The syllable structure of the word

3

The
Syllabic Structure of English Words

Phonemes
are rarely pronounced in isolation, they usually occur in sequences.
Sound sequences are broken up into smaller units known as syllables,
which are the minimal units of sounding speech.

A
syllable may consist of one or a number of phonemes, i.e. it may be
formed by any vowel (alone or in combination with consonants) or by a
word-final sonorant preceded by a consonant. A speech sound which is
capable of forming a syllable is called syllabic. It is the most
sonorous sound in the syllable and makes up the peak of prominence
(the centre of the syllable, the nucleus). Speech sounds which are
not capable of forming syllables are called non-syllabic. The
consonants which precede the peak are called the onset and those
which follow it are called the coda. J.Kenyon defines the syllable as
“one or more speech sounds forming a single uninterrupted unit of
utterance, which may be a word (e.g. man), or a commonly recognized
subdivision of a word (e.g. [ɪƞ-glɪʃ]) or wordform (e.g.
[leɪ-tǝ].”

The
syllabic structure of words may be graphically represented by the
letter V standing for a vowel and the letter C standing for a
consonant. The syllabic sonorant is represented by S.

Every
syllable has a definite structure. A syllable which begins in a
consonant is called covered, a syllable which begins in a vowel is
called uncovered. A syllable which ends in a consonant is called
closed, a syllable which ends in a vowel is called open. There are
four
main types of syllables
:

V
– uncovered, open, or fully open, e.g. or [o:], I [aɪ];

VC
– uncovered, closed, e.g. it [ɪt], add [æd];

CV
– covered, closed, e.g. see [si:], no[nou];

CVC
– covered, closed, or fully closed, e.g. catch [kæʧ], pit [pɪt].

In
English the typical and the most fundamental syllabic structure is of
CVC type, in Russian CV types are more common than VC types.

There
are a great number of variants in the syllabic structure which are
formed by increasing the number of consonants in the initial and
final positions, as in:

VCC
(and, eggs, oaks), VCCC (ends, acts), CCV (blue, grow), CCCV (spray,
stray), CVCC (cats, bolt), CVCCC (facts, minds), CCVC (sleep,
shrewd), CCVCC (stoves, flex), CCVCCC (clasps, sphinx), CCCVC
(street, splash), CCCVCC (splint, splashed), CCCVCCC (splints).

English
and Russian have almost an equal number of syllabic models: 23 vs 21.
Though the same structures are used in both languages, their
frequency of occurrence is different. Some of them are extremely rare
in Russian, while in English they are in common use. A specific
English feature is that /l,m,n,r/ may become syllabic after a
consonant. The types of syllabic structures formed by sonorants: S:
[æp-l], [bʌt-n]; CS: [teɪ-bl], [ga:-dn]; CSC: [neɪ-ʃnz],
[ou-pnz], SC: [dɪd-nt], [plez-nt]. In Russian sonorants are
non-syllabic.

As
to the number of consonants before the peak of the syllable, the
maximum number in English is three: /s/+/p,t,k/+/r,l,w,j/, e.g.
street, squash, splash.

Russian
enjoys greater freedom of combinability in initial three-consonant
clusters, while in a four-consonant cluster the first sound must be
/в/: встряхнуть, всплакнуть вскрикнуть.
Initial consonant clusters in Russian represent grammatical prefixes.

Final
clusters in English are more complex than initial ones, they express
different grammatical meanings: plurality, tense, number, e.g. texts,
mixed, glimpsed. In Russian there can be no more than three
consonants at the end of the syllable ( монстр

an exception).

An
important point of difference in syllable formation is that in
Russian there is a close contact between the onset consonants and the
following vowels (CV), which affects the quality of vowels:
palatalized consonants make the following vowels closer( compare the
quality of the vowel [э] in the words жест and мел). In
English, like in all Germanic languages, there is a close contact
between the vowel and the coda consonants (VC), which affects the
length of vowels (positional length).

There
are several theories
which try to explain the mechanism of syllable formation and syllable
division.

The
oldest theory is the
so-called expiratory
theory

byR.H.Stetson, according to which each syllable corresponds to one
expiration. A word consists of as many syllables as there are such
expirations made when the word is uttered. The point where a new
expiration starts indicates the syllabic boundary of the word.

