) This sensation of traditions is a lot more closely linked to beauty rather than to interpersonal science.

To some extent in the reaction to this utilization, another, as pioneered by Edward Tylor in Primitive Culture (1870), described a quality possessed by everybody in most social groupings, who nevertheless may be arrayed over an improvement (evolutionary) continuum (in Lewis Henry Morgan’s plan) from “savagery” through “barbarism” to “civilization”.

It is actually really worth quoting Tylor’s definition in their entirety initial, mainly because it became the foundational one for anthropology and 2nd because it in part explains why Kroeber and Kluckhohn located definitional fecundity from the early 1950s. Tylor’s meaning of traditions is “that intricate whole which includes understanding, notion, artwork, morals, regulation, custom, and any other features and practices obtained by a person as part of society”.

As opposed to Arnold’s perspective, all people “have” customs, they will obtain by virtue of account in some social team – culture. And a total grab case of stuff, from knowledge to behavior to features, tends to make up customs. The extreme inclusivity of Tylor’s description stayed with anthropology a very long time it can be one particular reason politics experts who became interested in social queries from the late 1950s experienced it needed to delimit their relevant social domain to “political culture”.

Although the best legacy of Tylor’s definition lay down within his “complex whole” formulation. This was recognized even by those later anthropologists who forcefully denied his evolutionism. They had taken it to mean that cultures were wholes – integrated systems. Even if this assertion has fantastic heuristic importance, in addition, it, since we shall disagree below, simplifies the entire world substantially. The third and last using traditions created in anthropology inside the twentieth-century work of Franz Boas and his awesome college students, although with roots within the eighteenth-century articles of Johann von Herder.

As Tylor reacted to Arnold to establish a technological (as opposed to visual) grounds for customs, so Boas reacted against Tylor and other interpersonal evolutionists. Whereas the evolutionists stressed the widespread personality of your single culture, with assorted societies arrayed from savage to civilized, Boas emphasized the individuality of the many and diverse ethnicities of several people or communities. Additionally, he dismissed the worth judgments he found inherent in both the Arnoldian and Tylorean sights of the tradition for Boas, you need to never separate higher from lower traditions, and another ought not differentially valorize civilizations as savage or civilized. Here, then, are three totally different understandings of tradition.

A portion of the difficulty within the expression depends on its number of connotations. But to compound concerns, the difficulties usually are not merely conceptual or semantic. Every one of the usages and understandings come linked to, or Primary Principles 2 Precisely what is Customs? | © Spencer-Oatey 2012 might be connected to, distinct politics or ideological agendas that, in a single type or some other, still resonate these days.

Conclusion – What is Culture?

Culture is inherent.

Culture is developed as we seek to fill our basic needs.

It is learned, taught from one generation to the next, picked up when you had no idea that you were paying attention.

Culture is cumulative, ideas, and behaviors collected by each society.  Yes, like they were debris being picked up and carried along by a river.

It is not programmed, it is not automatic, but it is not something that we can avoid becoming part of.

The beliefs and social behaviors are ingrained into every human on earth. These social norms are connecting us to each other within our own culture.

And cross-culturally, these norms are allowing us to reach each other across what sometimes seems to be unfathomable distances.

Culture is everywhere – It’s is in art, music, dance, the way we decorate our pottery.

It is our governmental systems, it is our leisure time, it is the places of worship we build.

Culture is the way we speak to one another, whether we take our shoes off before we come into the house.

It is shared behavior; the result of humanity trying to negotiate the world it finds itself in and thriving as it does.

Petroglyphs in modern-day Gobustan, Azerbaijan, dating back to 10 000 BCE indicating a thriving culture

Culture (Latin: cultura, lit. «cultivation»)[1] is a term that has many different inter-related meanings. For example, in 1952, Alfred Kroeber and Clyde Kluckhohn compiled a list of 164 definitions of «culture» in Culture: A Critical Review of Concepts and Definitions.[2] However, the word «culture» is most commonly used in three basic senses:

  • Excellence of taste in the fine arts and humanities, also known as high culture
  • An integrated pattern of human knowledge, belief, and behavior that depends upon the capacity for symbolic thought and social learning
  • The set of shared attitudes, values, goals, and practices that characterizes an institution, organization, or group

When the concept first emerged in eighteenth- and nineteenth-century Europe, it connoted a process of cultivation or improvement, as in agriculture or horticulture. In the nineteenth century, it came to refer first to the betterment or refinement of the individual, especially through education, and then to the fulfillment of national aspirations or ideals. In the mid-nineteenth century, some scientists used the term «culture» to refer to a universal human capacity. For the German nonpositivist sociologist Georg Simmel, culture referred to «the cultivation of individuals through the agency of external forms which have been objectified in the course of history».[3]

In the twentieth century, «culture» emerged as a concept central to anthropology, encompassing all human phenomena that are not purely results of human genetics. Specifically, the term «culture» in American anthropology had two meanings: (1) the evolved human capacity to classify and represent experiences with symbols, and to act imaginatively and creatively; and (2) the distinct ways that people living in different parts of the world classified and represented their experiences, and acted creatively. Following World War II, the term became important, albeit with different meanings, in other disciplines such as cultural studies, organizational psychology and management studies.[citation needed]

Contents

  • 1 Etymology
  • 2 Early modern discourses
    • 2.1 German Romanticism
    • 2.2 English Romanticism
  • 3 20th century discourses
    • 3.1 American anthropology
      • 3.1.1 Biological anthropology: the evolution of culture
      • 3.1.2 Archeological approaches to culture: matter and meaning
      • 3.1.3 Language and culture
      • 3.1.4 Cultural anthropology
        • 3.1.4.1 1899–1946: Universal versus particular
        • 3.1.4.2 Structural-Functionalist challenge: Society versus culture
        • 3.1.4.3 1946–1968: Symbolic versus adaptive
        • 3.1.4.4 1940–present: Local versus global
    • 3.2 Cultural studies
  • 4 Cultural change
  • 5 See also
  • 6 Notes
  • 7 References
  • 8 External links

Etymology

The etymology of the modern term «culture» has a classical origin. In English, the word «culture» is based on a term used by Cicero, in his Tusculan Disputations, wrote of a cultivation of the soul or «cultura animi», thereby using an agricultural metaphor to describe the development of a philosophical soul, which was understood teleologically as the one natural highest possible ideal for human development. Samuel Pufendorf took over this metaphor in a modern context, meaning something similar, but no longer assuming that philosophy is man’s natural perfection. His use, and that of many writers after him «refers to all the ways in which human beings overcome their original barbarism, and through artifice, become fully human».[4]

As described by Velkley[4]:

The «term «culture,» which originally meant the cultivation of the soul or mind, acquires most of its later modern meanings in the writings of the eighteenth-century German thinkers, who on various levels developing Rousseau’s criticism of modern liberalism and Enlightenment. Thus a contrast between «culture» and «civilization» is usually implied in these authors, even when not expressed as such. Two primary meanings of culture emerge from this period: culture as the folk-spirit having a unique identity, and culture as cultivation of inwardness or free individuality. The first meaning is predominant in our current use of the term «culture,» although the second still plays a large role in what we think culture should achieve, namely the full «expression» of the unique of «authentic» self.

Early modern discourses

German Romanticism

Johann Herder called attention to national cultures.

The German philosopher Immanuel Kant (1724–1804) formulated an individualist definition of «enlightenment» similar to the concept of bildung: «Enlightenment is man’s emergence from his self-incurred immaturity.»[5] He argued that this immaturity comes not from a lack of understanding, but from a lack of courage to think independently. Against this intellectual cowardice, Kant urged: Sapere aude, «Dare to be wise!» In reaction to Kant, German scholars such as Johann Gottfried Herder (1744–1803) argued that human creativity, which necessarily takes unpredictable and highly diverse forms, is as important as human rationality. Moreover, Herder proposed a collective form of bildung: «For Herder, Bildung was the totality of experiences that provide a coherent identity, and sense of common destiny, to a people.»[6]

In 1795, the great linguist and philosopher Wilhelm von Humboldt (1767–1835) called for an anthropology that would synthesize Kant’s and Herder’s interests. During the Romantic era, scholars in Germany, especially those concerned with nationalist movements—such as the nationalist struggle to create a «Germany» out of diverse principalities, and the nationalist struggles by ethnic minorities against the Austro-Hungarian Empire—developed a more inclusive notion of culture as «worldview.» According to this school of thought, each ethnic group has a distinct worldview that is incommensurable with the worldviews of other groups. Although more inclusive than earlier views, this approach to culture still allowed for distinctions between «civilized» and «primitive» or «tribal» cultures.

In 1860, Adolf Bastian (1826–1905) argued for «the psychic unity of mankind». He proposed that a scientific comparison of all human societies would reveal that distinct worldviews consisted of the same basic elements. According to Bastian, all human societies share a set of «elementary ideas» (Elementargedanken); different cultures, or different «folk ideas» (Völkergedanken), are local modifications of the elementary ideas.[7] This view paved the way for the modern understanding of culture. Franz Boas (1858–1942) was trained in this tradition, and he brought it with him when he left Germany for the United States.

English Romanticism

British poet and critic Matthew Arnold viewed «culture» as the cultivation of the humanist ideal.

British anthropologist Edward Tylor was one of the first English-speaking scholars to use the term culture in an inclusive and universal sense.

In the nineteenth century, humanists such as English poet and essayist Matthew Arnold (1822–1888) used the word «culture» to refer to an ideal of individual human refinement, of «the best that has been thought and said in the world.»[8] This concept of culture is comparable to the German concept of bildung: «…culture being a pursuit of our total perfection by means of getting to know, on all the matters which most concern us, the best which has been thought and said in the world.»[8]

In practice, culture referred to an élite ideal and was associated with such activities as art, classical music, and haute cuisine.[9] As these forms were associated with urban life, «culture» was identified with «civilization» (from lat. civitas, city). Another facet of the Romantic movement was an interest in folklore, which led to identifying a «culture» among non-elites. This distinction is often characterized as that between «high culture», namely that of the ruling social group, and «low culture.» In other words, the idea of «culture» that developed in Europe during the 18th and early 19th centuries reflected inequalities within European societies.[10]

Matthew Arnold contrasted «culture» with «anarchy;» other Europeans, following philosophers Thomas Hobbes and Jean-Jacques Rousseau, contrasted «culture» with «the state of nature». According to Hobbes and Rousseau, the Native Americans who were being conquered by Europeans from the 16th centuries on were living in a state of nature; this opposition was expressed through the contrast between «civilized» and «uncivilized.» According to this way of thinking, one could classify some countries and nations as more civilized than others and some people as more cultured than others. This contrast led to Herbert Spencer’s theory of Social Darwinism and Lewis Henry Morgan’s theory of cultural evolution. Just as some critics have argued that the distinction between high and low cultures is really an expression of the conflict between European elites and non-elites, some critics have argued that the distinction between civilized and uncivilized people is really an expression of the conflict between European colonial powers and their colonial subjects.

Other 19th century critics, following Rousseau, have accepted this differentiation between higher and lower culture, but have seen the refinement and sophistication of high culture as corrupting and unnatural developments that obscure and distort people’s essential nature. These critics considered folk music (as produced by working-class people) to honestly express a natural way of life, while classical music seemed superficial and decadent. Equally, this view often portrayed indigenous peoples as «noble savages» living authentic and unblemished lives, uncomplicated and uncorrupted by the highly stratified capitalist systems of the West.

In 1870 Edward Tylor (1832–1917) applied these ideas of higher versus lower culture to propose a theory of the evolution of religion. According to this theory, religion evolves from more polytheistic to more monotheistic forms.[11] In the process, he redefined culture as a diverse set of activities characteristic of all human societies. This view paved the way for the modern understanding of culture.

20th century discourses

American anthropology

Although anthropologists worldwide refer to Tylor’s definition of culture, in the 20th century «culture» emerged as the central and unifying concept of American anthropology, where it most commonly refers to the universal human capacity to classify and encode their experiences symbolically, and communicate symbolically encoded experiences socially. American anthropology is organized into four fields, each of which plays an important role in research on culture: biological anthropology, linguistics, cultural anthropology and archaeology. Research in these fields have influenced anthropologists working in other countries to different degrees.

Biological anthropology: the evolution of culture

Discussion concerning culture among biological anthropologists centers around two debates. First, is culture uniquely human or shared by other species (most notably, other primates)? This is an important question, as the theory of evolution holds that humans are descended from (now extinct) non-human primates. Second, how did culture evolve among human beings?

