The meaning of the word joke

A joke is a display of humour in which words are used within a specific and well-defined narrative structure to make people laugh and is usually not meant to be interpreted literally. It usually takes the form of a story, often with dialogue, and ends in a punch line, whereby the humorous element of the story is revealed; this can be done using a pun or other type of word play, irony or sarcasm, logical incompatibility, hyperbole, or other means.[1] Linguist Robert Hetzron offers the definition:

A joke is a short humorous piece of oral literature in which the funniness culminates in the final sentence, called the punchline… In fact, the main condition is that the tension should reach its highest level at the very end. No continuation relieving the tension should be added. As for its being «oral,» it is true that jokes may appear printed, but when further transferred, there is no obligation to reproduce the text verbatim, as in the case of poetry.[2]

It is generally held that jokes benefit from brevity, containing no more detail than is needed to set the scene for the punchline at the end. In the case of riddle jokes or one-liners, the setting is implicitly understood, leaving only the dialogue and punchline to be verbalised. However, subverting these and other common guidelines can also be a source of humour—the shaggy dog story is an example of an anti-joke; although presented as a joke, it contains a long drawn-out narrative of time, place and character, rambles through many pointless inclusions and finally fails to deliver a punchline. Jokes are a form of humour, but not all humour is in the form of a joke. Some humorous forms which are not verbal jokes are: involuntary humour, situational humour, practical jokes, slapstick and anecdotes.

Identified as one of the simple forms of oral literature by the Dutch linguist André Jolles,[3] jokes are passed along anonymously. They are told in both private and public settings; a single person tells a joke to his friend in the natural flow of conversation, or a set of jokes is told to a group as part of scripted entertainment. Jokes are also passed along in written form or, more recently, through the internet.

Stand-up comics, comedians and slapstick work with comic timing and rhythm in their performance, and may rely on actions as well as on the verbal punchline to evoke laughter. This distinction has been formulated in the popular saying «A comic says funny things; a comedian says things funny».[note 1]

History in print

The Westcar Papyrus, dating to c. 1600 BC, contains an example of one of the earliest surviving jokes.[4]

Any joke documented from the past has been saved through happenstance rather than design. Jokes do not belong to refined culture, but rather to the entertainment and leisure of all classes. As such, any printed versions were considered ephemera, i.e., temporary documents created for a specific purpose and intended to be thrown away. Many of these early jokes deal with scatological and sexual topics, entertaining to all social classes but not to be valued and saved.

Various kinds of jokes have been identified in ancient pre-classical texts.[note 2] The oldest identified joke is an ancient Sumerian proverb from 1900 BC containing toilet humour: «Something which has never occurred since time immemorial; a young woman did not fart in her husband’s lap.» Its records were dated to the Old Babylonian period and the joke may go as far back as 2300 BC. The second oldest joke found, discovered on the Westcar Papyrus and believed to be about Sneferu, was from Ancient Egypt c. 1600 BC: «How do you entertain a bored pharaoh? You sail a boatload of young women dressed only in fishing nets down the Nile and urge the pharaoh to go catch a fish.» The tale of the three ox drivers from Adab completes the three known oldest jokes in the world. This is a comic triple dating back to 1200 BC Adab.[4] It concerns three men seeking justice from a king on the matter of ownership over a newborn calf, for whose birth they all consider themselves to be partially responsible. The king seeks advice from a priestess on how to rule the case, and she suggests a series of events involving the men’s households and wives. The final portion of the story (which included the punch line), has not survived intact, though legible fragments suggest it was bawdy in nature.

Jokes can be notoriously difficult to translate from language to language; particularly puns, which depend on specific words and not just on their meanings. For instance, Julius Caesar once sold land at a surprisingly cheap price to his lover Servilia, who was rumoured to be prostituting her daughter Tertia to Caesar in order to keep his favour. Cicero remarked that «conparavit Servilia hunc fundum tertia deducta.» The punny phrase, «tertia deducta», can be translated as «with one-third off (in price)», or «with Tertia putting out.»[5][6]

The earliest extant joke book is the Philogelos (Greek for The Laughter-Lover), a collection of 265 jokes written in crude ancient Greek dating to the fourth or fifth century AD.[7][8] The author of the collection is obscure[9] and a number of different authors are attributed to it, including «Hierokles and Philagros the grammatikos«, just «Hierokles», or, in the Suda, «Philistion».[10] British classicist Mary Beard states that the Philogelos may have been intended as a jokester’s handbook of quips to say on the fly, rather than a book meant to be read straight through.[10] Many of the jokes in this collection are surprisingly familiar, even though the typical protagonists are less recognisable to contemporary readers: the absent-minded professor, the eunuch, and people with hernias or bad breath.[7] The Philogelos even contains a joke similar to Monty Python’s «Dead Parrot Sketch».[7]

During the 15th century,[11] the printing revolution spread across Europe following the development of the movable type printing press. This was coupled with the growth of literacy in all social classes. Printers turned out Jestbooks along with Bibles to meet both lowbrow and highbrow interests of the populace. One early anthology of jokes was the Facetiae by the Italian Poggio Bracciolini, first published in 1470. The popularity of this jest book can be measured on the twenty editions of the book documented alone for the 15th century. Another popular form was a collection of jests, jokes and funny situations attributed to a single character in a more connected, narrative form of the picaresque novel. Examples of this are the characters of Rabelais in France, Till Eulenspiegel in Germany, Lazarillo de Tormes in Spain and Master Skelton in England. There is also a jest book ascribed to William Shakespeare, the contents of which appear to both inform and borrow from his plays. All of these early jestbooks corroborate both the rise in the literacy of the European populations and the general quest for leisure activities during the Renaissance in Europe.[11]

The practice of printers using jokes and cartoons as page fillers was also widely used in the broadsides and chapbooks of the 19th century and earlier. With the increase in literacy in the general population and the growth of the printing industry, these publications were the most common forms of printed material between the 16th and 19th centuries throughout Europe and North America. Along with reports of events, executions, ballads and verse, they also contained jokes. Only one of many broadsides archived in the Harvard library is described as «1706. Grinning made easy; or, Funny Dick’s unrivalled collection of curious, comical, odd, droll, humorous, witty, whimsical, laughable, and eccentric jests, jokes, bulls, epigrams, &c. With many other descriptions of wit and humour.»[12] These cheap publications, ephemera intended for mass distribution, were read alone, read aloud, posted and discarded.