The
expiratory theory is strongly criticized in Russia and abroad.
According to the experimental data more than ten syllables can easily
be pronounced during one expiration.

The
sonority theory

propounded by Otto Jespersen is widespread among foreign linguists.
The term ‘sonority’ is understood by Otto Jespersen as ‘the
degree of perceptibility’.

All
speech sounds have different inherent sonority. The most sonorous are
open back vowels, the least sonorous are the voiceless stops. Otto
Jespersen classified all speech sounds according to seven levels of
sonority:

(1)
vowels;

(2)
semi-vowels /j,w,/;

(3)
sonorants /l,r,m,n,ƞ/;

(4)voiced
fricatives /v, z,ʒ,/;

(5)
voiced stops /b,d,g/;

(6)
voiceless fricatives /f,ѳ,s,ʃ,h/;

(7)
voiceless stops /p,t,k/.

In
any sequence the most sonorous sounds tend to form the peak of the
syllable. For example, the word “popular” consists of three
peaks, that is why it has three syllables.

The
sonority theory is also criticized because it cannot explain the
mechanism of syllable formation and syllable division. Besides this
theory is helpless in determining the number of syllables in such
words as “going”, “highest”, “speak”, etc. According to
the sonority theory the words “going” and “highest” consist
of one syllable, because there is only one peak (or rather plateau)
of prominence. But in reality the words have two syllables.

The
words “speak” and “star” have two peaks of prominence, hence,
there should be two syllables in them, but that is wrong because
fricatives are non-syllabic in English.

Prof.
L.V.Shcherba put forward another theory of syllable formation and
syllable division, which was further developed by his followers and
at present is known as the
theory of muscular tension
.

According
to this theory a syllable is an arc of muscular tension, which is
weak in the beginning and in the end and strong in the middle. If a
syllable consists of a vowel, its strength increases in the
beginning, reaches the maximum at the peak and then gradually
decreases.

Consonants
within a syllable are characterized by different distribution of
muscular tension. Prof. Shcherba distinguishes the following types of
consonants:

1)
initially strong and finally weak, as in it,
on,
us;

2)
finally strong and initially weak, as in may,
tea,
no;

3)
double consonants (two similar sounds) which are strong at both ends
and weak in the middle, as in good
day,
misspell,
etc.

The
most energetic part of a consonant is attached to a vowel. For
instance, in the word “ten” there are two consonants: /t/ and
/n/. The consonant /t/ is finally strong, because its end is attached
to the vowel. The consonant /n/ is initially strong, because the
vowel is attached to its beginning. The syllable /ten/ may be
represented graphically as an arc, called an arc of muscular tension.
At the beginning /t/ is weak, at the end it gets stronger. The
muscular tension increases until it reaches its climax produced by
the vowel /e/. Then the muscular tension begins to decrease. The
sound /n/ is still strong at the beginning but gets quite weak at the
end. There are as many syllables in a word as there are arcs of
muscular tension.

The
syllabic boundary lies at a point where the consonant is the weakest.
Initially weak consonants constitute the beginning of a syllable.
Finally weak consonants constitute the end of a syllable.
Double-peaked consonants may only occur at the juncture of two
syllables, as in /gud-deɪ/, /mɪs-spel/, /ʌn-noun/, etc.

Prof.
N.I.Zhinkin’s investigation of the mechanism of syllable formation
and syllable division in Russian may serve as a basis for a general
theory of syllables. By using different complicated techniques Prof.
Zhinkin found out which speech organ causes a syllable to be formed.
This organ is the pharyngeal cavity. When the walls of the pharynx
are contracted, the passage through the pharyngeal cavity gets
narrower. This process increases the actual loudness of the sound and
this produces an arc of loudness. The peak of the syllable is louder
and higher in pitch than the onset and the coda. This theory
combines the level of production and the level of perception.

Prof.
Zhinkin has proved that the arc of loudness is due to the complex
work of all the speech organs. That is why Prof. Vassilyev suggests
that it would be more precise to call a syllable an “arc of
articulatory effort”. A syllable begins at a point where a new
articulatory effort starts and ends at a point where the articulatory
effort ends. So a syllable is a phonetic unit which is pronounced by
one articulatory effort accompanied by one muscular contraction,
which results acoustically and auditorily in one uninterrupted arc of
loudness.

