The morphological structure of a word lexicology

Word is the principal and
basic unit of the language system, the largest on the morphologic and
the smallest on the syntactic plane of linguistic analysis.

According
to the number of morphemes words can be classified into monomorphic
and polymorphic. Monomorphic or root-words consist of only one
root-morpheme, e.g. small, dog, make, give, etc. All polymorphic word
fall into two subgroups: derived words and compound words –
according to the number of root-morphemes they have. Derived words
are composed of one root-morpheme and one or more derivational
morphemes, e.g. acceptable, outdo, disagreeable, etc. Compound words
are those which contain at least two root-morphemes, the number of
derivational morphemes being insignificant.

There can be both root- and
derivational morphemes in compounds as in pen-holder,
light-mindedness, or only root-morphemes as in lamp-shade, eye-ball,
etc.

The
term morpheme
is derived from Greek
morphe

“form ”+ -eme.
The Greek suffix –eme
has been adopted by linguistic to denote the smallest unit or the
minimum distinctive
feature
.

The morpheme is the smallest
meaningful unit of form. A form in these cases a recurring discrete
unit of speech. Morphemes occur in speech only as constituent parts
of words, not independently, although a word may consist of single
morpheme.

The
root-morpheme

is the lexical nucleus of the word; it has a very general and
abstract lexical meaning common to a set of semantically related
words constituting one word-cluster, e.g. (to) teach,
teacher, teaching
.
Besides the lexical meaning root-morphemes possess all other types of
meaning proper to morphemes except the part-of-speech meaning which
is not found in roots.

Affixational
morphemes

include inflectional affixes or inflections and derivational affixes.
Inflections
carry only grammatical meaning and are thus relevant only for the
formation of word-forms. Derivational
affixes

are relevant for building various types of words. They are lexically
always dependent on the root which they modify. They possess the same
types of meaning as found in roots, most of them have the
part-of-speech meaning which makes them structurally the important
part of the word as they condition the lexico-grammatical class the
word belongs to. Due to this component of their meaning the
derivational affixes are classified into affixes building different
parts of speech: nouns, verbs, adjectives or adverbs.

Roots
and derivational affixes are generally easily distinguished and the
difference between them is clearly felt as, e.g., in the words
helpless,
handy, blackness, Londoner, refill
,
etc.: the root-morphemes help-,
hand-, black-, London-, fill-,

are understood as the lexical centers of the words, and less,
-y, -ness, -er, re-

are
felt as morphemes dependent on these roots.

Distinction is also made of
free and bound morphemes.

Free
morphemes

coincide with word-forms of independently functioning words. It is
obvious that free morphemes can be found only among roots, so the
morpheme boy-
in the word boy
is a free morpheme; in the word undesirable
there is only one free morpheme desire-;
the word pen-holder
has two free morphemes pen-
and
hold-.
It follows that bound
morphemes

are those that do not coincide with separate word- forms,
consequently all derivational morphemes, such as –ness,
-able, -er

are bound. Root-morphemes may be both free and bound. The morphemes
theor-
in the words theory,
theoretical,
or
horr-
in the words horror,
horrible, horrify; Angl-
in

Anglo-Saxon; Afr-

in Afro-Asian
are all bound roots as there are no identical word-forms.

The
stem
is defined as that part of the word which remains unchanged
throughout its paradigm, thus the stem which appears in the paradigm
(to) ask
( ), asks,
asked, asking
is
ask-;
the
stem of the word singer
(
), singer’s,
singers, singers’
is
singer-.
It is the stem of the word that takes the inflections which shape the
word grammatically as one or another part of speech.

Simple
stems
are
semantically non-motivated and do not constitute a pattern on analogy
with which new stems may be modeled.
Simple
stems are generally monomorphic and phonetically identical with the
root morpheme.
Retain, receive, horrible, pocket, motion,
etc.
should be regarded as simple, non- motivated stems.

Derived
stems –
root
and derivational affix.

Compound
stems
are
made up of two IC’s, both of which are themselves stems, for
example match-box,
driving-suit, pen-holder,

etc. It is built by joining of two stems, one of which is simple, the
other derived.