Gerald Weiss noted that although Tylor’s classic definition of culture was restricted to humans, many anthropologists take this for granted and thus elide that important qualification from later definitions, merely equating culture with any learned behavior. This slippage is a problem because during the formative years of modern primatology, some primatologists were trained in anthropology (and understood that culture refers to learned behavior among humans), and others were not. Notable non-anthropologists, like Robert Yerkes and Jane Goodall thus argued that since chimpanzees have learned behaviors, they have culture.[12][13] Today, anthropological primatologists are divided, several arguing that non-human primates have culture, others arguing that they do not.[14][15][16][17]

This scientific debate is complicated by ethical concerns. The subjects of primatology are non-human primates, and whatever culture these primates have is threatened by human activity. After reviewing the research on primate culture, W.C. McGrew concluded, «[a] discipline requires subjects, and most species of nonhuman primates are endangered by their human cousins. Ultimately, whatever its merit, cultural primatology must be committed to cultural survival [i.e. to the survival of primate cultures].»[18]

McGrew suggests a definition of culture that he finds scientifically useful for studying primate culture. He points out that scientists do not have access to the subjective thoughts or knowledge of non-human primates. Thus, if culture is defined in terms of knowledge, then scientists are severely limited in their attempts to study primate culture. Instead of defining culture as a kind of knowledge, McGrew suggests that we view culture as a process. He lists six steps in the process:

  1. A new pattern of behavior is invented, or an existing one is modified.
  2. The innovator transmits this pattern to another.
  3. The form of the pattern is consistent within and across performers, perhaps even in terms of recognizable stylistic features.
  4. The one who acquires the pattern retains the ability to perform it long after having acquired it.
  5. The pattern spreads across social units in a population. These social units may be families, clans, troops, or bands.
  6. The pattern endures across generations.[18]

McGrew admits that all six criteria may be strict, given the difficulties in observing primate behavior in the wild. But he also insists on the need to be as inclusive as possible, on the need for a definition of culture that «casts the net widely»:

Culture is considered to be group-specific behavior that is acquired, at least in part, from social influences. Here, group is considered to be the species-typical unit, whether it be a troop, lineage, subgroup, or so on. Prima facie evidence of culture comes from within-species but across-group variation in behavior, as when a pattern is persistent in one community of chimpanzees but is absent from another, or when different communities perform different versions of the same pattern. The suggestion of culture in action is stronger when the difference across the groups cannot be explained solely by ecological factors ….[19]

As Charles Frederick Voegelin pointed out, if «culture» is reduced to «learned behavior,» then all animals have culture.[20] Certainly all specialists agree that all primate species evidence common cognitive skills: knowledge of object-permanence, cognitive mapping, the ability to categorize objects, and creative problem solving.[21] Moreover, all primate species show evidence of shared social skills: they recognize members of their social group; they form direct relationships based on degrees of kinship and rank; they recognize third-party social relationships; they predict future behavior; and they cooperate in problem-solving.[21]

One current view of the temporal and geographical distribution of hominid populations

Nevertheless, the term «culture» applies to non-human animals only if we define culture as any or all learned behavior. Within mainstream physical anthropology, scholars tend to think that a more restrictive definition is necessary. These researchers are concerned with how human beings evolved to be different from other species. A more precise definition of culture, which excludes non-human social behavior, would allow physical anthropologists to study how humans evolved their unique capacity for «culture».

Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes and Pan paniscus) are humans’ (Homo sapiens) closest living relative; both are descended from a common ancestor which lived around five or six million years ago. This is the same amount of time it took for horses and zebras, lions and tigers to diverge from their respective common ancestors.[22] The evolution of modern humans is rapid: Australopithicenes evolved four million years ago and modern humans in past several hundred thousand years.[23] During this time humanity evolved three distinctive features:

(a) the creation and use of conventional symbols, including linguistic symbols and their derivatives, such as written language and mathematical symbols and notations; (b) the creation and use of complex tools and other instrumental technologies; and (c) the creation and participation in complex social organization and institutions.[24]

According to developmental psychologist Michael Tomasello, «where these complex and species-unique behavioral practices, and the cognitive skills that underlie them, came from» is a fundamental anthropological question. Given that contemporary humans and chimpanzees are far more different than horses and zebras, or rats and mice, and that the evolution of this great difference occurred in such a short period of time, «our search must be for some small difference that made a big difference – some adaptation, or small set of adaptations, that changed the process of primate cognitive evolution in fundamental ways.» According to Tomasello, the answer to this question must form the basis of a scientific definition of «human culture.»[24]

In a recent review of the major research on human and primate tool-use, communication, and learning strategies, Tomasello argues that the key human advances over primates (language, complex technologies, complex social organization) are all the results of humans pooling cognitive resources. This is called «the ratchet effect:» innovations spread and are shared by a group, and mastered «by youngsters, which enables them to remain in their new and improved form within the group until something better comes along.» The key point is that children are born good at a particular kind of social learning; this creates a favored environment for social innovations, making them more likely to be maintained and transmitted to new generations than individual innovations.[25] For Tomasello, human social learning—the kind of learning that distinguishes humans from other primates and that played a decisive role in human evolution—is based on two elements: first, what he calls «imitative learning,» (as opposed to «emulative learning» characteristic of other primates) and second, the fact that humans represent their experiences symbolically (rather than iconically, as is characteristic of other primates). Together, these elements enable humans to be both inventive, and to preserve useful inventions. It is this combination that produces the ratchet effect.

Chimpanzee extracting insects

The Japanese Macaques at Jigokudani hotspring in Nagano

The kind of learning found among other primates is «emulation learning,» which «focuses on the environmental events involved – results or changes of state in the environment that the other produced – rather than on the actions that produced those results.»[26][27][28] Tomasello emphasizes that emulation learning is a highly adaptive strategy for apes because it focuses on the effects of an act. In laboratory experiments, chimpanzees were shown two different ways for using a rake-like tool to obtain an out-of-reach-object. Both methods were effective, but one was more efficient than the other. Chimpanzees consistently emulated the more efficient method.[29]

Examples of emulation learning are well documented among primates. Notable examples include Japanese macaque potato washing, Chimpanzee tool use, and Chimpanzee gestural communication. In 1953, an 18-month-old female macaque monkey was observed taking sandy pieces of sweet potato (given to the monkeys by observers) to a stream (and later, to the ocean) to wash off the sand. After three months, the same behavior was observed in her mother and two playmates, and then the playmates’ mothers. Over the next two years seven other young macaques were observed washing their potatoes, and by the end of the third year 40% of the troop had adopted the practice.[30][31] Although this story is popularly represented as a straightforward example of human-like learning, evidence suggests that it is not. Many monkeys naturally brush sand off of food; this behavior had been observed in the macaque troop prior to the first observed washing. Moreover, potato washing was observed in four other separate macaque troops, suggesting that at least four other individual monkeys had learned to wash off sand on their own.[31] Other monkey species in captivity quickly learn to wash off their food.[32] Finally, the spread of learning among the Japanese macaques was fairly slow, and the rate at which new members of the troop learned did not keep pace with the growth of the troop. If the form of learning were imitation, the rate of learning should have been exponential. It is more likely that the monkeys’ washing behavior is based on the common behavior of cleaning off food, and that monkeys that spent time by the water independently learned to wash, rather than wipe their food. This explains both why those monkeys that kept company with the original washer, and who thus spent a good deal of time by the water, also figured out how to wash their potatoes. It also explains why the rate at which this behavior spread was slow.[33]

Chimpanzees exhibit a variety of population-specific tool use: termite-fishing, ant-fishing, ant-dipping, nut-cracking, and leaf-sponging. Gombe chimpanzees fish for termites using small, thin sticks, but chimpanzees in Western Africa use large sticks to break holes in mounds and use their hands to scoop up termites. Some of this variation may be the result of «environmental shaping» (there is more rainfall in western Africa, softening termite mounds and making them easier to break apart, than in the Gombe reserve in eastern Africa. Nevertheless it is clear that chimpanzees are good at emulation learning. Chimpanzee children independently know how to roll over logs, and know how to eat insects. When children see their mothers rolling over logs to eat the insects beneath, they quickly learn to do the same. In other words, this form of learning builds on activities the children already know.[27][34]

The kind of learning characteristic of human children is «Imitative learning,» which «means reproducing an instrumental act understood intentionally.»[35] Human infants begin to display some evidence of this form of learning between the ages of nine and twelve months, when infants fix their attention not only on an object, but on the gaze of an adult which enables them to use adults as points of reference and thus «act on objects in the way adults are acting on them.» [36] This dynamic is well documented and has also been termed «joint engagement» or «joint attention.»[37][38] Essential to this dynamic is the infant’s growing capacity to recognize others as «intentional agents:» people «with the power to control their spontaneous behavior» and who «have goals and make active choices among behavioral means for attaining those goals.»[39]

The development of skills in joint attention by the end of a human child’s first year of life provides the basis for the development of imitative learning in the second year. In one study 14-month old children imitated an adult’s over-complex method of turning on a light, even when they could have used an easier and more natural motion to the same effect.[40] In another study, 16-month old children interacted with adults who alternated between a complex series of motions that appeared intentional and a comparable set of motions that appeared accidental; they imitated only those motions that appeared intentional.[41] Another study of 18-month old children revealed that children imitate actions that adults intend, yet in some way fail, to perform.[42] Tomasello emphasizes that this kind of imitative learning «relies fundamentally on infants’ tendency to identify with adults, and on their ability to distinguish in the actions of others the underlying goal and the different means that might be used to achieve it.»[43] He calls this kind of imitative learning «cultural learning because the child is not just learning about things from other persons, she is also learning things through them — in the sense that she must know something of the adult’s perspective on a situation to learn the active use of this same intentional act.»[44][45] He concludes that the key feature of cultural learning is that it occurs only when an individual «understands others as intentional agents, like the self, who have a perspective on the world that can be followed into, directed and shared.»[46]

Emulation learning and imitative learning are two different adaptations that can only be assessed in their larger environmental and evolutionary contexts. In one experiment, chimpanzees and two-year-old children were separately presented with a rake-like-tool and an out-of-reach object. Adult humans then demonstrated two different ways to use the tool, one more efficient, one less efficient. Chimpanzees used the same efficient method following both demonstrations. Most of the human children, however, imitated whichever method the adult was demonstrating. Were chimps and humans to be compared on the basis of these results, one might think that Chimpanzees are more intelligent. From an evolutionary perspective they are equally intelligent, but with different kinds of intelligence adapted to different environments.[29] Chimpanzee learning strategies are well-suited to a stable physical environment that requires little social cooperation (compared to humans). Human learning strategies are well-suited to a complex social environment in which understanding the intentions of others may be more important than success at a specific task. Tomasello argues that this strategy has made possible the «ratchet effect» that enabled humans to evolve complex social systems that have enabled humans to adapt to virtually every physical environment on the surface of the earth.[47]

Tomasello further argues that cultural learning is essential for language-acquisition. Most children in any society, and all children in some, do not learn all words through the direct efforts of adults. «In general, for the vast majority of words in their language, children must find a way to learn in the ongoing flow of social interaction, sometimes from speech not even addressed to them.»[48] This finding has been confirmed by a variety of experiments in which children learned words even when the referent was not present, multiple referents were possible, and the adult was not directly trying to teach the word to the child.[49][50][51] Tomasello concludes that «a linguistic symbol is nothing other than a marker for an intersubjectively shared understanding of a situation.»[52]

Tomasello’s 1999 review of the research contrasting human and non-human primate learning strategies confirms biological anthropologist Ralph Holloway’s 1969 argument that a specific kind of sociality linked to symbolic cognition were the keys to human evolution, and constitute the nature of culture. According to Holloway, the key issue in the evolution of H. sapiens, and the key to understanding «culture,» «is how man organizes his experience.» Culture is «the imposition of arbitrary form upon the environment.«[53] This fact, Holloway argued, is primary to and explains what is distinctive about human learning strategies, tool-use, and language. Human tool-making and language express «similar, if not identical, cognitive processes» and provide important evidence for how humankind evolved.[54]

In other words, whereas McGrew argues that anthropologists must focus on behaviors like communication and tool-use because they have no access to the mind, Holloway argues that human language and tool-use, including the earliest stone tools in the fossil record, are highly suggestive of cognitive differences between humans and non-humans, and that such cognitive differences in turn explain human evolution. For Holloway, the question is not whether other primates communicate, learn or make tools, but the way they do these things. «Washing potatoes in the ocean … stripping branches of leaves to get termites,» and other examples of primate tool-use and learning «are iconic, and there is no feedback from the environment to the animal .»[55] Human tools, however, express an independence from natural form that manifests symbolic thinking. «In the preparation of the stick for termite-eating, the relation between product and raw material is iconic. In the making of a stone tool, in contrast, there is no necessary relation between the form of the final product and the original material.»[56]

In Holloway’s view, our non-human ancestors, like those of modern chimpanzees and other primates, shared motor and sensory skills, curiosity, memory, and intelligence, with perhaps differences in degree. «It is when these are integrated with the unique attributes of arbitrary production (symbolization) and imposition that man qua cultural man appears.»[57]

I have suggested above that whatever culture may be, it includes «the imposition of arbitrary forms upon the environment.» This phrase has two components. One is a recognition that the relationship between the coding process and the phenomenon (be it a tool, social network, or abstract principle) is non-iconic. The other is an idea of man as a creature who can make delusional systems work—who imposes his fantasies, his non-iconic constructs (and constructions) , upon the environment. The altered environment shapes his perceptions, and these are again forced back on the environment, are incorporated into the environment, and press for further adaptation.[58]

This is comparable to the «ratcheting» aspect suggested by Tomasello and others that enabled human evolution to accelerate. Holloway concludes that the first instance of symbolic thought among humans provided a «kick-start» for brain development, tool complexity, social structure, and language to evolve through a constant dynamic of positive feedback. «This interaction between the propensity to structure the environment arbitrarily and the feedback from the environment to the organism is an emergent process, a process different in kind from anything that preceded it .»[59]

Arbitrariness

Simple-edge chopper

Chopping-tool

Linguists Charles Hockett and R. Ascher have identified thirteen design-features of language, some shared by other forms of animal communication. One feature that distinguishes human language is its tremendous productivity; in other words, competent speakers of a language are capable of producing an exponential number of original utterances. This productivity seems to be made possible by a few critical features unique to human language. One is «duality of patterning,» meaning that human language consists of the articulation of several distinct processes, each with its own set of rules: combining phonemes to produce morphemes, combining morphemes to produce words, and combining words to produce sentences. This means that a person can master a relatively limited number of signals and sets of rules, to create infinite combinations. Another crucial element is that human language is symbolic: the sound of words (or their shape, when written) typically bear no relation to what they represent.[60] In other words, their meaning is arbitrary. That words have meaning is a matter of convention. Since the meaning of words are arbitrary, any word may have several meanings, and any object may be referred to using a variety of words; the actual word used to describe a particular object depends on the context, the intention of the speaker, and the ability of the listener to judge these appropriately. As Tomasello notes,

An individual language user looks at a tree and, before drawing the attention of her interlocutor to that tree, must decide, based on her assessment of the listener’s current knowledge and expectations, whether to say «that tree over there,» «it,» «the oak,» «that hundred-year-oak,» «the tree,» «the bagswing tree,» «that thing in the front yard,» «the ornament,» «the embarrassment,» or any of a number of other expressions. … And these decisions are not made on the basis of the speaker’s direct goal with respect to the object or activity involved, but rather that they are made on the basis of her goal with respect to the listener’s interest and attention to that object or activity.