Each
language has its own peculiarities in its syllabic structure. There
are a nmber of factors determining the rules for syllable division in
English. Syllable division in English is closely connected with the
checked or free character of the vowel in a stressed position. The
Sequence of CVCV may have different types of syllables depending on
the character of the vowel. In this case the syllable division is
governed by the following rules:

1)
The sequence of /’CVCV/ may include two open syllables if the
stressed vowel is a long monophthong or a diphthong, e.g. /si:-lɪƞ/,
/ʃu:-tǝ/, /meɪ-kǝ/, /nɪǝ-rǝ/. The syllable division in Russian
fully coincides with this pattern, as in both the languages the
consonants are strong at the end.

2)
The sequence of /’CVCV/ has a closed syllable and an open one
/CVC-V/ if the stressed vowel is a short monophthong, e.g. /pɪt-ɪ/,
/mer-ɪ/, /æp-l/, /ful-ɪ/.The first syllable remains closed because
short stressed vowels are checked, i.e. they should be immediately
followed by consonants. The results of instrumental analysis show
that the point of syllable division is inside the intervocalic
consonant. Syllables of this kind present a great difficulty to
Russian students because in similar Russian words there are two open
syllables. In English the intervocalic consonants of this type are
initially strong while in Russian they are finally strong. Cf.: Ма-ня
— /mʌn-ɪ/, Си-ти — /sɪt-ɪ/.

3)
Short and long monophthongs and diphthongs make for an open type of
syllable if they are unstressed and are separated from the
neighbouring vowels by only one consonant, e.g. /ri:-ækt/, /bɪ-gɪn/,
/mju:-zɪǝm/, /a:-tɪstɪk/.

Phonetic
and orthographic syllables should not be confused. Sometimes they
coincide (ear-ly — /ǝ:-lɪ/, late-ly — /leɪt-lɪ/), sometimes they
do not (art-ist-ic — /a:-tɪs-tɪk/, driv-er — /draɪ-vǝ/, lat-er —
/leɪ-tǝ/. Division of words into syllables in writing is based on
morphological principles. The part of a word which is separated
should be either a prefix, or a suffix, or a root.

The
syllabic structure of English performs three main functions:

1)
constitutive, 2) distinctive, 3) recognitive. The
constitutive function

consists in the fact that syllables constitute the material forms of
all the words, phrases and sentences.

The
distinctive function

of the syllabic structure includes differences in both syllable
formation and syllable division. Presence or absence of a syllable in
one and the same position, as well as different syllabic boundaries
may differentiate one word (or phrase, or sentence) from another word
(or phrase, or sentence). Here are some phonological oppositions of
presence vs absence of a syllable in the same position in a minimal
pair: /bet/ — /betǝ/, /dri:m/ — /dri:mɪ/, /sli:p/ — /ǝ’sli:p/.

The
word-distinctive function of syllable division may be illustrated by
the example /naɪ-treɪt/ — /naɪt-reɪt/ (nitrate – night-rate).
The number of combinations of words distinguished from each other by
different syllabic boundaries is rather considerable: a name – an
aim, I scream – ice-cream, a nice house – an ice house, I saw her
eyes – I saw her rise, that’s tough – that stuff, confined –
can find, a notion – an ocean, I saw th meat, I saw them eat, etc.

The
recognitive function

of the syllabic structure manifests itself in the fact that the right
syllabic boundary makes it easier to recognize words, phrases and
sentences. Compare the following:

Correct
pronunciation Wrong pronunciation

happy
/hæp-ɪ/ /hæ-pɪ/

Stand
up! /stænd-ʌp/ /stæn-dʌp/

an
apple /ǝn-æpl/ /ǝ-næpl/

at
eight /ǝt-eɪt/ /ǝ-teɪt/

The
violation of the recognitive function results in the following:

1)
wrong syllable division produces a strong foreign accent;

2)
it produces a comic impression on native speakers;

3)
it hampers the process of communication.