Bound
stem –
is
not harmonious to a separate word.

To
study the motivation of the word the method of immediate ultimate
consistent is used. It is based on bannery opposition – each state
of segmentation involves 2 components words brake into.

Соседние файлы в предмете [НЕСОРТИРОВАННОЕ]

  • #
  • #
  • #
  • #
  • #
  • #
  • #
  • #
  • #
  • #
  • #

WORD STRUCTURE IN MODERN ENGLISH

  I.   The morphological structure of a word. Morphemes. Types of morphemes. Allomorphs.

II.   Structural types of words.

III.   Principles of morphemic analysis.

  IV.   Derivational level of analysis. Stems. Types of stems. Derivational types of words.

I.   The morphological structure of a word. Morphemes. Types of Morphemes.  Allomorphs.

There are two levels of approach to the study of word- structure: the level of morphemic analysis and the level of derivational or word-formation analysis.

Word is the principal and basic unit of the language system, the largest on the morphologic and the smallest on the syntactic plane of linguistic analysis.

It has been universally acknowledged that a great many words have a composite nature and are made up of morphemes, the basic units on the morphemic level, which are defined as the smallest indivisible two-facet language units.

The term morpheme is derived from Greek morphe “form ”+ -eme. The Greek suffix –eme has been adopted by linguistic to denote the smallest unit or the minimum distinctive feature.

The morpheme is the smallest meaningful unit of form. A form in these cases a recurring discrete unit of speech. Morphemes occur in speech only as constituent parts of words, not independently, although a word may consist of single morpheme. Even a cursory examination of the morphemic structure of English words reveals that they are composed of morphemes of different types: root-morphemes and affixational morphemes. Words that consist of a root and an affix are called derived words or derivatives and are produced by the process of word building known as affixation (or derivation).

The root-morpheme is the lexical nucleus of the word; it has a very general and abstract lexical meaning common to a set of semantically related words constituting one word-cluster, e.g. (to) teach, teacher, teaching. Besides the lexical meaning root-morphemes possess all other types of meaning proper to morphemes except the part-of-speech meaning which is not found in roots.

Affixational morphemes include inflectional affixes or inflections and derivational affixes. Inflections carry only grammatical meaning and are thus relevant only for the formation of word-forms. Derivational affixes are relevant for building various types of words. They are lexically always dependent on the root which they modify. They possess the same types of meaning as found in roots, but unlike root-morphemes most of them have the part-of-speech meaning which makes them structurally the important part of the word as they condition the lexico-grammatical class the word belongs to. Due to this component of their meaning the derivational affixes are classified into affixes building different parts of speech: nouns, verbs, adjectives or adverbs.

Roots and derivational affixes are generally easily distinguished and the difference between them is clearly felt as, e.g., in the words helpless, handy, blackness, Londoner, refill, etc.: the root-morphemes help-, hand-, black-, London-, fill-, are understood as the lexical centers of the words, and less, -y,      -ness, -er, re- are felt as morphemes dependent on these roots.

 Distinction is also made of free and bound morphemes.

Free morphemes coincide with word-forms of independently functioning words. It is obvious that free morphemes can be found only among roots, so the morpheme boy- in the word boy is a free morpheme; in the word undesirable there is only one free morpheme desire-; the word pen-holder has two free morphemes  pen- and hold-. It follows that bound morphemes are those that do not coincide with separate word- forms, consequently all derivational morphemes, such as –ness, -able, -er are bound. Root-morphemes may be both free and bound. The morphemes theor- in the words theory, theoretical, or horr- in the words horror, horrible, horrify; Angl- in  Anglo-Saxon; Afr- in Afro-Asian are all bound roots as there are no identical word-forms.

It should also be noted that morphemes may have different phonemic shapes. In the word-cluster please , pleasing , pleasure , pleasant the phonemic shapes of the word stand in complementary distribution or in alternation with each other. All the representations of the given morpheme, that manifest alternation are called allomorphs/or morphemic variants/ of that morpheme.