There are many types of joke books in print today; a search on the internet provides a plethora of titles available for purchase. They can be read alone for solitary entertainment, or used to stock up on new jokes to entertain friends. Some people try to find a deeper meaning in jokes, as in «Plato and a Platypus Walk into a Bar… Understanding Philosophy Through Jokes».[13][note 3] However a deeper meaning is not necessary to appreciate their inherent entertainment value.[14] Magazines frequently use jokes and cartoons as filler for the printed page. Reader’s Digest closes out many articles with an (unrelated) joke at the bottom of the article. The New Yorker was first published in 1925 with the stated goal of being a «sophisticated humour magazine» and is still known for its cartoons.

Telling jokes

Telling a joke is a cooperative effort;[15][16] it requires that the teller and the audience mutually agree in one form or another to understand the narrative which follows as a joke. In a study of conversation analysis, the sociologist Harvey Sacks describes in detail the sequential organisation in the telling of a single joke. «This telling is composed, as for stories, of three serially ordered and adjacently placed types of sequences … the preface [framing], the telling, and the response sequences.»[17] Folklorists expand this to include the context of the joking. Who is telling what jokes to whom? And why is he telling them when?[18][19] The context of the joke telling in turn leads into a study of joking relationships, a term coined by anthropologists to refer to social groups within a culture who engage in institutionalised banter and joking.

Framing: «Have you heard the one…»

Framing is done with a (frequently formulaic) expression which keys the audience in to expect a joke. «Have you heard the one…», «Reminds me of a joke I heard…», «So, a lawyer and a doctor…»; these conversational markers are just a few examples of linguistic frames used to start a joke. Regardless of the frame used, it creates a social space and clear boundaries around the narrative which follows.[20] Audience response to this initial frame can be acknowledgement and anticipation of the joke to follow. It can also be a dismissal, as in «this is no joking matter» or «this is no time for jokes».

The performance frame serves to label joke-telling as a culturally marked form of communication. Both the performer and audience understand it to be set apart from the «real» world. «An elephant walks into a bar…»; a person sufficiently familiar with both the English language and the way jokes are told automatically understands that such a compressed and formulaic story, being told with no substantiating details, and placing an unlikely combination of characters into an unlikely setting and involving them in an unrealistic plot, is the start of a joke, and the story that follows is not meant to be taken at face value (i.e. it is non-bona-fide communication).[21] The framing itself invokes a play mode; if the audience is unable or unwilling to move into play, then nothing will seem funny.[22]

Telling

Following its linguistic framing the joke, in the form of a story, can be told. It is not required to be verbatim text like other forms of oral literature such as riddles and proverbs. The teller can and does modify the text of the joke, depending both on memory and the present audience. The important characteristic is that the narrative is succinct, containing only those details which lead directly to an understanding and decoding of the punchline. This requires that it support the same (or similar) divergent scripts which are to be embodied in the punchline.[23]

The narrative always contains a protagonist who becomes the «butt» or target of the joke.[citation needed] This labelling serves to develop and solidify stereotypes within the culture. It also enables researchers to group and analyse the creation, persistence and interpretation of joke cycles around a certain character. Some people are naturally better performers than others; however, anyone can tell a joke because the comic trigger is contained in the narrative text and punchline. A joke poorly told is still funny, unless errors or omissions make the intended relationship between the narrative and the punchline unintelligible.

Punchline

The punchline is intended to make the audience laugh. A linguistic interpretation of this punchline/response is elucidated by Victor Raskin in his Script-based Semantic Theory of Humour. Humour is evoked when a trigger contained in the punchline causes the audience to abruptly shift its understanding of the story from the primary (or more obvious) interpretation to a secondary, opposing interpretation. «The punchline is the pivot on which the joke text turns as it signals the shift between the [semantic] scripts necessary to interpret [re-interpret] the joke text.»[24] To produce the humour in the verbal joke, the two interpretations (i.e. scripts) need to both be compatible with the joke text and opposite or incompatible with each other.[25] Thomas R. Shultz, a psychologist, independently expands Raskin’s linguistic theory to include «two stages of incongruity: perception and resolution.» He explains that «… incongruity alone is insufficient to account for the structure of humour. […] Within this framework, humour appreciation is conceptualized as a biphasic sequence involving first the discovery of incongruity followed by a resolution of the incongruity.»[26] In the case of a joke, that resolution generates laughter.

This is the point at which the field of neurolinguistics offers some insight into the cognitive processing involved in this abrupt laughter at the punchline. Studies by the cognitive science researchers Coulson and Kutas directly address the theory of script switching articulated by Raskin in their work.[27] The article «Getting it: Human event-related brain response to jokes in good and poor comprehenders» measures brain activity in response to reading jokes.[28] Additional studies by others in the field support more generally the theory of two-stage processing of humour, as evidenced in the longer processing time they require.[29] In the related field of neuroscience, it has been shown that the expression of laughter is caused by two partially independent neuronal pathways: an «involuntary» or «emotionally driven» system and a «voluntary» system.[30] This study adds credence to the common experience when exposed to an off-colour joke; a laugh is followed in the next breath by a disclaimer: «Oh, that’s bad…» Here the multiple steps in cognition are clearly evident in the stepped response, the perception being processed just a breath faster than the resolution of the moral/ethical content in the joke.