This is why symbolic cognition and communication and imitative learning go hand-in-hand.[61]

Holloway argues that the stone-tools associated with genus Homo have the same features of human language:

Returning to matter of syntax, rules, and concatenated activity mentioned above, almost any model which describes a language process can also be used to describe tool-making. This is hardly surprising. Both activities are concatenated, both have rigid rules about the serialization of unit activities (the grammar, syntax), both are hierarchical systems of activity (as is any motor activity), and both produce arbitrary configurations which thence become part of the environment, either temporarily or permanently.[62]
Productivity can be seen in the facts that basic types were probably used for multiple purposes, that tool industries tend to expand with time, and that a slight variation on a basic pattern may be made to met some new functional requisite. Elements of a basic «vocabulary» of motor operations—flakes, detachment, rotation, preparation of striking platform, etc.—are used in different combinations to produce dissimilar tools, with different forms, and supposedly, different uses. . . . Taking each motor event alone, no one action is complete; each action depends on the prior one and requires a further one, and each is dependent on another ax on the original plan. In other words, at each point of the action except the last, the piece is not «satisfactory» in structure. Each unit action is meaningless by itself in the sense of the use of the tool; it is meaningful only in the context of the whole completed set of actions culminating in the final product. This exactly parallels language.[63]

As Tomasello has demonstrated, symbolic thought can operate only in a particular social environment:

Arbitrary symbols enforce consensus of perceptions, which not only allows members to communicate about the same objects in terms of space and time (as in hunting) but it also makes it possible for social relationships to be standardized and manipulated through symbols. It means that idiosyncrasies are smoothed out and perceived within classes of behavior. By enforcing perceptual invariance, symbols also enforce social behavioral constancy, and enforcing social behavioral constancy is a prerequisite to differential task-role sectors in a differentiated social group adapting not only to the outside environment but to its own membership.[64]

Biological anthropologist Terrence Deacon, in a synthesis of over twenty years of research on human evolution, human neurology, and primatology, describes this «ratcheting effect» as a form of «Baldwinian Evolution.» Named after psychologist James Baldwin, this describes a situation in which an animal’s behavior has evolutionary consequences when it changes the natural environment and thus the selective forces acting on the animal.[65]

Once some useful behavior spreads within a population and becomes more important for subsistence, it will generate selection pressures on genetic traits that support its propagation … Stone and symbolic tools, which were initially acquired with the aid of flexible ape-learning abilities, ultimately turned the tables on their users and forced them to adapt to a new niche opened by these technologies. Rather than being just useful tricks, these behavioral prostheses for obtaining food and organizing social behaviors became indispensable elements in a new adaptive complex. The origin of «humanness» can be defined as that point in our evolution where these tools became the principle [sic?] source of selection on our bodies and brains. It is the diagnostic of Homo symbolicus.[66]

According to Deacon, this occurred between 2 and 2.5 million years ago, when we have the first fossil evidence of stone tool use and the beginning of a trend in an increase in brain size. But it is the evolution of symbolic language which is the cause—and not the effect—of these trends.[67] More specifically, Deacon is suggesting that Australopithecines, like contemporary apes, used tools; it is possible that over the millions of years of Australopithecine history, many troops developed symbolic communication systems. All that was necessary was that one of these groups so altered their environment that «it introduced selection for very different learning abilities than affected prior species.»[68] This troop or population kick-started the Baldwinian process (the «ratchet effect») that led to their evolution to genus Homo.

The question for Deacon is, what behavioral-environmental changes could have made the development of symbolic thinking adaptive? Here he emphasizes the importance of distinguishing humans from all other species, not to privilege human intelligence but to problematize it. Given that the evolution of H. sapiens began with ancestors who did not yet have «culture,» what led them to move away from cognitive, learning, communication, and tool-making strategies that were and continued to be adaptive for most other primates (and, some have suggested, most other species of animals)? Learning symbol systems is more time consuming than other forms of communication, so symbolic thought made possible a different communication strategy, but not a more efficient one than other primates. Nevertheless, it must have offered some selective advantage to H. sapiens to have evolved. Deacon starts by looking at two key determinants in evolutionary history: foraging behavior, and patterns of sexual relations. As he observes competition for sexual access limits the possibilities for social cooperation in many species; yet, Deacon observes, there are three consistent patterns in human reproduction that distinguish them from other species:

  1. Both males and females usually contribute effort towards the rearing of their offspring, though often to differing extents and in very different ways.
  2. In all societies, the great majority of adult males and females are bound by long-term, exclusive sexual access rights and prohibitions to particular individuals of the opposite sex.
  3. They maintain these exclusive sexual relations while living in modest to large-sized, multi-male, multi-female, cooperative social groups.[69]

Moreover, there is one feature common to all known human foraging societies (all humans prior to ten or fifteen thousand years ago), and markedly different from other primates: «the use of meat. . . . The appearance of the first stone tools nearly 2.5 million years ago almost certainly correlates with a radical shift in foraging behavior to gain access to meat.»[70] Deacon does not believe that symbolic thought was necessary for hunting or tool-making (although tool-making may be a reliable index of symbolic thought); rather, it was necessary for the success of distinctive social relations.

The key is that while men and women are equally effective foragers, mothers carrying dependent children are not effective hunters. They must thus depend on male hunters. This favors a system in which males have exclusive sexual access to females, and females can predict that their sexual partner will provide food for them and their children. In most mammalian species the result is a system of rank or sexual competition that results in either polygyny, or life-long pair-bonding between two individuals who live relatively independent of other adults of their species; in both cases male aggression plays an important role in maintaining sexual access to mate(s). What is unique about humans?

Human reliance on resources that are relatively unavailable to females with infants selects not only for cooperation between a child’s father and mother but also for the cooperation of other relatives and friends, including elderly individuals and juveniles, who can be relied upon for assistance. The special demands of acquiring meat and caring for infants in our own evolution together contribute to the underlying impetus for the third characteristic feature of human reproductive patterns: cooperative group living.[71]

What is uniquely characteristic about human societies is what required symbolic cognition, which consequently leads to the evolution of culture: «cooperative, mixed-sex social groups, with significant male care and provisioning of offspring, and relatively stable patterns of reproductive exclusion.» This combination is relatively rare in other species because it is «highly susceptible to disintegration.» Language and culture provide the glue that holds it together.[72]

Chimpanzees also, on occasion, hunt meat; in most cases, however, males consume the meat immediately, and only on occasion share with females who happen to be nearby. Among chimpanzees, hunting for meat increases when other sources of food become scarce, but under these conditions, sharing decreases. The first forms of symbolic thinking made stone-tools possible, which in turn made hunting for meat a more dependable source of food for our nonhuman ancestors while making possible forms of social communication that make sharing—between males and females, but also among males, decreasing sexual competition:

So the socio-ecological problem posed by the transition to a meat-supplemented subsistence strategy is that it cannot be utilized without a social structure which guarantees unambiguous and exclusive mating and is sufficiently egalitarian to sustain cooperation via shared or parallel reproductive interests. This problem can be solved symbolically.[73]

Symbols and symbolic thinking thus make possible a central feature of social relations in every human population: reciprocity. Evolutionary scientists have developed a model to explain reciprocal altruism among closely related individuals. Symbolic thought makes possible reciprocity between distantly related individuals.[74]

Archeological approaches to culture: matter and meaning

Bifacial points, engraved ochre and bone tools from the c. 75,000–80,000 year old M1 & M2 phases at Blombos cave

Excavations at the South Area of Çatal Höyük

In the 19th century archeology was often a supplement to history, and the goal of archeologists was to identify artifacts according to their typology and stratigraphy, thus marking their location in time and space. Franz Boas established that archeology be one of American anthropology’s four fields, and debates among archeologists have often paralleled debates among cultural anthropologists. In the 1920s and 1930s, Australian-British archeologist V. Gordon Childe and American archeologist W. C. McKern independently began moving from asking about the date of an artifact, to asking about the people who produced it — when archeologists work alongside historians, historical materials generally help answer these questions, but when historical materials are unavailable, archeologists had to develop new methods. Childe and McKern focused on analyzing the relationships among objects found together; their work established the foundation for a three-tiered model:

  1. An individual artifact, which has surface, shape, and technological attributes (e.g. an arrowhead)
  2. A sub-assemblage, consisting of artifacts that are found, and were likely used, together (e.g. an arrowhead, bow and knife)
  3. An assemblage of sub-assemblages that together constitute the archeological site (e.g. the arrowhead, bow and knife; a pot and the remains of a hearth; a shelter)

Childe argued that a «constantly recurring assemblage of artifacts» to be an «archaeological culture.»[75][76] Childe and others viewed «each archeological culture … the manifestation in material terms of a specific people[77]

In 1948 Walter Taylor systematized the methods and concepts that archeologists had developed and proposed a general model for the archeological contribution to knowledge of cultures. He began with the mainstream understanding of culture as the product of human cognitive activity, and the Boasian emphasis on the subjective meanings of objects as dependent on their cultural context. He defined culture as «a mental phenomenon, consisting of the contents of minds, not of material objects or observable behavior.»[78] He then devised a three-tiered model linking cultural anthropology to archeology, which he called conjunctive archeology:

  1. Culture, which is unobservable and nonmaterial
  2. Behaviors resulting from culture, which are observable and nonmaterial
  3. Objectifications, such as artifacts and architecture, which are the result of behavior and material

That is, material artifacts were the material residue of culture, but not culture itself.[79] Taylor’s point was that the archeological record could contribute to anthropological knowledge, but only if archeologists reconceived their work not just as digging up artifacts and recording their location in time and space, but as inferring from material remains the behaviors through which they were produced and used, and inferring from these behaviors the mental activities of people. Although many archeologists agreed that their research was integral to anthropology, Taylor’s program was never fully implemented. One reason was that his three-tier model of inferences required too much fieldwork and laboratory analysis to be practical.[80] Moreover, his view that material remains were not themselves cultural, and in fact twice-removed from culture, in fact left archeology marginal to cultural anthropology.[81]

In 1962 Leslie White’s former student Lewis Binford proposed a new model for anthropological archeology, called «the New Archeology» or «Processual Archeology,» based on White’s definition of culture as «the extra-somatic means of adaptation for the human organism.»[82] This definition allowed Binford to establish archeology as a crucial field for the pursuit of the methodology of Julian Steward’s cultural ecology:

The comparative study of cultural systems with variable technologies in a similar environmental range or similar technologies in differing environments is a major methodology of what Steward (1955: 36–42) has called «cultural ecology,» and certainly is a valuable means of increasing our understanding of cultural processes. Such a methodology is also useful in elucidating the structural relationships between major cultural sub-systems such as the social and ideological sub-systems.[83]

In other words, Binford proposed an archeology that would be central to the dominant project of cultural anthropologists at the time (culture as non-genetic adaptations to the environment); the «new archeology» was the cultural anthropology (in the form of cultural ecology or ecological anthropology) of the past.

In the 1980s, there was a movement in the United Kingdom and Europe against the view of archeology as a field of anthropology, echoing Radcliffe-Brown’s earlier rejection of cultural anthropology.[84] During this same period, then-Cambridge archeologist Ian Hodder developed «post-processual archeology» as an alternative. Like Binford (and unlike Taylor) Hodder views artifacts not as objectifications of culture but as culture itself. Unlike Binford, however, Hodder does not view culture as an environmental adaptation. Instead, he «is committed to a fluid semiotic version of the traditional culture concept in which material items, artifacts, are full participants in the creation, deployment, alteration, and fading away of symbolic complexes.»[85] His 1982 book, Symbols in Action, evokes the symbolic anthropology of Geertz, Schneider, with their focus on the context dependent meanings of cultural things, as an alternative to White and Steward’s materialist view of culture.[86] In his 1991 textbook, Reading the Past: Current Approaches to Interpretation in Archaeology Hodder argued that archeology is more closely aligned to history than to anthropology.[87]

Language and culture

The connection between culture and language has been noted as far back as the classical period and probably long before. The ancient Greeks, for example, distinguished between civilized peoples and bárbaros «those who babble», i.e. those who speak unintelligible languages.[88] The fact that different groups speak different, unintelligible languages is often considered more tangible evidence for cultural differences than other less obvious cultural traits.