The combining form allo- from Greek allos “other” is used in linguistic terminology to denote elements of a group whose members together consistute a structural unit of the language (allophones, allomorphs). Thus, for example, -ion/ -tion/ -sion/ -ation are the positional variants of the same suffix, they do not differ in meaning or function but show a slight difference in sound form depending on the final phoneme of the preceding stem. They are considered as variants of one and the same morpheme and called its allomorphs.

Allomorph is defined as a positional variant of a morpheme occurring in a specific environment and so characterized by complementary description.

Complementary distribution is said to take place, when two linguistic variants cannot appear in the same environment.

Different morphemes are characterized by contrastive distribution, i.e. if they occur in the same environment they signal different meanings. The suffixes –able and –ed, for instance, are different morphemes, not allomorphs, because adjectives in –able mean “ capable of beings”.

Allomorphs will also occur among prefixes. Their form then depends on the initials of the stem with which they will assimilate.

Two or more sound forms of a stem existing under conditions of complementary distribution may also be regarded as allomorphs, as, for instance, in long a: length n.

II. Structural types of words.

The morphological analysis of word- structure on the morphemic level aims at splitting the word into its constituent morphemes – the basic units at this level of analysis – and at determining their number and types. The four types (root words, derived words, compound, shortenings) represent the main structural types of Modern English words, and conversion, derivation and composition the most productive ways of word building.

According to the number of morphemes words can be classified into monomorphic and polymorphic. Monomorphic or root-words consist of only one root-morpheme, e.g. small, dog, make, give, etc. All polymorphic word fall into two subgroups:  derived words and compound words – according to the number of root-morphemes they have. Derived words are composed of one root-morpheme and one or more derivational morphemes, e.g. acceptable, outdo, disagreeable, etc. Compound words are those which contain at least two root-morphemes, the number of derivational morphemes being insignificant. There can be both root- and derivational morphemes in compounds as in pen-holder, light-mindedness, or only root-morphemes as in lamp-shade, eye-ball, etc.

These structural types are not of equal importance. The clue to the correct understanding of their comparative value lies in a careful consideration of: 1)the importance of each type in the existing wordstock, and 2) their frequency value in actual speech. Frequency is by far the most important factor. According to the available word counts made in different parts of speech, we find that derived words numerically constitute the largest class of words in the existing wordstock; derived nouns comprise approximately 67% of the total number, adjectives about 86%, whereas compound nouns make about 15% and adjectives about 4%. Root words come to 18% in nouns, i.e. a trifle more than the number of compound words; adjectives root words come to approximately 12%.

But we cannot fail to perceive that root-words occupy a predominant place. In English, according to the recent frequency counts, about 60% of the total number of nouns and 62% of the total number of adjectives in current use are root-words. Of the total number of adjectives and nouns, derived words comprise about 38% and 37% respectively while compound words comprise an insignificant 2% in nouns and 0.2% in adjectives. Thus it is the root-words that constitute the foundation and the backbone of the vocabulary and that are of paramount importance in speech. It should also be mentioned that root words are characterized by a high degree of collocability and a complex variety of meanings in contrast with words of other structural types whose semantic structures are much poorer. Root- words also serve as parent forms for all types of derived and compound words.

III. Principles of morphemic analysis.

In most cases the morphemic structure of words is transparent enough and individual morphemes clearly stand out within the word. The segmentation of words is generally carried out according to the method of Immediate and Ultimate Constituents. This method is based on the binary principle, i.e. each stage of the procedure involves two components the word immediately breaks into. At each stage these two components are referred to as the Immediate Constituents. Each Immediate Constituent at the next stage of analysis is in turn broken into smaller meaningful elements. The analysis is completed when we arrive at constituents incapable of further division, i.e. morphemes. These are referred to Ultimate Constituents.

A synchronic morphological analysis is most effectively accomplished by the procedure known as the analysis into Immediate Constituents. ICs are the two meaningful parts forming a large linguistic unity.

The method is based on the fact that a word characterized by morphological divisibility is involved in certain structural correlations. To sum up: as we break the word we obtain at any level only ICs one of which is the stem of the given word. All the time the analysis is based on the patterns characteristic of the English vocabulary. As a pattern showing the interdependence of all the constituents segregated at various stages, we obtain the following formula:

un+ { [ ( gent- + -le ) + -man ] + -ly}

Breaking a word into its Immediate Constituents we observe in each cut the structural order of the constituents.