Response

Expected response to a joke is laughter. The joke teller hopes the audience «gets it» and is entertained. This leads to the premise that a joke is actually an «understanding test» between individuals and groups.[31] If the listeners do not get the joke, they are not understanding the two scripts which are contained in the narrative as they were intended. Or they do «get it» and don’t laugh; it might be too obscene, too gross or too dumb for the current audience. A woman might respond differently to a joke told by a male colleague around the water cooler than she would to the same joke overheard in a women’s lavatory. A joke involving toilet humour may be funnier told on the playground at elementary school than on a college campus. The same joke will elicit different responses in different settings. The punchline in the joke remains the same, however, it is more or less appropriate depending on the current context.

Shifting contexts, shifting texts

The context explores the specific social situation in which joking occurs.[32] The narrator automatically modifies the text of the joke to be acceptable to different audiences, while at the same time supporting the same divergent scripts in the punchline. The vocabulary used in telling the same joke at a university fraternity party and to one’s grandmother might well vary. In each situation, it is important to identify both the narrator and the audience as well as their relationship with each other. This varies to reflect the complexities of a matrix of different social factors: age, sex, race, ethnicity, kinship, political views, religion, power relationships, etc. When all the potential combinations of such factors between the narrator and the audience are considered, then a single joke can take on infinite shades of meaning for each unique social setting.

The context, however, should not be confused with the function of the joking. «Function is essentially an abstraction made on the basis of a number of contexts».[33] In one long-term observation of men coming off the late shift at a local café, joking with the waitresses was used to ascertain sexual availability for the evening. Different types of jokes, going from general to topical into explicitly sexual humour signalled openness on the part of the waitress for a connection.[34] This study describes how jokes and joking are used to communicate much more than just good humour. That is a single example of the function of joking in a social setting, but there are others. Sometimes jokes are used simply to get to know someone better. What makes them laugh, what do they find funny? Jokes concerning politics, religion or sexual topics can be used effectively to gauge the attitude of the audience to any one of these topics. They can also be used as a marker of group identity, signalling either inclusion or exclusion for the group. Among pre-adolescents, «dirty» jokes allow them to share information about their changing bodies.[35] And sometimes joking is just simple entertainment for a group of friends.

Relationships

The context of joking in turn leads to a study of joking relationships, a term coined by anthropologists to refer to social groups within a culture who take part in institutionalised banter and joking. These relationships can be either one-way or a mutual back and forth between partners. «The joking relationship is defined as a peculiar combination of friendliness and antagonism. The behaviour is such that in any other social context it would express and arouse hostility; but it is not meant seriously and must not be taken seriously. There is a pretence of hostility along with a real friendliness. To put it in another way, the relationship is one of permitted disrespect.»[36] Joking relationships were first described by anthropologists within kinship groups in Africa. But they have since been identified in cultures around the world, where jokes and joking are used to mark and reinforce appropriate boundaries of a relationship.[37]

Electronic

The advent of electronic communications at the end of the 20th century introduced new traditions into jokes. A verbal joke or cartoon is emailed to a friend or posted on a bulletin board; reactions include a replied email with a  :-) or LOL, or a forward on to further recipients. Interaction is limited to the computer screen and for the most part solitary. While preserving the text of a joke, both context and variants are lost in internet joking; for the most part, emailed jokes are passed along verbatim.[38] The framing of the joke frequently occurs in the subject line: «RE: laugh for the day» or something similar. The forward of an email joke can increase the number of recipients exponentially.

Internet joking forces a re-evaluation of social spaces and social groups. They are no longer only defined by physical presence and locality, they also exist in the connectivity in cyberspace.[39] «The computer networks appear to make possible communities that, although physically dispersed, display attributes of the direct, unconstrained, unofficial exchanges folklorists typically concern themselves with».[40] This is particularly evident in the spread of topical jokes, «that genre of lore in which whole crops of jokes spring up seemingly overnight around some sensational event … flourish briefly and then disappear, as the mass media move on to fresh maimings and new collective tragedies».[41] This correlates with the new understanding of the internet as an «active folkloric space» with evolving social and cultural forces and clearly identifiable performers and audiences.[42]

A study by the folklorist Bill Ellis documented how an evolving cycle was circulated over the internet.[43] By accessing message boards that specialised in humour immediately following the 9/11 disaster, Ellis was able to observe in real-time both the topical jokes being posted electronically and responses to the jokes. «Previous folklore research has been limited to collecting and documenting successful jokes, and only after they had emerged and come to folklorists’ attention. Now, an Internet-enhanced collection creates a time machine, as it were, where we can observe what happens in the period before the risible moment, when attempts at humour are unsuccessful».[44] Access to archived message boards also enables us to track the development of a single joke thread in the context of a more complicated virtual conversation.[43]

Joke cycles

A joke cycle is a collection of jokes about a single target or situation which displays consistent narrative structure and type of humour.[45] Some well-known cycles are elephant jokes using nonsense humour, dead baby jokes incorporating black humour and light bulb jokes, which describe all kinds of operational stupidity. Joke cycles can centre on ethnic groups, professions (viola jokes), catastrophes, settings (…walks into a bar), absurd characters (wind-up dolls), or logical mechanisms which generate the humour (knock-knock jokes). A joke can be reused in different joke cycles; an example of this is the same Head & Shoulders joke refitted to the tragedies of Vic Morrow, Admiral Mountbatten and the crew of the Challenger space shuttle.[note 4][46] These cycles seem to appear spontaneously, spread rapidly across countries and borders only to dissipate after some time. Folklorists and others have studied individual joke cycles in an attempt to understand their function and significance within the culture.