Соседние файлы в предмете [НЕСОРТИРОВАННОЕ]

  • #
  • #
  • #
  • #
  • #
  • #
  • #
  • #
  • #
  • #
  • #

The syllabic structure in English (Syllabification)

1. Theories on syllable formation and division

Vowels and consonants typically do not act alone; they show a tendency to cluster or group. These smallest phonetic groups are generally given the name of syllables. Being the smallest pronounceable units, syllables form morphemes, words and phrases. Each of these units is characterized by a certain syllabic structure. Thus a meaningful language unit phonetically may be considered from the point of view of syllable formation and syllable division.

A syllable is a minimal unit of organization for a sequence of speech sounds, acting as a unit of rhythm. Rhythm in its turn is the perceived regularity of prominent units of speech. It is stated in terms of such patterns as stressed vs. unstressed syllables (English) or short vs. long (Latin). (Rhythm is understood by speakers as the use of stressed syllables which occur at regular intervals in the stream of speech). So a syllable connects sound and rhythmic structures.

A syllable is a complicated phenomenon and like a speech sound it can be studied on four levels — articulatory, acoustic, auditory and functional.

Acoustically and auditory the syllable is characterized by the force of utterance, or accent, pitch of the voice, sonority and length.

Auditory the syllable is the smallest unit of perception: the listener identifies the whole of the syllable and only after that the sounds contained.

The articulotary energy which constitutes the syllable results from the combined action of the power, vibrator, resonator and obstructer mechanism.

Phonologically the syllable is regarded and defined in terms of its structural and functional properties.

The complexity of the phenomenon gave rise to many attempts to identify a syllable and its boundaries.

The most ancient theory states that there are as many syllables in a word as there are vowels. This theory is primitive and insufficient since it does not take into consideration consonants which also can form syllables in some languages, neither does it explain the boundary of syllables.

The so-called expiratory (chest pulse or pressure) theory was suggested by R.H. Stetson. This theory is based on the assumption that expiration in speech is a pulsating process and each syllable should correspond to a single expiration. So the number of syllables in an utterance is determined by the number of expirations made in the production of the utterance. The main objection to the theory is the following: in a phrase a number of words and consequently a number of syllables can be pronounced with a single expiration, e.g. in adjacent syllables when two vowels co-occur (seeing, doing). This fact makes the validity of the theory doubtful. (articulatory approach).

Another theory of a syllable put forward by O. Jespersen is generally called the sonority theory (or prominence theory). According to O. Jespersen, each sound is characterized by a certain degree of sonority which is understood as acoustic property of a sound that determines its perceptibility. According to this sound property a ranking of speech sounds could be established:

<the least sonorous>

voiceless plosives à voiced fricatives àvoiced plosives à voiced fricatives à sonorants à close vowels àopen vowels

<the most sonorous>

Sounds are grouped around the most sonorous ones, which form the peaks of sonority in a syllable. Two points of lower sonority constitute the beginning and the end of one syllable.

Melt & metal:&nbsp;in the first word [e] is the most sonorous sound, the only peak of sonority, it is a one-syllable word. In the word metal  there are two peaks of sonority [e,l], it is a two-syllable word. (sudden, пятница). (auditory approach)

 According to V.A. Vassilyev the most serious drawback of this theory is that it fails to explain the actual mechanism of syllable formation and syllable division, because it does not state to which syllable the weak sound at the boundary of two syllables belongs. In such words as master, should the syllable division be ma-ster, mas-ter, or mast-er? We are left with this problem, even though in each case the relative sonority of the sounds is the same.

Further experimental work aimed at description of the syllable resulted in lots of other theories. However, the question of articulatory mechanism of syllable is still an open question in phonetics. We might suppose that this mechanism is similar in all languages and could be regarded as phonetic universal.

In Russian linguistics there has been adopted the theory of syllable by LV Shcherba. It is called the theory of muscular tension. In most languages there is the syllabic phoneme in the centre of the syllable which is usually a vowel phoneme or, in some languages, a sonorant. The phonemes preceding or following the syllabic peak are called marginal (slopes). The tenseness of articulation increases within the range of prevocalic consonants and then decreases within the range of postvocalic consonants.