A  diagram presenting the four cuts described looks as follows:

1. un- / gentlemanly

2.   un- / gentleman / — ly

3.   un- / gentle / — man / — ly

4.   un- / gentl / — e / — man / — ly

A similar analysis on the word-formation level showing not only the morphemic constituents of the word but also the structural pattern on which it is built.

The analysis of word-structure at the morphemic level must proceed to the stage of Ultimate Constituents. For example, the noun friendliness is first segmented into the ICs: [frendlı-] recurring in the adjectives friendly-looking and friendly and [-nıs] found in a countless number  of nouns, such as unhappiness, blackness, sameness, etc. the IC [-nıs] is at the same time an UC of the word, as it cannot be broken into any smaller elements possessing both sound-form and meaning. Any further division of –ness would give individual speech-sounds which denote nothing by themselves. The IC [frendlı-] is next broken into the ICs [-lı] and [frend-] which are both UCs of the word.

Morphemic analysis under the method of Ultimate Constituents may be carried out on the basis of two principles: the so-called root-principle and affix principle.

According to the affix principle the splitting of the word into its constituent morphemes is based on the identification of the affix within a set of words, e.g. the identification of the suffix –er leads to the segmentation of words singer, teacher, swimmer into the derivational morpheme er  and the roots teach- , sing-, drive-.

According to the root-principle, the segmentation of the word is based on the identification of the root-morpheme in a word-cluster, for example the identification of the root-morpheme agree-  in the words agreeable, agreement, disagree.

As a rule, the application of these principles is sufficient for the morphemic segmentation of words.

However, the morphemic structure of words in a number of cases defies such analysis, as it is not always so transparent and simple as in the cases mentioned above. Sometimes not only the segmentation of words into morphemes, but the recognition of certain sound-clusters as morphemes become doubtful which naturally affects the classification of words. In words like retain, detain, contain or  receive, deceive, conceive, perceive the sound-clusters [rı-], [dı-] seem to be singled quite easily, on the other hand, they undoubtedly have nothing in common with the phonetically identical prefixes  re-, de- as found in words re-write, re-organize, de-organize, de-code. Moreover, neither the sound-cluster [rı-] or [dı-], nor the [-teın] or [-sı:v] possess any lexical or functional meaning of their own. Yet, these sound-clusters are felt as having a certain meaning because [rı-] distinguishes retain from detain and [-teın] distinguishes retain from receive.

It follows that all these sound-clusters have a differential and a certain distributional meaning as their order arrangement point to the affixal status of re-, de-, con-, per- and makes one understand —tain and –ceive as roots. The differential and distributional meanings seem to give sufficient ground to recognize these sound-clusters as morphemes, but as they lack lexical meaning of their own, they are set apart from all other types of morphemes and are known in linguistic literature as pseudo- morphemes. Pseudo- morphemes of the same kind  are also encountered in words like rusty-fusty.

IV.   Derivational level of analysis. Stems. Types of Stems. Derivational types of word.

The morphemic analysis of words only defines the constituent morphemes, determining their types and their meaning but does not reveal the hierarchy of the morphemes comprising the word. Words are no mere sum totals of morpheme, the latter reveal a definite, sometimes very complex interrelation. Morphemes are arranged according to certain rules, the arrangement differing in various types of words and particular groups within the same types. The pattern of morpheme arrangement underlies the classification of words into different types and enables one to understand how new words appear in the language. These relations within the word and the interrelations between different types and classes of words are known as derivative or word- formation relations.

The analysis of derivative relations aims at establishing a correlation between different types and the structural patterns words are built on. The basic unit at the derivational level is the stem.

The stem is defined as that part of the word which remains unchanged throughout its paradigm, thus the stem which appears in the paradigm (to) ask ( ), asks, asked, asking is ask-; thestem of the word singer ( ), singer’s, singers, singers’ is singer-. It is the stem of the word that takes the inflections which shape the word grammatically as one or another part of speech.