Why did the chicken cross the road? To get to the other side.

Joke cycles circulated in the recent past include:

  • Conditional jokes
  • Bar jokes
  • Bellman jokes
  • Blonde joke, lawyer joke and Microsoft joke cycles
  • Burgenland jokes (Austria)
  • Carabinieri jokes (Italy)
  • Challenger (space shuttle) jokes[47]
  • Chernobyl jokes[48]
  • Chicken jokes
  • Two cow jokes
  • Dead baby jokes[49]
  • East Frisian jokes (Germany)
  • Essex girl joke cycle (United Kingdom)[50]
  • Helen Keller joke cycle[51]
  • Irish jokes
  • Desert island jokes
  • Jew and Polack joke cycles[52]
  • Jewish American Princess and Jewish Mother joke cycles[53]
  • Knock-knock jokes[54]
  • Lightbulb jokes[55]
  • Little Willie and Quadriplegic joke cycles[56]
  • Manta jokes (Germany)
  • NASA joke cycle[57]
  • Newfie joke cycle (Canada)[58]
  • Persian Gulf War jokes[59]
  • Policeman jokes (Hungary)
  • Polish jokes
  • Redneck jokes
  • Riddle jokes
  • Sardarji jokes (India)
  • Said the actress to the bishop jokes
  • Viola jokes[60]
  • Wind-up doll joke cycle[61]
  • Yo Mama jokes

Tragedies and catastrophes

As with the 9/11 disaster discussed above, cycles attach themselves to celebrities or national catastrophes such as the death of Diana, Princess of Wales, the death of Michael Jackson, and the Space Shuttle Challenger disaster. These cycles arise regularly as a response to terrible unexpected events which command the national news. An in-depth analysis of the Challenger joke cycle documents a change in the type of humour circulated following the disaster, from February to March 1986. «It shows that the jokes appeared in distinct ‘waves’, the first responding to the disaster with clever wordplay and the second playing with grim and troubling images associated with the event…The primary social function of disaster jokes appears to be to provide closure to an event that provoked communal grieving, by signalling that it was time to move on and pay attention to more immediate concerns».[62]

Ethnic jokes

The sociologist Christie Davies has written extensively on ethnic jokes told in countries around the world.[63] In ethnic jokes he finds that the «stupid» ethnic target in the joke is no stranger to the culture, but rather a peripheral social group (geographic, economic, cultural, linguistic) well known to the joke tellers.[64] So Americans tell jokes about Polacks and Italians, Germans tell jokes about Ostfriesens, and the English tell jokes about the Irish. In a review of Davies’ theories it is said that «For Davies, [ethnic] jokes are more about how joke tellers imagine themselves than about how they imagine those others who serve as their putative targets…The jokes thus serve to center one in the world – to remind people of their place and to reassure them that they are in it.»[65]

Absurdities and gallows humour

A third category of joke cycles identifies absurd characters as the butt: for example the grape, the dead baby or the elephant. Beginning in the 1960s, social and cultural interpretations of these joke cycles, spearheaded by the folklorist Alan Dundes, began to appear in academic journals. Dead baby jokes are posited to reflect societal changes and guilt caused by widespread use of contraception and abortion beginning in the 1960s.[note 5][66] Elephant jokes have been interpreted variously as stand-ins for American blacks during the Civil Rights Era[67] or as an «image of something large and wild abroad in the land captur[ing] the sense of counterculture» of the sixties.[68] These interpretations strive for a cultural understanding of the themes of these jokes which go beyond the simple collection and documentation undertaken previously by folklorists and ethnologists.

Classification systems

As folktales and other types of oral literature became collectables throughout Europe in the 19th century (Brothers Grimm et al.), folklorists and anthropologists of the time needed a system to organise these items. The Aarne–Thompson classification system was first published in 1910 by Antti Aarne, and later expanded by Stith Thompson to become the most renowned classification system for European folktales and other types of oral literature. Its final section addresses anecdotes and jokes, listing traditional humorous tales ordered by their protagonist; «This section of the Index is essentially a classification of the older European jests, or merry tales – humorous stories characterized by short, fairly simple plots. …»[69] Due to its focus on older tale types and obsolete actors (e.g., numbskull), the Aarne–Thompson Index does not provide much help in identifying and classifying the modern joke.

A more granular classification system used widely by folklorists and cultural anthropologists is the Thompson Motif Index, which separates tales into their individual story elements. This system enables jokes to be classified according to individual motifs included in the narrative: actors, items and incidents. It does not provide a system to classify the text by more than one element at a time while at the same time making it theoretically possible to classify the same text under multiple motifs.[70]

The Thompson Motif Index has spawned further specialised motif indices, each of which focuses on a single aspect of one subset of jokes. A sampling of just a few of these specialised indices have been listed under other motif indices. Here one can select an index for medieval Spanish folk narratives,[71] another index for linguistic verbal jokes,[72] and a third one for sexual humour.[73] To assist the researcher with this increasingly confusing situation, there are also multiple bibliographies of indices[74] as well as a how-to guide on creating your own index.[75]