Russian linguist and psychologist N.I. Zhinkin has suggested the so-called loudness theory which seems to combine both production and perception levels. The experiments carried out by N.I. Zhinkin showed that the arc of loudness of perception level is formed due to variations of the volume of the pharyngeal passage which is modified by contractions of its walls. The narrowing of the passage and the increase in muscular tension which results from it reinforce the actual loudness of a vowel thus forming the peak of the syllable. So the syllable is the arc оf loudness which correlates with the arc of articulatory effort on the speed production level since variations in loudness are due to the work of all speech mechanisms. (mistake: It consists of two arcs of loudness in which [m, t] are finally strong consonants and [s, k] are finally weak. [s] constitutes  the end of the arc of loudness, [t] constitutes the beginning.)

It is perfectly obvious that no phonetician has succeeded so far in giving an adequate explanation of what the syllable is. The difficulties seem to arise from the various possibilities of approach to the unit. There exist two points of view:

1. Sоme linguists consider the syllable to be a purely articulatory unit which lacks any functional value. This point of view is defended on the ground that the boundaries of syllables do not always coincide with those of morphemes.

2. However the majority of linguists treat the syllable as the smallest pronounceable unit which can reveal some linguistic function.

Trying to define the syllable from the articulatory point of view we may talk about universals. When we mean the functional aspect of the syllable it should be defined with the reference to the structure of one particular language.

The definition of the syllable from the functional point of view tends to single out the following features of the syllable:

a) a syllable is a chain of phonemes of varying length;

b) a syllable is constructed on the basis of contrast of its constituents (which is usually of vowel — consonant type);

c) the nucleus of a syllable is a vowel, the presence of consonants is optional; there are no languages in which vowels are not used as syllable nuclei, however, there are languages in which this function is sometimes performed by consonants;

d) the distribution of phonemes in the syllabic structure follows the rules which are specific enough for a particular language.

2. The structure and functions of syllables in English

Syllable formation in English is based on the phonological opposition vowel — consonant. Vowels are usually syllabic while consonants are not with the exceptions of [l], [m], [n], which become syllabic in a final position proceeded by a noise consonant: bottle [bσtl], bottom [bσtm], button [b/tn] and [r] (in those accents which pronounce [r]) perhaps [præps].

The structure of English syllables can be summarized as follows:

Many syllables have one or more consonants preceding the nucleus. These make up the syllable onsetmeso, plow.

• Many syllables have one or more consonants, following the nucleus (peak, crest). They make up the syllable coda. They are traditionally known as closed syllables: catjump.

• The combination of nucleus and coda has a special significance, making up the rhyme of a syllable.

The English language has developed the closed type of a syllable as the fundamental one while in Russian it is the open type that forms the basis of syllable formation.

The other aspect of this component is syllable division. The problem of syllable division in case of intervocalic consonants and their clusters, like in such words as city, extra, standing and others.

Let us consider the first word [‘sit.i]. There exist two possibilities:

a) the point of syllable division is after the intervocalic consonant:

b) the point of syllable division is inside the consonant.

In both cases the first syllable remains closed because the short vowel should remain checked. The results of instrumental analysis show that the point of syllable division in such words is inside the intervocalic consonant. EPD indicates the point of division after the consonant.

The second case. There are two syllables in the word extra but where should the boundary between them fall?

1) [e — kstrə]. It is unlike that people would opt for a division between [e] and [kstrə] because there are no syllables in English which begin with consonant sequence [kstr].

2) Similarly, a division between [ekstr] and [ə] would be unnatural.

3) [ek — strə], [eks — trə], [ekst — rə] are possible. People usually prefer either of the first two options here, but there is no obvious way of deciding between them.

In some cases we may take into account the morphemic structure of words. For example, standing consists of two syllables; on phonetic grounds [stæn — diŋ). On grammatical grounds [stænd — iŋ].

The syllable as a phonological unit performs three functions: constitutive, distinctive, and identificatory. They are closely connected

The first is the constitutive function. It lies in its ability to be a part of a word itself. The syllables form language units of greater magnitude that is words, morphemes, and utterances. In this respect two things should be emphasized. First, the syllable is the unit within which the relations between distinctive features of phonemes and their acoustic correlates are revealed. Second, within a syllable (or syllables) prosodic characteristics of speech are realized, which form the stress pattern of a word and the intonation structure of an utterance. Thus, the syllable is a specific minimal structure of both segmental and suprasegmental features.