The German romanticists of the 19th century such as Herder, Wundt and Humbolt, often saw language not just as one cultural trait among many but rather as the direct expression of a people’s national character, and as such as culture in a kind of condensed form. Herder for example suggests, «Denn jedes Volk ist Volk; es hat seine National Bildung wie seine Sprache» (Since every people is a People, it has its own national culture expressed through its own language).[89]

Franz Boas, founder of American anthropology, like his German forerunners, maintained that the shared language of a community is the most essential carrier of their common culture. Boas was the first anthropologist who considered it unimaginable to study the culture of a foreign people without also becoming acquainted with their language. For Boas, the fact that the intellectual culture of a people was largely constructed, shared and maintained through the use of language, meant that understanding the language of a cultural group was the key to understanding its culture. At the same time, though, Boas and his students were aware that culture and language are not directly dependent on one another. That is, groups with widely different cultures may share a common language, and speakers of completely unrelated languages may share the same fundamental cultural traits.[90][91] Numerous other scholars have suggested that the form of language determines specific cultural traits.[92] This is similar to the notion of Linguistic determinism, which states that the form of language determines individual thought. While Boas himself rejected a causal link between language and culture, some of his intellectual heirs entertained the idea that habitual patterns of speaking and thinking in a particular language may influence the culture of the linguistic group.[93] Such belief is related to the theory of Linguistic relativity. Boas, like most modern anthropologists, however, was more inclined to relate the interconnectedness between language and culture to the fact that, as B.L. Whorf put it, «they have grown up together».[94]

Indeed, the origin of language, understood as the human capacity of complex symbolic communication, and the origin of complex culture is often thought to stem from the same evolutionary process in early man. Evolutionary anthropologists[citation needed] suppose that language evolved as early humans began to live in large communities which required the use of complex communication to maintain social coherence. Language and culture then both emerged as a means of using symbols to construct social identity and maintain coherence within a social group too large to rely exclusively on pre-human ways of building community such as for example grooming. Since language and culture are both in essence symbolic systems, twentieth century cultural theorists have applied the methods of analyzing language developed in the science of linguistics to also analyze culture. Particularly the structural theory of Ferdinand de Saussure, which describes symbolic systems as consisting of signs (a pairing of a particular form with a particular meaning), has come to be applied widely in the study of culture. But also post-structuralist theories, that nonetheless still rely on the parallel between language and culture as systems of symbolic communication, have been applied in the field of semiotics. The parallel between language and culture can then be understood as analog to the parallel between a linguistic sign, consisting for example of the sound [kau] and the meaning «cow», and a cultural sign, consisting for example of the cultural form of «wearing a crown» and the cultural meaning of «being king». In this way it can be argued that culture is itself a kind of language. Another parallel between cultural and linguistic systems is that they are both systems of practice, that is they are a set of special ways of doing things that is constructed and perpetuated through social interactions.[95] Children, for example, acquire language in the same way as they acquire the basic cultural norms of the society they grow up in – through interaction with older members of their cultural group.

However, languages, now understood as the particular set of speech norms of a particular community, are also a part of the larger culture of the community that speak them. Humans use language as a way of signalling identity with one cultural group and difference from others. Even among speakers of one language several different ways of using the language exist, and each is used to signal affiliation with particular subgroups within a larger culture. In linguistics such different ways of using the same language are called «varieties». For example, the English language is spoken differently in the USA, the UK and Australia, and even within English-speaking countries there are hundreds of dialects of English that each signal a belonging to a particular region and/or subculture. For example, in the UK the cockney dialect signals its speakers’ belonging to the group of lower class workers of east London. Differences between varieties of the same language often consist in different pronunciations and vocabulary, but also sometimes of different grammatical systems and very often in using different styles (e.g. cockney Rhyming slang or Lawyers’ jargon). Linguists and anthropologists, particularly sociolinguists, ethnolinguists and linguistic anthropologists have specialized in studying how ways of speaking vary between speech communities.

A community’s ways of speaking or signing are a part of the community’s culture, just as other shared practices are. Language use is a way of establishing and displaying group identity. Ways of speaking function not only to facilitate communication, but also to identify the social position of the speaker. Linguists call different ways of speaking language varieties, a term that encompasses geographically or socioculturally defined dialects as well as the jargons or styles of subcultures. Linguistic anthropologists and sociologists of language define communicative style as the ways that language is used and understood within a particular culture.[96]

The differences between languages does not consist only in differences in pronunciation, vocabulary or grammar, but also in different «cultures of speaking». Some cultures for example have elaborate systems of «social deixis», systems of signalling social distance through linguistic means.[97] In English, social deixis is shown mostly though distinguishing between addressing some people by first name and others by surname, but also in titles such as «Mrs.», «boy», «Doctor» or «Your Honor», but in other languages such systems may be highly complex and codified in the entire grammar and vocabulary of the language. In several languages of east Asia, for example Thai, Burmese and Javanese, different words are used according to whether a speaker is addressing someone of higher or lower rank than oneself in a ranking system with animals and children ranking the lowest and gods and members of royalty as the highest.[97] Other languages may use different forms of address when speaking to speakers of the opposite gender or in-law relatives and many languages have special ways of speaking to infants and children. Among other groups, the culture of speaking may entail not speaking to particular people, for example many indigenous cultures of Australia have a taboo against talking to one’s in-law relatives, and in some cultures speech is not addressed directly to children. Some languages also require different ways of speaking for different social classes of speakers, and often such a system is based on gender differences, as in Japanese and Koasati.[98]

Cultural anthropology

1899–1946: Universal versus particular

Franz Boas established modern American anthropology as the study of the sum total of human phenomena.

The modern anthropological understanding of culture has its origins in the 19th century with German anthropologist Adolf Bastian’s theory of the «psychic unity of mankind,» which, influenced by Herder and von Humboldt, challenged the identification of «culture» with the way of life of European elites, and British anthropologist Edward Burnett Tylor’s attempt to define culture as inclusively as possible. Tylor in 1874 described culture in the following way: «Culture or civilization, taken in its wide ethnographic sense, is that complex whole which includes knowledge, belief, art, morals, law, custom, and any other capabilities and habits acquired by man as a member of society.»[99] Although Tylor was not aiming to propose a general theory of culture (he explained his understanding of culture in the course of a larger argument about the nature of religion), American anthropologists have generally presented their various definitions of culture as refinements of Tylor’s. Franz Boas’s student Alfred Kroeber (1876–1970) identified culture with the «superorganic,» that is, a domain with ordering principles and laws that could not be explained by or reduced to biology.[100] In 1973, Gerald Weiss reviewed various definitions of culture and debates as to their parsimony and power, and proposed as the most scientifically useful definition that «culture» be defined «as our generic term for all human nongenetic, or metabiological, phenomena» (italics in the original).[101]

Ruth Benedict was instrumental in establishing the modern conception of distinct cultures being patterned.

Franz Boas, founded modern American anthropology with the establishment of the first graduate program in anthropology at Columbia University in 1896. At the time the dominant model of culture was that of cultural evolution, which posited that human societies progressed through stages of savagery to barbarism to civilization; thus, societies that for example are based on horticulture and Iroquois kinship terminology are less evolved than societies based on agriculture and Eskimo kinship terminology. One of Boas’s greatest accomplishments was to demonstrate convincingly that this model is fundamentally flawed, empirically, methodologically, and theoretically. Moreover, he felt that our knowledge of different cultures was so incomplete, and often based on unsystematic or unscientific research, that it was impossible to develop any scientifically valid general model of human cultures. Instead, he established the principle of cultural relativism and trained students to conduct rigorous participant observation field research in different societies. Boas understood the capacity for culture to involve symbolic thought and social learning, and considered the evolution of a capacity for culture to coincide with the evolution of other, biological, features defining genus Homo. Nevertheless, he argued that culture could not be reduced to biology or other expressions of symbolic thought, such as language. Boas and his students understood culture inclusively and resisted developing a general definition of culture. Indeed, they resisted identifying «culture» as a thing, instead using culture as an adjective rather than a noun. Boas argued that cultural «types» or «forms» are always in a state of flux.[102][103] His student Alfred Kroeber argued that the «unlimited receptivity and assimilativeness of culture» made it practically impossible to think of cultures as discrete things.[104]

Zuñi girl with jar, 1903

Boas’s students dominated cultural anthropology through World War II, and continued to have great influence through the 1960s. They were especially interested in two phenomena: the great variety of forms culture took around the world,[105] and the many ways individuals were shaped by and acted creatively through their own cultures.[106][107] This led his students to focus on the history of cultural traits: how they spread from one society to another, and how their meanings changed over time[108][109]—and the life histories of members of other societies.[110][111][112][113][114][115][116][117] Others, such as Ruth Benedict (1887–1948) and Margaret Mead (1901–1978), produced monographs or comparative studies analyzing the forms of creativity possible to individuals within specific cultural configurations.[118][119][120] Essential to their research was the concept of «context»: culture provided a context that made the behavior of individuals understandable; geography and history provided a context for understanding the differences between cultures. Thus, although Boasians were committed to the belief in the psychic unity of humankind and the universality of culture, their emphasis on local context and cultural diversity led them away from proposing cultural universals or universal theories of culture.

There is a tension in cultural anthropology between the claim that culture is a universal (the fact that all human societies have culture), and that it is also particular (culture takes a tremendous variety of forms around the world). Since Boas, two debates have dominated cultural anthropology. The first has to do with ways of modeling particular cultures. Specifically, anthropologists have argued as to whether «culture» can be thought of as a bounded and integrated thing, or as a quality of a diverse collection of things, the numbers and meanings of which are in constant flux. Boas’s student Ruth Benedict suggested that in any given society cultural traits may be more or less «integrated,» that is, constituting a pattern of action and thought that gives purpose to people’s lives, and provides them with a basis from which to evaluate new actions and thoughts, although she implies that there are various degrees of integration; indeed, she observes that some cultures fail to integrate.[121] Boas, however, argued that complete integration is rare and that a given culture only appears to be integrated because of observer bias.[122] For Boas, the appearance of such patterns—a national culture, for example—was the effect of a particular point of view.[123]

The first debate was effectively suspended in 1934 when Ruth Benedict published Patterns of Culture, which has continuously been in print. Although this book is well known for popularizing the Boasian principle of cultural relativism, among anthropologists it constituted both an important summary of the discoveries of Boasians, and a decisive break from Boas’s emphasis on the mobility of diverse cultural traits. «Anthropological work has been overwhelmingly devoted to the analysis of cultural traits,» she wrote «rather than to the study of cultures as articulated wholes.»[124] Influenced by Polish-British social anthropologist Bronisław Malinowski, however, she argued that «The first essential, so it seems today, is to study the living culture, to know its habits of thought and the functions of its institutions» and that «the only way in which we can know the significance of the selected detail of behavior is against the background of the motives and emotions and values that are institutionalized in that culture.»[125] Influenced by German historians Wilhelm Dilthey and Oswald Spengler, as well as by gestalt psychology, she argued that «the whole determines its parts, not only their relation but their very nature,»[126] and that «cultures, likewise, are more than the sum of their traits.»[127] Just as each spoken language draws very selectively from an extensive, but finite, set of sounds any human mouth (free from defect) can make, she concluded that in each society people, over time and through both conscious and unconscious processes, selected from an extensive but finite set of cultural traits which then combine to form a unique and distinctive pattern.»[128]

The significance of cultural behavior is not exhausted when we have clearly understood that it is local and man-made and hugely variable. It tends to be integrated. A culture, like an individual, is a more or less consistent pattern of thought and action. Within each culture there come into being characteristic purposes not necessarily shared by other types of society. In obedience to their purposes, each people further and further consolidates its experience, and in proportion to the urgency of these drives the heterogeneous items of behavior take more and more congruous shape. Taken up by a well-integrated culture, the most ill-assorted acts become characteristic of its particular goals, often by the most unlikely metamorphoses.[129]

Although Benedict felt that virtually all cultures are patterned, she argued that these patterns change over time as a consequence of human creativity, and therefore different societies around the world had distinct characters. Patterns of Culture contrasts Zuňi, Dobu and Kwakiutl cultures as a way of highlighting different ways of being human. Benedict observed that many Westerners felt that this view forced them to abandon their «dreams of permanence and ideality and with the individual’s illusions of autonomy» and that for many, this made existence «empty.»[130] She argued however that once people accepted the results of scientific research, people would «arrive then at a more realistic social faith, accepting as grounds of hope and as new bases for tolerance the coexisting and equally valid patterns of life which mankind has created for itself from the raw materials of existence.»[130]

This view of culture has had a tremendous impact outside of anthropology, and dominated American anthropology until the Cold War, when anthropologists like Sidney Mintz and Eric Wolf rejected the validity and value of approaching «each culture» as «a world in itself» and «relatively stable.»[131] They felt that, too often, this approach ignored the impact of imperialism, colonialism, and the world capitalist economy on the peoples Benedict and her followers studied (and thus re-opened the debate on the relationship between the universal and the particular, in the form of the relationship between the global and the local). In the meantime, its emphasis on metamorphosing patterns influenced French structuralism and made American anthropologists receptive to British structural-functionalism.

Turkish nomad clan with the nodes as marriages

Mexican village with the nodes as marriages

Iroqois Kinship Structure

The second debate has been over the ability to make universal claims about all cultures. Although Boas argued that anthropologists had yet to collect enough solid evidence from a diverse sample of societies to make any valid general or universal claims about culture, by the 1940s some felt ready. Whereas Kroeber and Benedict had argued that «culture»—which could refer to local, regional, or trans-regional scales—was in some way «patterned» or «configured,» some anthropologists now felt that enough data had been collected to demonstrate that it often took highly structured forms. The question these anthropologists debated was, were these structures statistical artifacts, or where they expressions of mental models? This debate emerged full-fledged in 1949, with the publication of George Murdock’s Social Structure, and Claude Lévi-Strauss’s Les Structures Élémentaires de la Parenté.

Opposing Boas and his students was Yale anthropologist George Murdock, who compiled the Human Relations Area Files. These files code cultural variables found in different societies, so that anthropologists can use statistical methods to study correlations among different variables.[132][133][134] The ultimate aim of this project is to develop generalizations that apply to increasingly larger numbers of individual cultures. Later, Murdock and Douglas R. White developed the standard cross-cultural sample as a way to refine this method.