Several difficulties have been identified with these systems of identifying oral narratives according to either tale types or story elements.[76] A first major problem is their hierarchical organisation; one element of the narrative is selected as the major element, while all other parts are arrayed subordinate to this. A second problem with these systems is that the listed motifs are not qualitatively equal; actors, items and incidents are all considered side-by-side.[77] And because incidents will always have at least one actor and usually have an item, most narratives can be ordered under multiple headings. This leads to confusion about both where to order an item and where to find it. A third significant problem is that the «excessive prudery» common in the middle of the 20th century means that obscene, sexual and scatological elements were regularly ignored in many of the indices.[78]

The folklorist Robert Georges has summed up the concerns with these existing classification systems:

…Yet what the multiplicity and variety of sets and subsets reveal is that folklore [jokes] not only takes many forms, but that it is also multifaceted, with purpose, use, structure, content, style, and function all being relevant and important. Any one or combination of these multiple and varied aspects of a folklore example [such as jokes] might emerge as dominant in a specific situation or for a particular inquiry.[79]

It has proven difficult to organise all different elements of a joke into a multi-dimensional classification system which could be of real value in the study and evaluation of this (primarily oral) complex narrative form.

The General Theory of Verbal Humour or GTVH, developed by the linguists Victor Raskin and Salvatore Attardo, attempts to do exactly this. This classification system was developed specifically for jokes and later expanded to include longer types of humorous narratives.[80] Six different aspects of the narrative, labelled Knowledge Resources or KRs, can be evaluated largely independently of each other, and then combined into a concatenated classification label. These six KRs of the joke structure include:

  1. Script Opposition (SO) references the script opposition included in Raskin’s SSTH. This includes, among others, themes such as real (unreal), actual (non-actual), normal (abnormal), possible (impossible).
  2. Logical Mechanism (LM) refers to the mechanism which connects the different scripts in the joke. These can range from a simple verbal technique like a pun to more complex LMs such as faulty logic or false analogies.
  3. Situation (SI) can include objects, activities, instruments, props needed to tell the story.
  4. Target (TA) identifies the actor(s) who become the «butt» of the joke. This labelling serves to develop and solidify stereotypes of ethnic groups, professions, etc.
  5. Narrative strategy (NS) addresses the narrative format of the joke, as either a simple narrative, a dialogue, or a riddle. It attempts to classify the different genres and subgenres of verbal humour. In a subsequent study Attardo expands the NS to include oral and printed humorous narratives of any length, not just jokes.[80]
  6. Language (LA) «…contains all the information necessary for the verbalization of a text. It is responsible for the exact wording …and for the placement of the functional elements.»[81]

As development of the GTVH progressed, a hierarchy of the KRs was established to partially restrict the options for lower-level KRs depending on the KRs defined above them. For example, a lightbulb joke (SI) will always be in the form of a riddle (NS). Outside of these restrictions, the KRs can create a multitude of combinations, enabling a researcher to select jokes for analysis which contain only one or two defined KRs. It also allows for an evaluation of the similarity or dissimilarity of jokes depending on the similarity of their labels. «The GTVH presents itself as a mechanism … of generating [or describing] an infinite number of jokes by combining the various values that each parameter can take. … Descriptively, to analyze a joke in the GTVH consists of listing the values of the 6 KRs (with the caveat that TA and LM may be empty).»[82] This classification system provides a functional multi-dimensional label for any joke, and indeed any verbal humour.

Joke and humour research

Many academic disciplines lay claim to the study of jokes (and other forms of humour) as within their purview. Fortunately, there are enough jokes, good, bad and worse, to go around. The studies of jokes from each of the interested disciplines bring to mind the tale of the blind men and an elephant where the observations, although accurate reflections of their own competent methodological inquiry, frequently fail to grasp the beast in its entirety. This attests to the joke as a traditional narrative form which is indeed complex, concise and complete in and of itself.[83] It requires a «multidisciplinary, interdisciplinary, and cross-disciplinary field of inquiry»[84] to truly appreciate these nuggets of cultural insight.[note 6][85]

Psychology

Sigmund Freud was one of the first modern scholars to recognise jokes as an important object of investigation.[86] In his 1905 study Jokes and their Relation to the Unconscious[87] Freud describes the social nature of humour and illustrates his text with many examples of contemporary Viennese jokes.[88] His work is particularly noteworthy in this context because Freud distinguishes in his writings between jokes, humour and the comic.[89] These are distinctions which become easily blurred in many subsequent studies where everything funny tends to be gathered under the umbrella term of «humour», making for a much more diffuse discussion.

Since the publication of Freud’s study, psychologists have continued to explore humour and jokes in their quest to explain, predict and control an individual’s «sense of humour». Why do people laugh? Why do people find something funny? Can jokes predict character, or vice versa, can character predict the jokes an individual laughs at? What is a «sense of humour»? A current review of the popular magazine Psychology Today lists over 200 articles discussing various aspects of humour; in psychological jargon, the subject area has become both an emotion to measure and a tool to use in diagnostics and treatment. A new psychological assessment tool, the Values in Action Inventory developed by the American psychologists Christopher Peterson and Martin Seligman includes humour (and playfulness) as one of the core character strengths of an individual. As such, it could be a good predictor of life satisfaction.[90] For psychologists, it would be useful to measure both how much of this strength an individual has and how it can be measurably increased.