The other function is distinctive one. In this respect the syllable is characterized by its ability to differentiate words and word-forms. The following minimal pair illustrates the word distinctive function in the syllabic: nitrate — night-rate. There are rather many combinations in English distinguished from each other by means of the difference of the place of the syllabic boundary: an aim — a name; an ice house — a nice house, etc. Sometimes the difference in syllable division may be the basic ground for differentiation in such pairs as I saw her rise.- I saw her eyes; I saw the meat — I saw them eat.

The identificatory function is conditioned by the pronunciation of the speaker. The listener can understand the exact meaning of the utterance only if s/he perceives the correct syllabic boundary: pea stalks – peace talks; my train – might rain.

3. Brief Comparison of English and Russian Systems of Syllable Formation and Division.

1) Similar syllabic structural types can be found.

2) In both languages the single intervocal consonant between two phonetic syllables belongs to the next vowel: morning [mo:-niŋ] – море; cozy [kou-zi]- воляю

3) All consonants may begin the syllable in English with the exception of [ŋ]. In Russian all consonants may begin the syllable.

4) In Russian the initial clusters are more complex (вдрызг). In English the final clusters are more complex (texts, strengths, twelfths).

5) English diphthongs belong to one syllable, triphthongs may be divided into two parts.

The conclusion: Thus, the syllable is a specific minimal structure of both segmental and suprasegmental features. It is a minimal unit of organization for a sequence of speech sounds, acting as a unit of rhythm. Rhythm in its turn is the perceived regularity of prominent units of speech. It is stated in terms of such patterns as stressed vs. unstressed syllables (English) or short vs. long (Latin). So a syllable connects sound and rhythmic structures, because rhythm depends upon the alternation of stressed and unstressed syllables. And rhythm is the pivot of such a complex phenomenon as intonation (suprasegmental unit). (Rhythm is understood by speakers as the use of stressed syllables which occur at regular intervals in the stream of speech). So a syllable connects sound and rhythmic structures.

Syllable is another important concept of English pronunciation. Syllables are the building blocks of a word. Unless we understand syllables, we will continue to make mistakes in pronunciation. To know about a syllable in detail, click here.
Understanding syllables helps a lot with pronunciation. They improve reading and speaking skills also. When we say a word, the sounds we create naturally divide the word into small parts. We call these parts “syllables.” The process of breaking a word into small parts is called syllabification.

For Example

Reflect /rɪ/ˈflekt/ has two syllables.

photography  /fə/ˈtɒ/ɡrə/fi/ has four syllables.

Bag /bæɡ/ has only one syllable and it forms the whole word.

I // has only one syllable and it forms the whole word.

To understand the sound symbols between the slashes, click here.

Syllable Structure

Each language has certain restrictions to use possible combinations of sounds to form words or part of words. In English language, we cannot have a complete word having all consonants or words beginning with a consonant sequence like fpg or zbn etc. Considering these possible combinations of sounds (syllables), a syllable structure is followed in English language.

A syllable can have three parts: onset, nucleus, and coda. The onset and the coda are consonants or consonant clusters. The onset appears at the beginning and the coda appears at the end of the syllable. The nucleus is a vowel which forms the core of the syllable.
For example, in a word pin which has one syllable, [p] is the onset,[i ] is a nucleus and [n] is the coda.

If a coda is present in a syllable, the nucleus and the coda form a single unit called a rhyme; otherwise the nucleus makes up the rhyme by itself. Looking at “pin” again, [in] forms the rhyme.
The onset and the coda are not necessarily present in a syllable but the nucleus is always present. The nucleus can be preceded or followed by a consonant or consonant clusters.
For example
The word eye /ai/ has only one syllable. It has neither the onset nor the coda but only the nucleus.
Fry /frai/ has the onset and the nucleus but not the coda.
Ice /ais/ has the nucleus and the coda but not the onset.

Read about syllabification and its important rules.

Like this post? Please share to your friends:
  • The sweet word for friend
  • The sight word can
  • The talking word factory
  • The same word pronounced differently
  • The sweet word for boyfriend