French anthropologist Claude Lévi-Strauss’s structuralist anthropology brought together ideas of Boas (especially Boas’s belief in the mutability of cultural forms, and Bastian’s belief in the psychic unity of humankind) and French sociologist’s Émile Durkheim’s focus on social structures (institutionalized relationships among persons and groups of persons). Instead of making generalizations that applied to large numbers of societies, Lévi-Strauss sought to derive from concrete cases increasingly abstract models of human nature. His method begins with the supposition that culture exists in two different forms: the many distinct structures that could be inferred from observing members of the same society interact (and of which members of a society are themselves aware), and abstract structures developed by analyzing shared ways (such as myths and rituals) members of a society represent their social life (and of which members of a society are not only not consciously aware, but which moreover typically stand in opposition to, or negate, the social structures of which people are aware). He then sought to develop one universal mental structure that could only be inferred through the systematic comparison of particular social and cultural structures. He argued that just as there are laws through which a finite and relatively small number of chemical elements could be combined to create a seemingly infinite variety of things, there were a finite and relatively small number of cultural elements which people combine to create the great variety of cultures anthropologists observe. The systematic comparison of societies would enable an anthropologist to develop this cultural «table of elements,» and once completed, this table of cultural elements would enable an anthropologist to analyze specific cultures and achieve insights hidden to the very people who produced and lived through these cultures.[135][136] Structuralism came to dominate French anthropology and, in the late 1960s and 1970s, came to have great influence on American and British anthropology.

Murdock’s HRAF and Lévi-Strauss’s structuralism provide two ambitious ways to seek the universal in the particular, and both approaches continue to appeal to different anthropologists. However, the differences between them reveal a tension implicit in the heritage of Tylor and Bastian. Is culture to be found in empirically observed behaviors that may form the basis of generalizations? Or does it consist of universal mental processes, which must be inferred and abstracted from observed behavior? This question has driven debates among biological anthropologists and archeologists as well.

Structural-Functionalist challenge: Society versus culture

In the 1940s the Boasian understanding of culture was challenged by a new paradigm for anthropological and social science research called Structural functionalism. This paradigm developed independently but in parallel in both the United Kingdom and in the United States (In both cases it is sui generis: it has no direct relationship to «structuralism» except that both French structuralism and Anglo-American Structural-Functionalism were all influenced by Durkheim. It is also analogous, but unrelated to, other forms of «functionalism»). Whereas the Boasians viewed anthropology as that natural science dedicated to the study of humankind, structural functionalists viewed anthropology as one social science among many, dedicated to the study of one specific facet of humanity. This led structural-functionalists to redefine and minimize the scope of «culture.»

In the United Kingdom, the creation of structural functionalism was anticipated by Raymond Firth’s (1901–2002) We the Tikopia, published in 1936, and marked by the publication of African Political Systems, edited by Meyer Fortes (1906–1983) and E.E. Evans-Pritchard (1902–1973) in 1940.[137][138] In these works these anthropologists forwarded a synthesis of the ideas of their mentor, Bronisław Malinowski (1884–1942), and his rival, A. R. Radcliffe-Brown (1881–1955). Both Malinowski and Radcliffe-Brown viewed anthropology—what they call «social anthropology»—as that branch of sociology that studied so-called primitive societies. According to Malinowski’s theory of functionalism, all human beings have certain biological needs, such as the need for food and shelter, and humankind has the biological need to reproduce. Every society develops its own institutions, which function to fulfill these needs. In order for these institutions to function, individuals take on particular social roles that regulate how they act and interact. Although members of any given society may not understand the ultimate functions of their roles and institutions, an ethnographer can develop a model of these functions through the careful observation of social life.[139] Radcliffe-Brown rejected Malinowski’s notion of function, and believed that a general theory of primitive social life could only be built up through the careful comparison of different societies. Influenced by the work of French sociologist Émile Durkheim (1858–1917), who argued that primitive and modern societies are distinguished by distinct social structures, Radcliffe-Brown argued that anthropologists first had to map out the social structure of any given society before comparing the structures of different societies.[140] Firth, Fortes, and Evans-Pritchard found it easy to combine Malinowski’s attention to social roles and institutions with Radcliffe-Brown’s concern with social structures. They distinguished between «social organization» (observable social interactions) and «social structure» (rule-governed patterns of social interaction), and shifted their attention from biological functions to social functions. For example, how different institutions are functionally integrated, and the extent to, and ways in, which institutions function to promote social solidarity and stability. In short, instead of culture (understood as all human non-genetic or extra-somatic phenomena) they made «sociality» (interactions and relationships among persons and groups of people) their object of study. (Indeed, Radcliffe-Brown once wrote «I should like to invoke a taboo on the word culture.»)[141]

Coincidentally, in 1946 sociologist Talcott Parsons (1902–1979) founded the Department of Social Relations at Harvard University. Influenced by such European sociologists as Émile Durkheim and Max Weber, Parsons developed a theory of social action that was closer to British social anthropology than to Boas’s American anthropology, and which he also called «structural functionalism.» Parson’s intention was to develop a total theory of social action (why people act as they do), and to develop at Harvard and inter-disciplinary program that would direct research according to this theory. His model explained human action as the result of four systems:

  1. the «behavioral system» of biological needs
  2. the «personality system» of an individual’s characteristics affecting their functioning in the social world
  3. the «social system» of patterns of units of social interaction, especially social status and role
  4. the «cultural system» of norms and values that regulate social action symbolically

According to this theory, the second system was the proper object of study for psychologists; the third system for sociologists, and the fourth system for cultural anthropologists.[142][143] Whereas the Boasians considered all of these systems to be objects of study by anthropologists, and «personality» and «status and role» to be as much a part of «culture» as «norms and values,» Parsons envisioned a much narrower role for anthropology and a much narrower definition of culture.

Although Boasian cultural anthropologists were interested in norms and values, among many other things, it was only with the rise of structural functionalism that people came to identify «culture» with «norms and values.» Many American anthropologists rejected this view of culture (and by implication, anthropology). In 1980, anthropologist Eric Wolf wrote,

As the social sciences transformed themselves into «behavioral» science, explanations for behavior were no longer traced to culture: behavior was to be understood in terms of psychological encounters, strategies of economic choice, strivings for payoffs in games of power. Culture, once extended to all acts and ideas employed in social life, was now relegated to the margins as «world view» or «values.»[144]

Nevertheless, several of Talcott Parsons’ students emerged as leading American anthropologists. At the same time, many American anthropologists had a high regard for the research produced by social anthropologists in the 1940s and 1950s, and found structural-functionalism to provide a very useful model for conducting ethnographic research.

The combination of American cultural anthropology theory with British social anthropology methods has led to some confusion between the concepts of «society» and «culture.» For most anthropologists, these are distinct concepts. Society refers to a group of people; culture refers to a pan-human capacity and the totality of non-genetic human phenomena. Societies are often clearly bounded; cultural traits are often mobile, and cultural boundaries, such as they are, can be typically porous, permeable, and plural.[145] During the 1950s and 1960s anthropologists often worked in places where social and cultural boundaries coincided, thus obscuring the distinction. When disjunctures between these boundaries become highly salient, for example during the period of European de-colonization of Africa in the 1960s and 1970s, or during the post-Bretton Woods realignment of globalization, however, the difference often becomes central to anthropological debates.[146][147][148][149][150]

1946–1968: Symbolic versus adaptive

Huli Wigman from the Southern Highlands

In Hinduism, the cow is a symbol of wealth, strength, and selfless giving.

Cleveley’s depiction of Captain Cook

Vietcong troops pose with new AK-47 rifles

Parsons’ students Clifford Geertz and David M. Schneider, and Schneider’s student Roy Wagner, went on to important careers as cultural anthropologists and developed a school within American cultural anthropology called «symbolic anthropology,» the study of the social construction and social effects of symbols.[151][152][153][154] Since symbolic anthropology easily complemented social anthropologists’ studies of social life and social structure, many British structural-functionalists (who rejected or were uninterested in Boasian cultural anthropology) accepted the Parsonian definition of «culture» and «cultural anthropology.» British anthropologist Victor Turner (who eventually left the United Kingdom to teach in the United States) was an important bridge between American and British symbolic anthropology.[155]

Attention to symbols, the meaning of which depended almost entirely on their historical and social context, appealed to many Boasians. Leslie White asked of cultural things, «What sort of objects are they? Are they physical objects? Mental objects? Both? Metaphors? Symbols? Reifications?» In Science of Culture (1949), he concluded that they are objects «sui generis«; that is, of their own kind. In trying to define that kind, he hit upon a previously unrealized aspect of symbolization, which he called «the symbolate»—an object created by the act of symbolization. He thus defined culture as «symbolates understood in an extra-somatic context.»[156]

Nevertheless, by the 1930s White began turning away from the Boasian approach.[157] He wrote,

In order to live man, like all other species, must come to terms with the external world…. Man employs his sense organs, nerves, glands, and muscles in adjusting himself to the external world. But in addition to this he has another means of adjustment and control…. This mechanism is culture.[158]

Although this view echoes that of Malinowski, the key concept for White was not «function» but «adaptation.» Whereas the Boasians were interested in the history of specific traits, White was interested in the cultural history of the human species, which he felt should be studied from an evolutionary perspective. Thus, the task of anthropology is to study «not only how culture evolves, but why as well…. In the case of man … the power to invent and to discover, the ability to select and use the better of two tools or ways of doing something— these are the factors of cultural evolution.»[159] Unlike 19th century evolutionists, who were concerned with how civilized societies rose above primitive societies, White was interested in documenting how, over time, humankind as a whole has through cultural means discovered more and more ways for capturing and harnessing energy from the environment, in the process transforming culture.

At the same time that White was developing his theory of cultural evolution, Kroeber’s student Julian Steward was developing his theory of cultural ecology. In 1938 he published Basin-Plateau Aboriginal Socio-Political Groups in which he argued that diverse societies—for example the indigenous Shoshone or White farmers on the Great Plains—were not less or more evolved; rather, they had adapted differently to different environments.[160] Whereas Leslie White was interested in culture understood holistically as a property of the human species, Julian Steward was interested in culture as the property of distinct societies. Like White he viewed culture as a means of adapting to the environment, but he criticized Whites «unilineal» (one direction) theory of cultural evolution and instead proposed a model of «multilineal» evolution in which (in the Boasian tradition) each society has its own cultural history.[161]

When Julian Steward left a teaching position at the University of Michigan to work in Utah in 1930, Leslie White took his place; in 1946 Julian Steward was made Chair of the Columbia University Anthropology Department. In the 1940s and 1950s their students, most notably Marvin Harris, Sidney Mintz, Robert Murphy, Roy Rappaport, Marshall Sahlins, Elman Service, Andrew P. Vayda and Eric Wolf dominated American anthropology.[162][163][164][165][166][167][168][169][170] Most promoted materialist understandings of culture in opposition to the symbolic approaches of Geertz and Schneider. Harris, Rappaport, and Vayda were especially important for their contributions to cultural materialism and ecological anthropology, both of which argued that «culture» constituted an extra-somatic (or non-biological) means through which human beings could adapt to life in drastically differing physical environments.

The debate between symbolic and materialist approaches to culture dominated American Anthropologists in the 1960s and 1970s. The Vietnam War and the publication of Dell Hymes’ Reinventing Anthropology, however, marked a growing dissatisfaction with the then dominant approaches to culture. Hymes argued that fundamental elements of the Boasian project such as holism and an interest in diversity were still worth pursuing: «interest in other peoples and their ways of life, and concern to explain them within a frame of reference that includes ourselves.»[171] Moreover, he argued that cultural anthropologists are singularly well-equipped to lead this study (with an indirect rebuke to sociologists like Parsons who sought to subsume anthropology to their own project):

In the practice there is a traditional place for openness to phenomena in ways not predefined by theory or design – attentiveness to complex phenomena, to phenomena of interest, perhaps aesthetic, for their own sake, to the sensory as well as intellectual, aspects of the subject. These comparative and practical perspectives, though not unique to formal anthropology, are specially husbanded there, and might well be impaired, if the study of man were to be united under the guidance of others who lose touch with experience in concern for methodology, who forget the ends of social knowledge in elaborating its means, or who are unwittingly or unconcernedly culture-bound.[172]

It is these elements, Hymes argued, that justify a «general study of man,» that is, «anthropology».[173]

During this time notable anthropologists such as Mintz, Murphy, Sahlins, and Wolf eventually broke away, experimenting with structuralist and Marxist approaches to culture, they continued to promote cultural anthropology against structural functionalism.[174][175][176][177][178]

1940–present: Local versus global

Big Tree, a Kiowa chief and warrior

The Tepozteco mountain dominates views from Tepoztlán.

Boas and Malinowski established ethnographic research as a highly localized method for studying culture. Yet Boas emphasized that culture is dynamic, moving from one group of people to another, and that specific cultural forms have to be analyzed in a larger context. This has led anthropologists to explore different ways of understanding the global dimensions of culture.

In the 1940s and 1950s, several key studies focused on how trade between indigenous peoples and the Europeans who had conquered and colonized the Americas influenced indigenous culture, either through change in the organization of labor, or change in critical technologies. Bernard Mishkin studied the effect of the introduction of horses on Kiowa political organization and warfare.[179] Oscar Lewis explored the influence of the fur trade on Blackfoot culture (relying heavily on historical sources).[180] Joseph Jablow documented how Cheyenne social organization and subsistence strategy between 1795 and 1840 were determined by their position in trade networks linking Whites and other Indians.[181] Frank Secoy argued that Great Plains Indians’ social organization and military tactics changed as horses, introduced by the Spanish in the south, diffused north, and guns, introduced by the British and French in the east, diffused west.[182]

In the 1950s Robert Redfield and students of Julian Steward pioneered «community studies,» namely, the study of distinct communities (whether identified by race, ethnicity, or economic class) in Western or «Westernized» societies, especially cities. They thus encountered the antagonisms 19th century critics described using the terms «high culture» and «low culture.» These 20th century anthropologists struggled to describe people who were politically and economically inferior but not, they believed, culturally inferior. Oscar Lewis proposed the concept of a «culture of poverty» to describe the cultural mechanisms through which people adapted to a life of economic poverty. Other anthropologists and sociologists began using the term «sub-culture» to describe culturally distinct communities that were part of larger societies.