A 2007 survey of existing tools to measure humour identified more than 60 psychological measurement instruments.[91] These measurement tools use many different approaches to quantify humour along with its related states and traits. There are tools to measure an individual’s physical response by their smile; the Facial Action Coding System (FACS) is one of several tools used to identify any one of multiple types of smiles.[92] Or the laugh can be measured to calculate the funniness response of an individual; multiple types of laughter have been identified. It must be stressed here that both smiles and laughter are not always a response to something funny. In trying to develop a measurement tool, most systems use «jokes and cartoons» as their test materials. However, because no two tools use the same jokes, and across languages this would not be feasible, how does one determine that the assessment objects are comparable? Moving on, whom does one ask to rate the sense of humour of an individual? Does one ask the person themselves, an impartial observer, or their family, friends and colleagues? Furthermore, has the current mood of the test subjects been considered; someone with a recent death in the family might not be much prone to laughter. Given the plethora of variants revealed by even a superficial glance at the problem,[93] it becomes evident that these paths of scientific inquiry are mined with problematic pitfalls and questionable solutions.

The psychologist Willibald Ruch [de] has been very active in the research of humour. He has collaborated with the linguists Raskin and Attardo on their General Theory of Verbal Humour (GTVH) classification system. Their goal is to empirically test both the six autonomous classification types (KRs) and the hierarchical ordering of these KRs. Advancement in this direction would be a win-win for both fields of study; linguistics would have empirical verification of this multi-dimensional classification system for jokes, and psychology would have a standardised joke classification with which they could develop verifiably comparable measurement tools.

Linguistics

«The linguistics of humor has made gigantic strides forward in the last decade and a half and replaced the psychology of humor as the most advanced theoretical approach to the study of this important and universal human faculty.»[94] This recent statement by one noted linguist and humour researcher describes, from his perspective, contemporary linguistic humour research. Linguists study words, how words are strung together to build sentences, how sentences create meaning which can be communicated from one individual to another, and how our interaction with each other using words creates discourse. Jokes have been defined above as oral narratives in which words and sentences are engineered to build toward a punchline. The linguist’s question is: what exactly makes the punchline funny? This question focuses on how the words used in the punchline create humour, in contrast to the psychologist’s concern (see above) with the audience’s response to the punchline. The assessment of humour by psychologists «is made from the individual’s perspective; e.g. the phenomenon associated with responding to or creating humor and not a description of humor itself.»[95] Linguistics, on the other hand, endeavours to provide a precise description of what makes a text funny.[96]

Two major new linguistic theories have been developed and tested within the last decades. The first was advanced by Victor Raskin in «Semantic Mechanisms of Humor», published 1985.[97] While being a variant on the more general concepts of the incongruity theory of humour, it is the first theory to identify its approach as exclusively linguistic. The Script-based Semantic Theory of Humour (SSTH) begins by identifying two linguistic conditions which make a text funny. It then goes on to identify the mechanisms involved in creating the punchline. This theory established the semantic/pragmatic foundation of humour as well as the humour competence of speakers.[note 7][98]

Several years later the SSTH was incorporated into a more expansive theory of jokes put forth by Raskin and his colleague Salvatore Attardo. In the General Theory of Verbal Humour, the SSTH was relabelled as a Logical Mechanism (LM) (referring to the mechanism which connects the different linguistic scripts in the joke) and added to five other independent Knowledge Resources (KR). Together these six KRs could now function as a multi-dimensional descriptive label for any piece of humorous text.

Linguistics has developed further methodological tools which can be applied to jokes: discourse analysis and conversation analysis of joking. Both of these subspecialties within the field focus on «naturally occurring» language use, i.e. the analysis of real (usually recorded) conversations. One of these studies has already been discussed above, where Harvey Sacks describes in detail the sequential organisation in telling a single joke.[99] Discourse analysis emphasises the entire context of social joking, the social interaction which cradles the words.

Folklore and anthropology

Folklore and cultural anthropology have perhaps the strongest claims on jokes as belonging to their bailiwick. Jokes remain one of the few remaining forms of traditional folk literature transmitted orally in western cultures. Identified as one of the «simple forms» of oral literature by André Jolles in 1930,[3] they have been collected and studied since there were folklorists and anthropologists abroad in the lands. As a genre they were important enough at the beginning of the 20th century to be included under their own heading in the Aarne–Thompson index first published in 1910: Anecdotes and jokes.

Beginning in the 1960s, cultural researchers began to expand their role from collectors and archivists of «folk ideas»[85] to a more active role of interpreters of cultural artefacts. One of the foremost scholars active during this transitional time was the folklorist Alan Dundes. He started asking questions of tradition and transmission with the key observation that «No piece of folklore continues to be transmitted unless it means something, even if neither the speaker nor the audience can articulate what that meaning might be.»[100] In the context of jokes, this then becomes the basis for further research. Why is the joke told right now? Only in this expanded perspective is an understanding of its meaning to the participants possible.

This questioning resulted in a blossoming of monographs to explore the significance of many joke cycles. What is so funny about absurd nonsense elephant jokes? Why make light of dead babies? In an article on contemporary German jokes about Auschwitz and the Holocaust, Dundes justifies this research: «Whether one finds Auschwitz jokes funny or not is not an issue. This material exists and should be recorded. Jokes are always an important barometer of the attitudes of a group. The jokes exist and they obviously must fill some psychic need for those individuals who tell them and those who listen to them.»[101] A stimulating generation of new humour theories flourishes like mushrooms in the undergrowth: Elliott Oring’s theoretical discussions on «appropriate ambiguity» and Amy Carrell’s hypothesis of an «audience-based theory of verbal humor (1993)» to name just a few.