The structure of stems should be described in terms of IC’s analysis, which at this level aims at establishing the patterns of typical derivative relations within the stem and the derivative correlation between stems of different types.

There are three types of stems: simple, derived and compound.

Simple stems are semantically non-motivated and do not constitute a pattern on analogy with which new stems may be modeled. Simple stems are generally monomorphic and phonetically identical with the root morpheme. The derivational structure of stems does not always coincide with the result of morphemic analysis. Comparison proves that not all morphemes relevant at the morphemic level are relevant at the derivational level of analysis. It follows that bound morphemes and all types of pseudo- morphemes are irrelevant to the derivational structure of stems as they do not meet requirements of double opposition and derivative interrelations. So the stem of such words as retain, receive, horrible, pocket, motion, etc. should be regarded as simple, non- motivated stems.

Derived stems are built on stems of various structures though which they are motivated, i.e. derived stems are understood on the basis  of the derivative relations between their IC’s and the correlated stems. The derived stems are mostly polymorphic in which case the segmentation results only in one IC that is itself a stem, the other IC being necessarily a derivational affix.

Derived stems are not necessarily polymorphic.

Compound stems are made up of two IC’s, both of which are themselves stems, for example match-box, driving-suit, pen-holder, etc. It is built by joining of two stems, one of which is simple, the other derived.

In more complex cases the result of the analysis at the two levels sometimes seems even to contracted one another.

The derivational types of words are classified according to the structure of their stems into simple, derived and compound words.

Derived words are those composed of one root- morpheme and one or more derivational morpheme.

Compound words contain at least two root- morphemes, the number of derivational morphemes being insignificant.

Derivational compound is a word formed by a simultaneous process of composition and derivational.

Compound words proper are formed by joining together stems of word already available in the language.

>MODERN  ENGLISH LEXICOLOGY Morphological Structure of the English Word
MODERN ENGLISH LEXICOLOGY Morphological Structure of the English Word

>Problems for discussion Methods of word-structure analysis Distinction between “morpheme”, “morph”, “allomorph” Structural classification
Problems for discussion Methods of word-structure analysis Distinction between “morpheme”, “morph”, “allomorph” Structural classification of morphemes Semantic classification of morphemes Structural types of words

>Which methods are used to observe structural composition of words? Morphemic analysis  Morphological
Which methods are used to observe structural composition of words? Morphemic analysis Morphological analysis Method of Immediate and Ultimate Constituents

>Morphemic analysis    Segments a word into morphemes to determine their number
Morphemic analysis Segments a word into morphemes to determine their number and types: girlishness one root morpheme (-girl-) + two suffixes (-ish; -ness)

>Morphological analysis Compares structures of words to state:  a) the structural pattern of
Morphological analysis Compares structures of words to state: a) the structural pattern of the word formation: Girl :: girlish  “noun stem + -ish  adjective” b) the meaning of the pattern of word-formation: The pattern “noun stem + -ish  adjective” has the meaning“typical of, or having the qualities of”

>The method of immediate and ultimate constituents Observes the structural order of the constituents:
The method of immediate and ultimate constituents Observes the structural order of the constituents: girlishness Stage 1. girlish + -ness Stage 2. girl + -ish + -ness

>What is a morpheme?    Morpheme is the smallest indivisible two-facet language
What is a morpheme? Morpheme is the smallest indivisible two-facet language unit which occurs in speech only as a constituent part of words and recurs with the same meaning in a number of words: worker miner the doer of the action teacher

>What is a morph?    A morph is a single realization of
What is a morph? A morph is a single realization of a morpheme.