One important kind of subculture is that formed by an immigrant community. In dealing with immigrant groups and their cultures, there are various approaches:

  • Leitkultur (core culture): A model developed in Germany by Bassam Tibi. The idea is that minorities can have an identity of their own, but they should at least support the core concepts of the culture on which the society is based.
  • Melting Pot: In the United States, the traditional view has been one of a melting pot where all the immigrant cultures are mixed and amalgamated without state intervention.
  • Monoculturalism: In some European states, culture is very closely linked to nationalism, thus government policy is to assimilate immigrants, although recent increases in migration have led many European states to experiment with forms of multiculturalism.
  • Multiculturalism: A policy that immigrants and others should preserve their cultures with the different cultures interacting peacefully within one nation.

The way nation states treat immigrant cultures rarely falls neatly into one or another of the above approaches. The degree of difference with the host culture (i.e., «foreignness»), the number of immigrants, attitudes of the resident population, the type of government policies that are enacted, and the effectiveness of those policies all make it difficult to generalize about the effects. Similarly with other subcultures within a society, attitudes of the mainstream population and communications between various cultural groups play a major role in determining outcomes. The study of cultures within a society is complex and research must take into account a myriad of variables.

Cultural studies

In the United Kingdom, sociologists and other scholars influenced by Marxism, such as Stuart Hall and Raymond Williams, developed Cultural Studies. Following nineteenth century Romantics, they identified «culture» with consumption goods and leisure activities (such as art, music, film, food, sports, and clothing). Nevertheless, they understood patterns of consumption and leisure to be determined by relations of production, which led them to focus on class relations and the organization of production.[183][184] In the United States, «Cultural Studies» focuses largely on the study of popular culture, that is, the social meanings of mass-produced consumer and leisure goods. The term was coined by Richard Hoggart in 1964 when he founded the Birmingham Centre for Contemporary Cultural Studies or CCCS. It has since become strongly associated with Stuart Hall, who succeeded Hoggart as Director.

From the 1970s onward, Stuart Hall’s pioneering work, along with his colleagues Paul Willis, Dick Hebdige, Tony Jefferson, and Angela McRobbie, created an international intellectual movement. As the field developed it began to combine political economy, communication, sociology, social theory, literary theory, media theory, film/video studies, cultural anthropology, philosophy, museum studies and art history to study cultural phenomena or cultural texts. In this field researchers often concentrate on how particular phenomena relate to matters of ideology, nationality, ethnicity, social class, and/or gender.[citation needed] Cultural studies is concerned with the meaning and practices of everyday life. These practices comprise the ways people do particular things (such as watching television, or eating out) in a given culture. This field studies the meanings and uses people attribute to various objects and practices. Recently, as capitalism has spread throughout the world (a process called globalization), cultural studies has begun to analyse local and global forms of resistance to Western hegemony.[citation needed]

In the context of cultural studies, the idea of a text not only includes written language, but also films, photographs, fashion or hairstyles: the texts of cultural studies comprise all the meaningful artifacts of culture.[citation needed] Similarly, the discipline widens the concept of «culture». «Culture» for a cultural studies researcher not only includes traditional high culture (the culture of ruling social groups)[185] and popular culture, but also everyday meanings and practices. The last two, in fact, have become the main focus of cultural studies. A further and recent approach is comparative cultural studies, based on the discipline of comparative literature and cultural studies.[citation needed]

Scholars in the United Kingdom and the United States developed somewhat different versions of cultural studies after the field’s inception in the late 1970s. The British version of cultural studies was developed in the 1950s and 1960s mainly under the influence first of Richard Hoggart, E. P. Thompson, and Raymond Williams, and later Stuart Hall and others at the Centre for Contemporary Cultural Studies at the University of Birmingham. This included overtly political, left-wing views, and criticisms of popular culture as ‘capitalist’ mass culture; it absorbed some of the ideas of the Frankfurt School critique of the «culture industry» (i.e. mass culture). This emerges in the writings of early British cultural-studies scholars and their influences: see the work of (for example) Raymond Williams, Stuart Hall, Paul Willis, and Paul Gilroy.

Whereas in the United States Lindlof & Taylor say that «cultural studies was grounded in a pragmatic, liberal-pluralist tradition».[186] The American version of cultural studies initially concerned itself more with understanding the subjective and appropriative side of audience reactions to, and uses of, mass culture; for example, American cultural-studies advocates wrote about the liberatory aspects of fandom.[citation needed] The distinction between American and British strands, however, has faded.[citation needed] Some researchers, especially in early British cultural studies, apply a Marxist model to the field. This strain of thinking has some influence from the Frankfurt School, but especially from the structuralist Marxism of Louis Althusser and others. The main focus of an orthodox Marxist approach concentrates on the production of meaning. This model assumes a mass production of culture and identifies power as residing with those producing cultural artifacts. In a Marxist view, those who control the means of production (the economic base) essentially control a culture.[citation needed] Other approaches to cultural studies, such as feminist cultural studies and later American developments of the field, distance themselves from this view. They criticize the Marxist assumption of a single, dominant meaning, shared by all, for any cultural product. The non-Marxist approaches suggest that different ways of consuming cultural artifacts affect the meaning of the product. This view is best exemplified by the book Doing Cultural Studies: The Case of the Sony Walkman (by Paul du Gay et al.), which seeks to challenge the notion that those who produce commodities control the meanings that people attribute to them. Feminist cultural analyst, theorist and art historian Griselda Pollock contributed to cultural studies from viewpoints of art history and psychoanalysis. The writer Julia Kristeva is influential voices in the turn of the century, contributing to cultural studies from the field of art and psychoanalytical French feminism.[citation needed]

Cultural change

A 19th century engraving showing Australian «natives» opposing the arrival of Captain James Cook in 1770

Cultural invention has come to mean any innovation that is new and found to be useful to a group of people and expressed in their behavior but which does not exist as a physical object. Humanity is in a global «accelerating culture change period», driven by the expansion of international commerce, the mass media, and above all, the human population explosion, among other factors.

Cultures are internally affected by both forces encouraging change and forces resisting change. These forces are related to both social structures and natural events, and are involved in the perpetuation of cultural ideas and practices within current structures, which themselves are subject to change.[187] (See structuration.)

Social conflict and the development of technologies can produce changes within a society by altering social dynamics and promoting new cultural models, and spurring or enabling generative action. These social shifts may accompany ideological shifts and other types of cultural change. For example, the U.S. feminist movement involved new practices that produced a shift in gender relations, altering both gender and economic structures. Environmental conditions may also enter as factors. For example, after tropical forests returned at the end of the last ice age, plants suitable for domestication were available, leading to the invention of agriculture, which in turn brought about many cultural innovations and shifts in social dynamics.[188]

Full-length profile portrait of Turkman woman, standing on a carpet at the entrance to a yurt, dressed in traditional clothing and jewelry

Cultures are externally affected via contact between societies, which may also produce—or inhibit—social shifts and changes in cultural practices. War or competition over resources may impact technological development or social dynamics. Additionally, cultural ideas may transfer from one society to another, through diffusion or acculturation. In diffusion, the form of something (though not necessarily its meaning) moves from one culture to another. For example, hamburgers, mundane in the United States, seemed exotic when introduced into China. «Stimulus diffusion» (the sharing of ideas) refers to an element of one culture leading to an invention or propagation in another. «Direct Borrowing» on the other hand tends to refer to technological or tangible diffusion from one culture to another. Diffusion of innovations theory presents a research-based model of why and when individuals and cultures adopt new ideas, practices, and products.

Acculturation has different meanings, but in this context refers to replacement of the traits of one culture with those of another, such has happened to certain Native American tribes and to many indigenous peoples across the globe during the process of colonization. Related processes on an individual level include assimilation (adoption of a different culture by an individual) and transculturation.

See also

  • Counterculture
  • Creative Culture
  • Cross-cultural communication – Intercultural competence
  • Cultural bias – Cultural imperialism – Ethnocentrism
  • Cultural dissonance
  • Cultural Institutions Studies
  • Culture theory
  • Culture war
  • Interculturality
  • Outline of culture
  • Sociocultural evolution
  • Urban culture