In his book Humor and Laughter: An Anthropological Approach,[37] the anthropologist Mahadev Apte presents a solid case for his own academic perspective.[102] «Two axioms underlie my discussion, namely, that humor is by and large culture based and that humor can be a major conceptual and methodological tool for gaining insights into cultural systems.» Apte goes on to call for legitimising the field of humour research as «humorology»; this would be a field of study incorporating an interdisciplinary character of humour studies.[103]

While the label «humorology» has yet to become a household word, great strides are being made in the international recognition of this interdisciplinary field of research. The International Society for Humor Studies was founded in 1989 with the stated purpose to «promote, stimulate and encourage the interdisciplinary study of humour; to support and cooperate with local, national, and international organizations having similar purposes; to organize and arrange meetings; and to issue and encourage publications concerning the purpose of the society». It also publishes Humor: International Journal of Humor Research and holds yearly conferences to promote and inform its speciality.

Physiology of laughter

Three quarter length portrait of sixty-year-old man, balding, with white hair and long white bushy beard, with heavy eyebrows shading his eyes looking thoughtfully into the distance, wearing a wide lapelled jacket.

Charles Darwin in his later years

In 1872, Charles Darwin published one of the first «comprehensive and in many ways remarkably accurate description of laughter in terms of respiration, vocalization, facial action and gesture and posture» (Laughter).[104] In this early study Darwin raises further questions about who laughs and why they laugh; the myriad responses since then illustrate the complexities of this behaviour. To understand laughter in humans and other primates, the science of gelotology (from the Greek gelos, meaning laughter) has been established; it is the study of laughter and its effects on the body from both a psychological and physiological perspective. While jokes can provoke laughter, laughter cannot be used as a one-to-one marker of jokes because there are multiple stimuli to laughter, humour being just one of them. The other six causes of laughter listed are social context, ignorance, anxiety, derision, acting apology, and tickling.[105] As such, the study of laughter is a secondary albeit entertaining perspective in an understanding of jokes.

Computational humour

Computational humour is a new field of study which uses computers to model humour;[106] it bridges the disciplines of computational linguistics and artificial intelligence. A primary ambition of this field is to develop computer programs which can both generate a joke and recognise a text snippet as a joke. Early programming attempts have dealt almost exclusively with punning because this lends itself to simple straightforward rules. These primitive programs display no intelligence; instead, they work off a template with a finite set of pre-defined punning options upon which to build.

More sophisticated computer joke programs have yet to be developed. Based on our understanding of the SSTH / GTVH humour theories, it is easy to see why. The linguistic scripts (a.k.a. frames) referenced in these theories include, for any given word, a «large chunk of semantic information surrounding the word and evoked by it […] a cognitive structure internalized by the native speaker».[107] These scripts extend much further than the lexical definition of a word; they contain the speaker’s complete knowledge of the concept as it exists in his world. As insentient machines, computers lack the encyclopaedic scripts which humans gain through life experience. They also lack the ability to gather the experiences needed to build wide-ranging semantic scripts and understand language in a broader context, a context that any child picks up in daily interaction with his environment.

Further development in this field must wait until computational linguists have succeeded in programming a computer with an ontological semantic natural language processing system. It is only «the most complex linguistic structures [which] can serve any formal and/or computational treatment of humor well».[108] Toy systems (i.e. dummy punning programs) are completely inadequate to the task. Despite the fact that the field of computational humour is small and underdeveloped, it is encouraging to note the many interdisciplinary efforts which are currently underway.[109] As this field grows in both understanding and methodology, it provides an ideal testbed for humour theories; the rules must firstly be clearly defined in order to write a computer program around a theory.

International Joke Day

1 July is celebrated around the world as International Joke Day.[citation needed]

See also

  • List of humour research publications

Notes

  1. ^ Generally attributed to Ed Wynn
  2. ^ In 2008, British TV channel Dave commissioned a team of academics, led by humour expert Paul McDonald from the University of Wolverhampton, to research the world’s oldest examples of recorded humour. Because humour may be difficult to define their condition was «a clear set-up and punch line structure». In review, McDonald stated: «… jokes have varied over the years, with some taking the question and answer format while others are witty proverbs or riddles. What they all share, however, is a willingness to deal with taboos and a degree of rebellion. Modern puns, Essex girl jokes and toilet humour can all be traced back to the very earliest jokes identified in this research.» Joseph 2008
  3. ^ NPR Interview with the authors Cathcart and Klein can be found at https://www.npr.org/2007/05/13/10158510/joking-and-learning-about-philosophy
  4. ^ How do we know that ___ had dandruff? They found his/her head and shoulders on the ___.
  5. ^ Contraceptive pills were first approved for use in the United States in 1960.
  6. ^ Our focus here is with the contemporary state of joke research. A more extensive survey of the history of various humour theories can be found under the topic theories of humor.
  7. ^ i.e. The necessary and sufficient conditions for a text to be funny