>What is an allomorph? im- (before b, m: impossible, immobilize),  il- (before l:
What is an allomorph? im- (before b, m: impossible, immobilize), il- (before l: illogical), ir- (before r: irregular) are allomorphs of in-

>Structural classification of morphemes Morpheme  free
Structural classification of morphemes Morpheme free bound semi-bound hand amazement sleep well handful handful well handy handy well-known

>Semantic classification of morphemes Morpheme  root morpheme
Semantic classification of morphemes Morpheme root morpheme non-root morpheme derivational functional prefix infix suffix inflection

>Examples unreadable:  un-
Examples unreadable: un- — prefix — read- — root — able — suffix stand -n- — infix near – nearer – nearest: -er-est — functional affixes

>Structural types of words Word
Structural types of words Word complex simple (root) derived compound compound-derived

>Simple words root morpheme (+ inflexion)  seldom chair longer asked
Simple words root morpheme (+ inflexion) seldom chair longer asked

>Derived words prefix + root (+inflexion) unkind root + suffix (+inflexion) readable prefix +
Derived words prefix + root (+inflexion) unkind root + suffix (+inflexion) readable prefix + root + suffix (+ inflexion) unanswerable

>Compound words root + root (+ inflexion)  sunflower baby-moon wait-and-see steam-boat
Compound words root + root (+ inflexion) sunflower baby-moon wait-and-see steam-boat

>Compound-derived words root + root + suffix (+inflexion)  prefix + root + root
Compound-derived words root + root + suffix (+inflexion) prefix + root + root (+inflexion) prefix + root + root + suffix (+inflexion) daydreamer middle-of-the-roader job-hopper