Notes

  1. ^ Harper, Douglas (2001). Online Etymology Dictionary
  2. ^ Kroeber, A. L. and C. Kluckhohn, 1952. Culture: A Critical Review of Concepts and Definitions.
  3. ^ Levine, Donald (ed) ‘Simmel: On individuality and social forms’ Chicago University Press, 1971. p6.
  4. ^ a b Velkley, Richard (2002). «The Tension in the Beautiful: On Culture and Civilization in Rousseau and German Philosophy». Being after Rousseau: Philosophy and Culture in Question. The University of Chicago Press. pp. 11–30
  5. ^ Immanuel Kant 1974 «Answering the Question: What is Enlightenment?» (German: «Beantwortung der Frage: Was ist Aufklärung?») Berlinische Monatsschrift, December (Berlin Monthly)
  6. ^ Michael Eldridge, «The German Bildung Tradition» UNC Charlotte
  7. ^ «Adolf Bastian», Today in Science History; «Adolf Bastian», Encyclopædia Britannica
  8. ^ a b Arnold, Matthew. 1869. Culture and Anarchy.
  9. ^ Williams (1983), p.90. Cited in Shuker, Roy (1994). Understanding Popular Music, p.5. ISBN 0-415-10723-7. argues that contemporary definitions of culture fall into three possibilities or mixture of the following three:
    • «a general process of intellectual, spiritual, and aesthetic development»
    • «a particular way of life, whether of a people, period, or a group»
    • «the works and practices of intellectual and especially artistic activity».
  10. ^ Bakhtin 1981, p.4
  11. ^ McClenon, p.528-529
  12. ^ Robert Yerkes 1943 Chimpanzees: A Laboratory Colony. New Haven: Yale University Press. 51–52, 189, 193
  13. ^ Jane Goodall 1963 «My Life Among Wild Chimpanzees» National Geographic 124: 308
  14. ^ R. J. Andrew 1963 «Comment on The Essential Morphological Basis for Human Culture» Alan Bryan Current Anthropology 4: 301–303, p. 301
  15. ^ Alan Bryan 1963 «The Essential Morphological basis for Human Culture» Current Anthropology 4: 297
  16. ^ Keleman 1963 «Comment on The Essential Morphological Basis for Human Culture» Alan Bryan Current Anthropology 4: 301–303 p.304
  17. ^ W. C. McGrew 1998 «Culture in nonhuman primates?» Annual Review of Anthropology 27: 301–328
  18. ^ a b W.C. McGrew 1998 «Culture in Nonhuman Primates?» Annual Review of Anthropology 27: 323
  19. ^ W.C. McGrew 1998 «Culture in Nonhuman Primates?» Annual Review of Anthropology 27: 305
  20. ^ C.F. Voegelin 1951 «Culture, Language and the Human Organism» Southwestern Journal of Anthropology 7: 370
  21. ^ a b Michael Tomasello 1999 «The Human Adaptation for Culture» in Annual Review of Anthropology vol. 28: 511
  22. ^ M. King and A Wilson 1975 «Evolution at two levels: in humans and chimpanzees» Science 188: 107–116
  23. ^ Stringer and McKiew 1996 African Exodus: The origins of Modern Humanity. London: Cape
  24. ^ a b Michael Tomasello 1999 «The Human Adaptation for Culture» in Annual Review of Anthropology vol. 28: 510
  25. ^ Michael Tomasello 1999 «The Human Adaptation for Culture» in Annual Review of Anthropology vol. 28: 512
  26. ^ Michael Tomasello 1999 «The Human Adaptation for Culture» in Annual Review of Anthropology vol. 28: 520
  27. ^ a b Michael Tomasello 1990 «Cultural Transmission in the Tool Use and Communicatory Signaling of Chimpanzees?» in «Language» and Intelligence in Monkeys and Apes: Comparative Developmental Perspectives ed. S. Parker, K. Bibson. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 274–311
  28. ^ Michael Tmoasello 1996 «Do Apes Ape?» in Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture ed. C. Heyes and B. Galef. New York: Academic Press, pp. 319–346
  29. ^ a b Nagell, K., Olguin K. and Tomasello M. 1993 «Processes of social learning in the tool use of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens)» in Journal of Comparative Psychology 107: 174–186
  30. ^ S. Kawamura 1959 «The process of subcultural propogation among Japanese macaques» Primates 2: 43–60
  31. ^ a b M. Kawai 1965 «Newly acquired pre-cultural behairo of the natural troop of Japanese monkeys on Koshima Islet Primates 6: 1–30
  32. ^ E. Visalberghi and D.M. Fragaszy 1990 «Food washing behavior in tufted Capuchin monkeys, Cebusapella, and crabeating macaques, Macaca fasciculais» Animal Behavior 40: 829–836
  33. ^ Michael Tomasello 1999 «The Human Adaptation for Culture» in Annual Review of Anthropology vol. 28: 519
  34. ^ Michael Tomasello 1996 «Do Apes Ape?» in Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture ed. C. Heyes and B. Galef. New York: Academic Press. pp: 319–346
  35. ^ Michael Tomasello 1999 «The Human Adaptation for Culture» in Annual Review of Anthropology vol. 28: 512
  36. ^ Michael Tomasello 1999 «The Human Adaptation for Culture» in Annual Review of Anthropology vol. 28: 513
  37. ^ R. Bakerman and L. Adamson 1984 «Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother-infant and peer-infant interaction» in Child Development 55: 1278–1289
  38. ^ C. Moore and P. Dunham 1995 Joint Attention: Its Origins and Role in Development. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Press.
  39. ^ M. Tomasello 1995 «Joint attention as social cognition» in Joint Attention: Its Origins and Role in Development, ed. C. Moore and P. Dunham. Hillsdale NJ: Erlbaum Press, pp. 103–130
  40. ^ W. C. Meltzoff 1988 «Infant imitation after a one-week delay: long term memory for novel acts and multiple stimuli» in Developmental Psychology 24: 470–476
  41. ^ M. Carpenter, N. Akhtar, M. Tomasello 1998 «Sixteen-month old infants differentially imitate intentional and accidental actions» in Infant behavioral Development 21: 315–330
  42. ^ A. Meltzoff 1995 «Understanding the intentions of others: re-enactment of intended acts by 18-month-old children» in Developmental Psychology 31: 838–850
  43. ^ Michael Tomasello 1999 «The Human Adaptation for Culture» in Annual Review of Anthropology vol. 28: 514
  44. ^ Michael Tomasello 1999 «The Human Adaptation for Culture» in Annual Review of Anthropology vol. 28: 515
  45. ^ Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. & Ratner, H. 1993 «Cultural learning» Behavioral and Brain Sciences 16, 495–552
  46. ^ Michael Tomasello 1999 «The Human Adaptation for Culture» in Annual Review of Anthropology vol. 28: 516
  47. ^ Michael Tomasello 1999 «The Human Adaptation for Culture» in Annual Review of Anthropology vol. 28: 520–521
  48. ^ Brown 1999
  49. ^ M. Tomasello and M. Barton 1994 «Learning Words in non-Ostensive Contexts» Developmental Psychology 30: 639–650
  50. ^ N. Akhtar and M. Tomasello 1996 «Twenty-four month old children learn words for absent objects and actions» in British Journal of Developmental Psychology 14: 79–93
  51. ^ M. Tomasello R. Strosberg, N. Akhtar 1996 «Eighteen-month old children learn words in non-ostensive contexts» in Journal of Child Language 23: 157–176
  52. ^ Michael Tomasello 1999 «The Human Adaptation for Culture» in Annual Review of Anthropology vol. 28: 516
  53. ^ Ralph L. Holloway Jr. 1969 «Culture: A Human domain» in Current Anthropology 10(4): 395
  54. ^ Ralph L. Holloway Jr. 1969 «Culture: A Human domain» in Current Anthropology 10(4): 396
  55. ^ Ralph L. Holloway Jr. 1969 «Culture: A Human domain» in Current Anthropology 10(4): 400
  56. ^ Ralph L. Holloway Jr. 1969 «Culture: A Human domain» in Current Anthropology 10(4): 401.
  57. ^ Ralph L. Holloway Jr. 1969 «Culture: A Human domain» in Current Anthropology 10(4): 399.
  58. ^ Ralph L. Holloway Jr. 1969 «Culture: A Human domain» in Current Anthropology 10(4): 395.
  59. ^ Ralph L. Holloway Jr. 1969 «Culture: A Human domain» in Current Anthropology 10(4): 399.
  60. ^ C.F. Hockett and R. Ascher 1964 «The Human Revolution» in Current Anthropology 4: 135–168.
  61. ^ Michael Tomasello 1999 «The Human Adaptation for Culture» in Annual Review of Anthropology vol. 28: 517.
  62. ^ Ralph L. Holloway Jr. 1969 «Culture: A Human domain» in Current Anthropology 10(4): 401
  63. ^ Ralph L. Holloway Jr. 1969 «Culture: A Human domain» in Current Anthropology 10(4): 402
  64. ^ Ralph L. Holloway Jr. 1969 «Culture: A Human domain» in Current Anthropology 10(4): 406.
  65. ^ Terrence Deacon 1997 The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain New York and London: W. W. Norton, p. 322
  66. ^ Terrence Deacon 1997 The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain New York and London: W.W. Norton, p. 344
  67. ^ Terrence Deacon 1997 The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain New York and London: W.W. Norton, p. 340
  68. ^ Terrence Deacon 1997 The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain New York and London: W.W. Norton, p. 347
  69. ^ Terrence Deacon 1997 The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain New York and London: W.W. Norton, pp. 384–385
  70. ^ Terrence Deacon 1997 The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain New York and London: W. W. Norton, p. 386.
  71. ^ Terrence Deacon 1997 The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain New York and London: W.W. Norton, pp. 386–387
  72. ^ Terrence Deacon 1997 The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain New York and London: W.W. Norton, p. 388
  73. ^ Terrence Deacon 1997 The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain New York and London: W.W. Norton, pp. 396–397
  74. ^ Terrence Deacon 1997 The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain New York and London: W.W. Norton, pp. 397–401
  75. ^ V. Gordon Childe 1929 The Danube in Prehistory Oxford: Clarendon Press
  76. ^ By R. Lee Lyman and Michael J. O’Brien, 2003 W.C. McKern and the Midwestern Taxonomic Method. University of Alabama Press, Tuscaloosa
  77. ^ Colin Renfrew and Paul Bahn, 2008 Archeology: Theories, Methods and Practice Fifth edition. New York: Thames and Hudson. p. 470
  78. ^ Walter Taylor 1948 A Study of Archeology Memoir 69, American Anthropological Association, reprinted, Carbondale Il: Southern Illinois University Press, 1967. p. 96
  79. ^ Walter Taylor 1948 A Study of Archeology Memoir 69, American Anthropological Association, reprinted, Carbondale Il: Southern Illinois University Press, 1967. p. 100
  80. ^ Patty Jo Watson 1995 «Archeology, Anthropology, and the Culture Concept» in American Anthropologist 97(4) p.685
  81. ^ Robert Dunnel 1986 «Five Decades of American Archeology» in American Archeology past and Future: A Celebration of the Society for American Archeology, 1935–1985. D. Meltzer and J. Sabloff, eds. Washington D.C.: Smithsonian Institution Press. p.36
  82. ^ Lewish Binford 1962 «Archeology as Anthropology» in American Antiquity 28(2):218; see White 1959 The Evolution of Culture New York:McGraw Hill p.8
  83. ^ Lewish Binford 1962 «Archeology as Anthropology» in American Antiquity 28(2):218; see Steward 1955 Theory of Culture Change. University of Illinois Press, Urbana.
  84. ^ Patty Jo Watson 1995 «Archeology, Anthropology, and the Culture Concept» in American Anthropologist 97(4) p.684
  85. ^ Patty Jo Watson 1995 «Archeology, Anthropology, and the Culture Concept» in American Anthropologist 97(4) pp. 687–6874
  86. ^ Ian Hodder 1982 Symbols in action: ethnoarchaeological studies of material culture Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
  87. ^ Ian Hodder 1986 Reading the past: current approaches to interpretation in archaeology Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
  88. ^ Baepler, Paul. 2003. «White slaves, African masters.» The ANNALS of the American Academy of Political and Social Science 588(1): 90–111. p. 91
  89. ^ Quoted in Anderson, Benedict R.O’G. 1983. Imagined Communities: Reflections on the Origin and Spread of Nationalism. London: Verso.
  90. ^ Sapir 1921:228
  91. ^ Sapir 1995: 59
  92. ^ e.g. Von Humbolt, Wilhelm. 1820. Über das vergleichende Sprachstudium in Beziehung auf die verschiedenen Epochen der Sprachentwicklung.
  93. ^ e.g. Whorf, Benjamin Lee. 1941. «The relation of habitual thought and behavior to language.» In Language, Culture, and Personality: Essays in Honor of Edward Sapir. Menasha, WI: Sapir Memorial Publication Fund.
  94. ^ Whorf, Benjamin Lee. 1941. «The relation of habitual thought and behavior to language.» In Language, Culture, and Personality: Essays in Honor of Edward Sapir. Menasha, WI: Sapir Memorial Publication Fund. p. 293. See also, e.g. Boas, Franz. 1911. «Introduction.» Handbook of American Indian Languages. Washington: Smithsonian Institution, Bureau of American Ethnology.
  95. ^ (Duranti 1997: 49)(Mannheim & Tedlock 1995: 2)
  96. ^ Clancy, Patricia. (1986) «The acquisition of communicative style in Japanese.» In B. Schieffelin and E. Ochs (eds) Language Socialization across Cultures. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
  97. ^ a b Foley 1997 p??
  98. ^ Men’s and Women’s Speech in Koasati, Mary R. Haas, Language, Vol. 20, No. 3 (Jul. – Sep., 1944), pp. 142–149 (also summarized in Foley 1997)
  99. ^ Tylor, E.B. 1874. Primitive culture: researches into the development of mythology, philosophy, religion, art, and custom.
  100. ^ A. L. Kroeber 1917 «The Superorganic» American Anthropologist, New Series, Vol. 19, No. 2 pp. 163–213.
  101. ^ Gerald Weiss 1973 «A Scientific Concept of Culture» in American Anthropologist 75(5): 1382
  102. ^ Franz Boas 1940 [1920] «The Methods of Ethnology», in Race, Language and Culture ed. George Stocking. Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 284.
  103. ^ Franz Boas 1940 [1932] «The Aims of Anthropological Research», in Race, Language and Culture ed. George Stocking. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 253
  104. ^ Kroeber, Alfred L., 1948, Anthropology: Race, Language, Culture, Psychology, Prehistory revised edition. New York: Harcourt, Brace, and World, Inc. p. 261
  105. ^ Franz Boas 1907 «Anthropology» in A Franz Boas Reader: The Shaping of American Anthropology 1883–1911 ed. George Stocking Jr. 267–382
  106. ^ Boas, Franz 1920 «The Methods of Ethnology» in Race, Language, and Culture. ed. George Stocking Jr. 1940 Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. pp. 281–289
  107. ^ Boas, Franz 1909 «Decorative Designs in Alaskan Needlecases: A Study in the History of Conventional designs Based on Materials in the U.S. National Museum» in Race, Language, and Culture. ed. George Stocking Jr. 1940 Chicago: The University of Chicago Press. pp. 564–592
  108. ^ Wissler, Clark (ed.) (1975) Societies of the Plains Indians AMS Press, New York, ISBN 0-404-11918-2 , Reprint of v. 11 of Anthropological papers of the American Museum of Natural History, published in 13 pts. from 1912 to 1916.
  109. ^ Kroeber, Alfred L. (1939) Cultural and Natural Areas of Native North America University of California Press, Berkeley, CA.
  110. ^ Dyk, Walter 1938 Left Handed, Son of Old Man Hat, by Walter Dyk. Lincoln: The University of Nebraska Press.
  111. ^ Lewis, Oscar 1961 The Children of Sanchez. New York: Vintage Books.
  112. ^ Lewis, Oscar 1964 Pedro Martinez. New York: Random House.
  113. ^ Mintz, Sidney 1960 Worker in the Cane: A Puerto Rican Life History. Yale Caribbean Series, vol. 2. New Haven: Yale University Press.
  114. ^ Radin, Paul 1913 «Personal Reminiscences of a Winnebago Indian,» in Journal of American Folklore 26: 293–318
  115. ^ Radin, Paul 1963 The Autobiography of a Winnebago Indian. New York: Dover Publications
  116. ^ Sapir, Edward 1922 «Sayach’apis, a Nootka Trader» in Elsie Clews Parsons, American Indian Life. New York: B.W. Huebesh.
  117. ^ Simmons, Leo, ed. 1942 Sun Chief: The Autobiography of a Hopi Indian. New Haven: Yale University Press.
  118. ^ Benedict 1934.
  119. ^ Benedict, Ruth. The Chrysanthemum and the Sword: Patterns of Japanese Culture. Rutland, VT and Tokyo, Japan: Charles E. Tuttle Co. 1954 orig. 1946.
  120. ^ Margaret Mead 1928 Coming of Age in Samoa
  121. ^ Benedict 1934 pp.46–47
  122. ^ Franz Boas 1940 [1932] «The Aims of Anthropological Research,» in Race, Language and Culture ed. George Stocking. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 256
  123. ^ Bashkow, Ira 2004 «A Neo-Boasian Conception of Cultural Boundaries» American Anthropologist 106(3): 446
  124. ^ Benedict 1934 p. 48
  125. ^ Benedict 1934 p. 49
  126. ^ Ruth Benedict 1934 Patterns of Culture Boston: Houghton Miflin Company p. 52
  127. ^ Benedict 1934 p. 47
  128. ^ Benedict 1934 pp. 23–24
  129. ^ Benedict 1934 p.46
  130. ^ a b Benedict 1934 p.277
  131. ^ Benedict 1934 p.271
  132. ^ Murdock, George, 1949 Social Structure New York: The Macmillan Company
  133. ^ Murdock, G. P. 1967. Ethnographic Atlas: A Summary. Pittsburgh: The University of Pittsburgh Press
  134. ^ Murdock, G. P. 1981. Atlas of World Cultures. Pittsburgh: The University of Pittsburgh Press.
  135. ^ Lévi-Strauss, Claude 1955 Tristes Tropiques Atheneum press
  136. ^ Lévi-Strauss, Claude Mythologiques I-IV (trans. John Weightman and Doreen Weightman);Le Cru et le cuit (1964), The Raw and the Cooked (1969); Du miel aux cendres (1966), From Honey to Ashes (1973); L’Origine des manières de table (1968) The Origin of Table Manners 1978); ‘L’Homme nu (1971) The Naked Man (1981)
  137. ^ Raymond Firth 1936 We the Tikopia: A Sociological Study of Kinship in Primitive Polynesia London Allen and Unwin
  138. ^ Meyer Fortes and E.E. Evans Pritchard 1940. African Political Systems. London and New York: International African Institute.
  139. ^ Bronisław Malinowski 1944 The Scientific Theory of Culture
  140. ^ A.R. Radcliffe-Brown 1952 Structure and Function in Primitive Society
  141. ^ a.R. Radcliffe-Brown 1957 A Natural Science of Society Glencoe: The Free Press p. 53
  142. ^ Talcott Parsons 1937, The Structure of Social Action
  143. ^ Talcott Parsons 1951, The Social System
  144. ^ Eric Wolf 1980 «They Divide and Subdivide and Call it Anthropology.» The New York Times November 30:E9.
  145. ^ Ira Bashkow, 2004 «A Neo-Boasian Conception of Cultural Boundaries,» American Anthropologist 106(3):445–446
  146. ^ Appadurai, Arjun 1986 The Social Life of Things. (Edited) New York: Cambridge University Press.
  147. ^ Appadurai, Arjun, 1996 Modernity at large: Cultural dimensions of globalization. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press.
  148. ^ Gupta, Akhil, and James Ferguson, 1992, «Beyond ‘Culture’: Space, Identity, and the Politics of Difference,» Cultural Anthropology 7(1): 6–23
  149. ^ Marcus, George E. 1995 «Ethnography in/of the World System: The Emergence of Multi-Sited Ethnography.» In Annual Review of Anthropology 24: 95–117
  150. ^ Wolf, Eric 1982 Europe and the people without history. Berkeley: The University of California Press.
  151. ^ Clifford Geertz 1973 The Interpretation of Cultures New York: Basic Books
  152. ^ David Schneider 1968 American Kinship: A Cultural Account Chicago: University of Chicago press
  153. ^ Roy Wagner 1980 American Kinship: A Cultural Account Chicago: University of Chicago Press
  154. ^ Janet Dolgin, David Kemnitzer, and David Schneider, eds. Symbolic Anthropology: a Reader in the Study of Symbols and Meanings
  155. ^ Victor Turner 1967 The Forest of Symbols: Aspects of Ndembu Ritual Ithaca:Cornell University Press
  156. ^ White, L. 1949. The Science of Culture: A study of man and civilization.
  157. ^ Richard A. Barrett 1989, «The Paradoxical Anthropology of Leslie White,» American Anthropologist Vol. 91, No. 4 (Dec., 1989), pp. 986–999
  158. ^ Leslie White, 1949 «Ethnological Theory.» In Philosophy for the Future: The Quest of Modern Materialism. R. W. Sellars, V.J. McGill, and M. Farber, eds. Pp. 357–384. New York: Macmillan.
  159. ^ Leslie White, 1943 «Energy and the Evolution of Culture.» American Anthropologist 45: 339
  160. ^ Julian Steward 1938 Basin Plateau Aboriginal Socio-political Groups (Smithsonian Institution Bureau of American Ethnology Bulletin, No 20)
  161. ^ Julian Steward 1955 Theory of Culture Change: The Methodology of Multilinear Evolution University of Illinois Press
  162. ^ Marvin Harris 1979 Cultural Materialism: The Struggle for a Science of Culture New York: Random House
  163. ^ Marvin Harris 1977 Cannibals and Kings: Origins of Cultures New York: Vintage
  164. ^ Marvin Harris 1974 Cows, Pigs, Wars, and Witches: The Riddles of Culture New York: Vintage
  165. ^ Roy A. Rappaport 1967 Pigs for the Ancestors: Ritual in the Ecology of a New Guinea People
  166. ^ Julian Steward, ed. 1966 The people of Puerto Rico: a study in social anthropology Chicago: University of Chicago Press (includes doctoral dissertations of Mintz and Wolf)
  167. ^ Robert F. Murphy 1960 Headhunter’s Heritage; Social and Economic Change Among the Mundurucu Indians
  168. ^ Marshall Sahlins and Elman Service, ‘
  169. ^ Elman R. Service 1962 Primitive social organization: an evolutionary perspective New York: Random House
  170. ^ Andrew Peter Vayda, ed. 1969 Environment and cultural behavior: ecological studies in cultural anthropology Garden City: Natural History Press
  171. ^ Dell Hymes 1969 Reinventing Anthropology p. 11
  172. ^ Dell Hymes 1969 Reinventing Anthropology p. 42
  173. ^ Dell Hymes 1969 Reinventing Anthropology p. 43
  174. ^ Sidney Mintz 1985 Sweetness and Power New York:Viking Press
  175. ^ Robert Murphy 1971 The Dialectics of Social Life New York: Basic Books
  176. ^ Marshall Sahlins 1976 Culture and Practical Reason Chicago: University of Chicago Press
  177. ^ Eric Wolf 1971 Peasant Wars of the Twentieth Century
  178. ^ Eric Wolf, 1982 Europe and the People Without History Berkeley: University of California Press
  179. ^ Mishkin, Bernard 1940 Rank and Warfare in Plains Indian Culture. Monographs of the American Ethnological Society no. 3. New York: J.J. Augustin.
  180. ^ Lewis, Oscar 1942 The Effects of White Contact upon Blackfoot Culture, with Special Reference to the Role of the Fur Trade. Monographs of the American Ethnological Society no. 6. New York: J.J. Augustin.
  181. ^ Jablow, Joseph 1951 The Cheyenne in Plains Indian Trade Relations, 1795–1972. American Ethnological Society Monograph 19. New York: J.J. Augustin.
  182. ^ Secoy, Frank 1953 Changing Military Patterns on the Great Plains(17th Century througn Early 19th Century). American Ethnological Society Monograph 21, New York: J.J. Augustin.
  183. ^ Raymond Williams (1976) Keywords: A Vocabulary of Culture and Society. Rev. Ed. (NewYork: Oxford UP, 1983), pp. 87–93 and 236–8.
  184. ^ John Berger, Peter Smith Pub. Inc., (1971) Ways of Seeing
  185. ^ Bakhtin, Mikhail 1981. The Dialogic Imagination. Austin, TX: UT Press, p.4
  186. ^ Lindlof & Taylor, 2002,p.60
  187. ^ O’Neil, D. 2006. «Processes of Change».
  188. ^ Pringle, H. 1998. The Slow Birth of Agriculture. Science 282: 1446.