Identifying a Word.
Morphological
Structure of the Word
Lecture 5
 5.1.Problems of the Definition of the
Word
 5.2. Lexical and Grammatical Words
 5.3. Morphological Structure of the
Word
 5.4. Types of Morphemic
Segmentability
The Problems of the Definition
A word has many different aspects:
 1) phonological, as it has a sound form;
 2) morphological, as it is a certain
arrangement of morphemes;
 3) syntactic, as it may occur in different word
forms and signal different meanings.
The Problems of the Definition
A word is defined as:
 minimum free form (L. Bloomfield)
 association of a given meaning with
a given group of sounds susceptible
of a given grammatical employment
(A. Meillet)
The Problems of the Definition
A word is an autonomous unit of the
language in which
 a particular meaning is associated with
 a particular sound complex
 capable of a particular grammatical
employment and
 able to form a sentence by itself.
The Problems of the Definition
 Orthographically words may be spelt
differently: teapot, tea-pot, tea pot. Words are
written separately, but not all words fit this
category, e.g. will not – two words; cannot –
one word.
 There may be found different forms but
different forms are not necessarily regarded
as different words, e.g. teach, teaches,
taught, teaching; nice, nicer, nicest – are not
separate words.
The Problems of the Definition
 Some words can have the same forms but
completely different and unrelated meanings,
e.g. fair.
 The existence of idioms and phrasal verbs
seems to upset attempts to define words in
any formal way, e.g. to kick the bucket – “to
die”, to put off – “to postpone”.
The Problems of the Definition
 A lexeme is the abstract unit which
underlies some of the variants we have
observed in connection with 'words'.
 Thus, teach is the lexeme which
underlies some of the variants teach,
teaches, taught, teaching which are the
word-forms.
The Problems of the Definition
 The term lexeme is also connected with
more than one word-form expressed by
such lexical items as:
 Multi-word verbs, e.g., to catch up
with;
 Phrasal verbs, e.g., to clear up, to
switch off;
 Idioms, e.g., kick the bucket.
Lexical and Grammatical Words
 Lexical words are full words or content
words. They include nouns, adjectives, verbs,
adverbs and belong to the open classes of
words. They carry a higher information
content and are syntactically structured by the
grammatical words.
 LW form an open class of words because
they are subject to diachronic change,
changes in form and meaning over a
period of time.
Lexical and Grammatical Words
Nouns
 A noun is a naming word. It gives the name to a
person, place, thing, etc.
 Most nouns possess the category of number and
have a plural form (-s/ -es).
 Some words make their plural forms in a different
way.
 Others never change their singular forms to make
their plurals.
 Some nouns never occur without the plural marker.
Lexical and Grammatical Words
Adjectives
 Adjectives denote a property or quality of an
object. All adjectives fall into two groups:
gradable and non-gradable.
 Gradable adjectives take grammatical forms
and represent degrees of comparison:
positive, comparative, superlative.
 Adjectives in English may appear either
before a noun or after a verb, e.g. juicy apple,
get wet, be happy. Not all adjectives appear
in both positions.
Lexical and Grammatical Words
Verbs
 There are two classes of verbs – lexical and
auxiliary verbs. Lexical verbs denote actions
or states. Each verb has five associated
grammatical words, e.g., ask – asks – asked
– asking – asked; go – goes – went – going –
gone.
 Verbs like ask are regular, they form the
majority of verbs in English. Irregular verbs
like go have different forms.
Lexical and Grammatical Words
Adverbs
 Two classes of adverbs – degree and general.
 Degree adverbs are a small group of words like quite,
far, very, more, and most. They always appear with
either an adjective or a general adverb, e.g. She
drives very well. His story is quite interesting.
 General adverbs form a large class and may appear
without a degree adverb, e.g. He cooks well.
Adverbs denote degree, manner, place, time.
Adverbs have no inflected forms, they take
comparisons like adjectives.
Lexical and Grammatical Words
 Grammatical words comprise a small
class of words that includes pronouns,
auxiliary verbs, determiners,
prepositions, conjunctions. They are
also known as functional words, or
empty words.
 Grammatical words constitute a closed
class, i.e. they do not accept new
members.
Lexical and Grammatical Words
 Pronouns are words that take the place of
nouns, noun phrases. Different types:
personal (I, we, she), demonstrative (this,
those), possessive (mine, yours),
interrogative (whom, whose, which), etc.
 Auxiliary verbs such as have, do, did, will
determine the mood, tense, or aspect of
another verb in a verb phrase.
 Conjunctions serve to connect words,
phrases, clauses, or sentences, e.g. and, but,
for, or, nor, yet, so.
Lexical and Grammatical Words
 Determiners introduce nouns; they are
articles (a, an, the), quantifiers (some, much).
 Prepositions are words that show the
relationship between a noun or pronoun and
other words in a sentence. They indicate
locations in time or space (at, in), agency
(by), comparison (like, as . . . as), direction
(to, toward, through), place (at, by, on),
possession (of), purpose (for), source (from,
out of), and time (at, before, on), etc.
Morphological Structure
Words consist of morphemes.
 A morpheme is the smallest meaningful
unit of speech, which cannot be split up
into smaller segments.
 A morpheme is not autonomous.
Morphemes
occur in speech as
constituent parts of words, not
independently.
Morphological Structure
 A word may consist of a single morpheme or
contain several, e.g. shelf (one morpheme),
indisputable (3 morphemes) – in-disput-able.
 The morpheme in can be found in words
insignificant, inexpensive; the morpheme
able – in words excusable, reliable; the
morpheme dispute – in such words as
disputing, disputes.
Morphological Structure