References

  • «Adolf Bastian». Today in Science History. 27 Jan 2009 Today in Science History
  • «Adolf Bastian», Encyclopædia Britannica Online, 27 January 2009
  • Ankerl, Guy (2000) [2000]. Global communication without universal civilization, vol.1: Coexisting contemporary civilizations: Arabo-Muslim, Bharati, Chinese, and Western. INU societal research. Geneva: INU Press. ISBN 2-88155-004-5.
  • Arnold, Matthew. 1869. Culture and Anarchy. New York: Macmillan. Third edition, 1882, available online. Retrieved: 2006-06-28.
  • Bakhtin, M. M. (1981) The Dialogic Imagination: Four Essays. Ed. Michael Holquist. Trans. Caryl Press. ISBN 978-0-252-06445-6.
  • Barzilai, Gad. 2003. Communities and Law: Politics and Cultures of Legal Identities University of Michigan Press. ISBN 0-472-11315-1
  • Benedict, Ruth (1934). Patterns of Culture. Boston: Houghton Miflin Company
  • Bourdieu, Pierre. 1977. Outline of a Theory of Practice. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0-521-29164-4
  • Cohen, Anthony P. 1985. The Symbolic Construction of Community. Routledge: New York,
  • Dawkins, R. 1982. The Extended Phenotype: The Long Reach of the Gene. Paperback ed., 1999. Oxford Paperbacks. ISBN 978-0-19-288051-2
  • Findley & Rothney. Twentieth-Century World (Houghton Mifflin, 1986)
  • Forsberg, A. Definitions of culture CCSF Cultural Geography course notes. Retrieved: 2006-06-29.
  • Geertz, Clifford. 1973. The Interpretation of Cultures: Selected Essays. New York. ISBN 978-0-465-09719-7.
  • — 1957. «Ritual and Social Change: A Javanese Example», American Anthropologist, Vol. 59, No. 1. Geertz
  • Goodall, J. 1986. The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. ISBN 978-0-674-11649-8
  • Hoult, T. F., ed. 1969. Dictionary of Modern Sociology. Totowa, New Jersey, United States: Littlefield, Adams & Co.
  • Jary, D. and J. Jary. 1991. The HarperCollins Dictionary of Sociology. New York: HarperCollins. ISBN 0-06-271543-7
  • Keiser, R. Lincoln 1969. The Vice Lords: Warriors of the Streets. Holt, Rinehart, and Winston. ISBN 978-0-03-080361-1.
  • Kroeber, A. L. and C. Kluckhohn, 1952. Culture: A Critical Review of Concepts and Definitions. Cambridge, MA: Peabody Museum
  • Kim, Uichol (2001). «Culture, science and indigenous psychologies: An integrated analysis.» In D. Matsumoto (Ed.), Handbook of culture and psychology. Oxford: Oxford University Press
  • McClenon, James. «Tylor, Edward B(urnett)». Encyclopedia of Religion and Society. Ed. William Swatos and Peter Kivisto. Walnut Creek: AltaMira, 1998. 528-29.
  • Middleton, R. 1990. Studying Popular Music. Philadelphia: Open University Press. ISBN 978-0-335-15275-9.
  • O’Neil, D. 2006. Cultural Anthropology Tutorials, Behavioral Sciences Department, Palomar College, San Marco, California. Retrieved: 2006-07-10.
  • Reagan, Ronald. «Final Radio Address to the Nation», January 14, 1989. Retrieved June 3, 2006.
  • Reese, W.L. 1980. Dictionary of Philosophy and Religion: Eastern and Western Thought. New Jersey U.S., Sussex, U.K: Humanities Press.
  • Tylor, E.B. 1974. Primitive culture: researches into the development of mythology, philosophy, religion, art, and custom. New York: Gordon Press. First published in 1871. ISBN 978-0-87968-091-6
  • UNESCO. 2002. Universal Declaration on Cultural Diversity, issued on International Mother Language Day, February 21, 2002. Retrieved: 2006-06-23.
  • White, L. 1949. The Science of Culture: A study of man and civilization. New York: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
  • Wilson, Edward O. (1998). Consilience: The Unity of Knowledge. Vintage: New York. ISBN 978-0-679-76867-8.
  • Wolfram, Stephen. 2002 A New Kind of Science. Wolfram Media, Inc. ISBN 978-1-57955-008-0

External links

  • Centre for Intercultural Learning
  • Detailed article on defining culture
  • Dictionary of the History of Ideas «culture» and «civilization» in modern times
  • Global Culture Essays on global issues and their impact on culture
  • Centre for cultural technologies
  • Reflections on the Politics of Culture by Michael Parenti
  • What is Culture? – Washington State University
  • Define Culture Compilation of 100+ user submitted definitions of culture from around the globe
  • Concepts of Culture in Cross-National and Cross-Cultural Perspectives
 Links to related articles
v · d · eArticles of the Universal Declaration of Human Rights
 General principles

Article 1: Freedom, Egalitarianism, Dignity and Brotherhood
Article 2: Universality of rights

 International Covenant on Civil and Political Rights

Article 1 and 2: Right to freedom from discrimination · Article 3: Right to life, liberty and security of person · Article 4: Freedom from slavery · Article 5: Freedom from torture and cruel and unusual punishment · Article 6: Right to personhood · Article 7: Equality before the law · Article 8: Right to effective remedy from the law · Article 9: Freedom from arbitrary arrest, detention and exile · Article 10: Right to a fair trial · Article 11.1: Presumption of innocence · Article 11.2: Prohibition of retrospective law · Article 12: Right to privacy · Article 13: Freedom of movement · Article 14: Right of asylum · Article 15: Right to a nationality · Article 16: Right to marriage and family life · Article 17: Right to property · Article 18: Freedom of thought, conscience and religion · Article 19: Freedom of opinion and expression · Article 20.1: Freedom of assembly · Article 20.2: Freedom of association · Article 21.1: Right to participation in government · Article 21.2: Right of equal access to public office · Article 21.3: Right to universal suffrage

 International Covenant on Economic, Social and Cultural Rights

Article 1 and 2: Right to freedom from discrimination · Article 22: Right to social security · Article 23.1: Right to work · Article 23.2: Right to equal pay for equal work · Article 23.3: Right to just remuneration · Article 23.4: Right to join a trade union · Article 24: Right to rest and leisure · Article 25.1: Right to an adequate standard of living · Article 25.2: Right to special care and assistance for mothers and children · Article 26.1: Right to education · Article 26.2: Human rights education · Article 26.3: Right to choice of education · Article 27: Right to science and culture ·

 Context, limitations and duties

Article 28: Social order · Article 29.1: Social responsibility  · Article 29.2: Limitations of human rights · Article 29.3: The supremacy of the purposes and principles of the United Nations
Article 30: Nothing in this Declaration may be interpreted as implying for any State, group or person any right to engage in any activity or to perform any act aimed at the destruction of any of the rights and freedoms set forth herein.

Category:Human rights · Human rights portal

human rights

Note: What is considered a human right is controversial and not all the topics listed are universally accepted as human rights.

Civil and political

Freedom from discrimination · Right to life · Right to die · Security of person · Liberty · Freedom of movement · Freedom from slavery · Personhood · Right to bear arms · Right to equality before the law · Freedom from arbitrary arrest and detention · Freedom from torture · Freedom from cruel and unusual punishment · Right to a fair trial · Presumption of innocence · Right of asylum · Nationality · Freedom from exile · Privacy · Freedom of thought and conscience · Freedom of religion · Freedom of expression · Freedom of information · Freedom of assembly · Freedom of association · Right to protest · Universal suffrage · Marriage · Family life · Same-sex sexual relations

Economic, social
and cultural

Labor rights · Fair remuneration · Equal pay for equal work · Trade union membership · Right to social security · Right to rest and leisure · Right to work · Right to property · Right to science and culture · Right to public participation · Right to education · Right to adequate standard of living · Right to housing · Right to development · Right to health · Right to healthcare · Right to water · Right to food · Right of return · Right to Internet access

Reproductive

Family planning · Reproductive health · Abortion · Freedom from involuntary female genital mutilation

War and conflict

Civilian · Combatant · Freedom from genocide · Prisoner of war · War rape

Related philosophy

Negative and positive rights · Claim rights and liberty rights · Freedom versus license · Desert claim · Social contract · Meritocracy · Equality before the law

Like this post? Please share to your friends:
  • The word culture in all languages
  • The word culture has many different meanings
  • The word culture comes from
  • The word colour in spanish
  • The word collector book