 Some
morphemes have only one
phonological form, while others may
have a number of variants which are
known as allomorph.
 Pronounce: please, pleasing, pleasure,
pleasant.
 The representations of the given
morpheme are called allomorphs or
morphemic variants.
Morphological Structure
 An allomorph is a positional variant of
a morpheme occurring in a specific
environment and characterized by
complementary distribution.
 Cows, cats, pigs, horses, sheep, oxen, mice
Allomorphs occur among prefixes.
 The morpheme in may have the following
phonological forms:
 im occurs before bilabials – impossible;
 ir occurs before r – irregular;
 il occurs before 1 – illegal;
 in occurs before other consonants and
vowels – inability, indirect.
Morphological Structure
 Semantically morphemes fall into root-
morphemes and non-root
morphemes.
 The root-morphemes are the lexical
centers of the words, as the basic
constituent part of a word without which
the word is inconceivable.
Morphological Structure
 Words hairless, lofty, darkness, refill – the
root morphemes are hair, loft, dark, fill
 The root-morpheme is the lexical nucleus of
a word; it has an individual lexical meaning
shared by no other morpheme of the
language.
 The root-morpheme is the morpheme
common to a set of words making up a wordcluster, e.g. dance in to dance, dancer,
dancing.
Morphological Structure
 Non-root morphemes include inflectional
morphemes (inflections) and derivational
morphemes (affixes).
Morphological Structure
 Inflections carry only grammatical
meaning, they build different forms of
one and the same word, e.g. cheap,
cheaper, cheapest.
 Affixes supply the stem with
components of lexical and lexicogrammatical meaning. They are
classified into prefixes and suffixes, e.g.
chain – unchain, broad - broaden.
Morphological Structure
 A suffix is a derivational morpheme
following the stem and forming a new
derivative in a different part of speech or
a different word class, e.g., -en, -y, less in hearten, hearty, heartless.
 A prefix is a derivational morpheme
standing before the root and modifying
meaning, e.g., to hearten - to dishearten.
Morphological Structure
Structurally morphemes fall into
 free morphemes,
 bound morphemes,
 semi-free (semi-bound morphemes).
Morphological Structure
 A free morpheme is defined as one that can
occur by itself as a whole word, e.g. cloudy –
cloud, excusable – excuse. A great number
of root morphemes are free morphemes, e.g.
child in the words childhood, children,
childish is naturally qualified as a free
morpheme because it coincides with one of
the forms of the noun child.
Morphological Structure
 A bound morpheme is attached to another
and is a constituent part of a word. Bound
morphemes are of two types: inflectional
morphemes and derivational ones.
 Inflectional morphemes: watch – watches,
look – looked, etc.
 Derivational morphemes are affixes, e.g. ness, -ship, -ize, dis-, de- in the words like
happiness, relationship, to displease, to
demoralize.
Morphological Structure
 Semi-free or semi-bound morphemes can
function in a morphemic sequence both as an
affix and as a free morpheme, e.g. half-ready,
seaman, womanlike.
Morphological Structure
 According to the number of morphemes
words fall into monomorphic and
polymorphic.
 Monomorphic or root-words consist of
only one root-morpheme, e.g. long, car,
make, drive, etc.
Morphological Structure
 All polymorphic words according to the
number of root-morphemes are
classified into two subgroups:
monoradical or root words and
polyradical words, i.e. words which
consist of two or more roots.
Morphemic Segmentability
 As far as the complexity of the
morphemic structure of the word is
concerned all English words fall into two
large classes:
 segmentable words, those allowing of
further morphemic segmentation, e.g.,
quickly, fearless, agreement;
 non-segmentable words, e.g., house,
girl, cat, woman.
Morphemic Segmentability
 The main aim of the morphemic
analysis is to split a word into its
constituent morphemes, determine their
number and type.
 It's called the method of immediate
and ultimate constituents.
Morphemic Segmentability
DISAGREEMENT
DISAGREE
DIS
AGREE
MENT
Morphemic Segmentability
There are three types of morphemic
segmentability:
 complete,
 conditional and
 defective.
Morphemic Segmentability
Complete segmentability:
 the individual morphemes are clearly singled
out within the word and can easily be
isolated. The constituent morphemes are met
in other words with the same meaning,
 e.g. speechless – two morphemes: speech
and less.
 Speech → speech, speeches, less →
nameless, useless.
Morphemic Segmentability
Conditional segmentability:
 the segmentation into the constituent
morphemes is doubtful for some semantic
reasons, e.g. retain, contain, detain.
 The words can be split into the following
re–tain, con–tain, de–tain. The constituent
part -tain unites these three verbs, but we
cannot trace the common meaning in each
verb, consequently we cannot regard -tain as a
morpheme.
Morphemic Segmentability
 Similarly we may find re-, con-, de- in
other verbs (recall, remind, consist,
confuse, delay, defeat), but each group
of words does not share a common
meaning. Such morphemes are called
pseudo-morphemes or quasimorphemes.
Morphemic Segmentability
 Defective segmentability is the property of words
whose constituent morphemes are seldom or never
met in other words.
 A unique morpheme is isolated and understood as
meaningful because the constituent morphemes
display a more or less clear denotational meaning.
cranberry → two morphemes: cran and berry.
 The morpheme -berry occurs in strawberry,
blackberry, etc.
 Cran does not recur in any other English word, thus,
this morpheme is unique.
Thank you for your attention.
Don’t forget to get ready for the
seminar.

Понравилась статья? Поделить с друзьями:
  • The morphological meaning of the word
  • The morphological form of a word
  • The morphemic type of the word
  • The morphemic structure of the word types of morphemes
  • The morphemic structure of the english word