References

Footnotes

  1. ^ Sløk-Andersen, Beate (2019). «The Butt of the Joke?: Laughter and Potency in the Becoming of Good Soldiers» (PDF). Cultural Analysis. Denmark: The University of California. 17 (1): 25–56. Retrieved 1 January 2021.
  2. ^ Hetzron 1991, pp. 65–66.
  3. ^ a b Jolles 1930.
  4. ^ a b Joseph 2008.
  5. ^ Sadler, J. D. (1982). «Latin Paronomasia». The Classical Journal. 78 (2): 138–141. ISSN 0009-8353. JSTOR 3297064.
  6. ^ Low, Peter Alan (2011-03-01). «Translating jokes and puns». Perspectives. 19 (1): 59–70. doi:10.1080/0907676X.2010.493219. ISSN 0907-676X. S2CID 145706281.
  7. ^ a b c Adams 2008.
  8. ^ Beard 2014, p. 185.
  9. ^ Beard 2014, pp. 186–188.
  10. ^ a b Beard 2014, p. 188.
  11. ^ a b Ward & Waller 2000.
  12. ^ Lane 1905.
  13. ^ Cathcart & Klein 2007.
  14. ^ Berry 2013.
  15. ^ Raskin 1985, p. 103.
  16. ^ Attardo & Chabanne 1992.
  17. ^ Sacks 1974, pp. 337–353.
  18. ^ Dundes 1980, pp. 20–32.
  19. ^ Bauman 1975.
  20. ^ Sims & Stephens 2005, p. 141.
  21. ^ Raskin 1992.
  22. ^ Ellis 2002, p. 3; Marcus 2001.
  23. ^ Toelken 1996, p. 55.
  24. ^ Carrell 2008, p. 308.
  25. ^ Raskin 1985, p. 99.
  26. ^ Shultz 1976, pp. 12–13; Carrell 2008, p. 312.
  27. ^ Coulson & Kutas 1998.
  28. ^ Coulson & Kutas 2001, pp. 71–74.
  29. ^ Attardo 2008, pp. 125–126.
  30. ^ Wild et al. 2003.
  31. ^ Sacks 1974, p. 350.
  32. ^ Dundes 1980, p. 23.
  33. ^ Dundes 1980, pp. 23–24.
  34. ^ Walle 1976; Oring 2008, p. 201.
  35. ^ Sims & Stephens 2005, p. 39.
  36. ^ Radcliffe-Brown 1940, p. 196.
  37. ^ a b Apte 1985.
  38. ^ Frank 2009, pp. 99–100.
  39. ^ Mason 1998.
  40. ^ Dorst 1990, pp. 180–181.
  41. ^ Dorst 1990.
  42. ^ Dorst 1990, p. 183.
  43. ^ a b Ellis 2002.
  44. ^ Ellis 2002, p. 2.
  45. ^ Salvatore Attardo (2001). Humorous Texts: A Semantic and Pragmatic Analysis. Walter de Gruyter. pp. 69–. ISBN 978-3-11-017068-9.
  46. ^ Gruner 1997, pp. 142–143.
  47. ^ Smyth 1986; Oring 1987.
  48. ^ Laszlo 1988.
  49. ^ Dundes 1979.
  50. ^ Davies 1998.
  51. ^ Hirsch & Barrick 1980.
  52. ^ Dundes 1971.
  53. ^ Dundes 1985.
  54. ^ Weeks 2015.
  55. ^ Dundes 1981; Kerman 1980.
  56. ^ Davies 1999.
  57. ^ Simons 1986; Smyth 1986; Oring 1987.
  58. ^ Davies 2002.
  59. ^ Kitchener 1991; Dundes & Pagter 1991.
  60. ^ Rahkonen 2000.
  61. ^ Hirsch 1964.
  62. ^ Ellis 1991.
  63. ^ Davies 1990.
  64. ^ Davies 2008, pp. 163–165.
  65. ^ Oring 2000.
  66. ^ Dundes 1987, pp. 3–14.
  67. ^ Dundes 1987, pp. 41–54.
  68. ^ Oring 2008, p. 194.
  69. ^ Brunvand 1968, p. 238; Dundes 1997.
  70. ^ Dundes 1997.
  71. ^ Goldberg 1998.
  72. ^ Lew 1996.
  73. ^ Legman 1968.
  74. ^ Azzolina 1987.
  75. ^ Jason 2000.
  76. ^ Apo 1997.
  77. ^ Dundes 1962.
  78. ^ Dundes 1997, p. 198.
  79. ^ Georges 1997, p. 111.
  80. ^ a b Attardo 2001.
  81. ^ Attardo 1994, p. 223.
  82. ^ Attardo 2001, p. 27.
  83. ^ Attardo & Chabanne 1992, p. 172.
  84. ^ Apte 1988, p. 7.
  85. ^ a b Dundes 1972.
  86. ^ Carrell 2008, p. 304.
  87. ^ Freud 1905.
  88. ^ Oring 1984.
  89. ^ Morreall 2008, p. 224.
  90. ^ Ruch 2008, p. 47.
  91. ^ Ruch 2008, p. 58.
  92. ^ Furnham 2014.
  93. ^ Ruch 2008, pp. 40–45.
  94. ^ Raskin 1992, p. 91.
  95. ^ Ruch 2008, p. 19.
  96. ^ Ruch 2008, p. 25.
  97. ^ Raskin 1985.
  98. ^ Attardo 2001, p. 114.
  99. ^ Sacks 1974.
  100. ^ Dundes & Pagter 1987, p. vii.
  101. ^ Dundes & Hauschild 1983, p. 250.
  102. ^ Apte 2002.
  103. ^ Apte 1988.
  104. ^ Ruch 2008, p. 24.
  105. ^ Giles & Oxford 1970; Attardo 2008, pp. 116–117.
  106. ^ Mulder & Nijholt 2002.
  107. ^ Raskin 1985, p. 46.
  108. ^ Raskin 2008, p. 17/349.
  109. ^ Hempelmann & Samson 2008, p. 354.

Bibliography

  • Adams, Stephen (2008). «Dead parrot sketch is 1600 years old: It’s long been held that the old jokes are the best jokes — and Monty Python’s Dead Parrot sketch is no different». The Telegraph.
  • Apo, Satu (1997). «Motif». In Green, Thomas (ed.). Folklore An Encyclopedia of Beliefs, Customs, Tales, Music, and Art. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO. pp. 563–564. ISBN 9780874369861.
  • Apte, Mahadev L. (1985). Humor and Laughter: An Anthropological Approach. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
  • Apte, Mahadev L. (1988). «Disciplinary boundaries in humorology: An anthropologist’s ruminations». Humor: International Journal of Humor Research. 1 (1): 5–25. doi:10.1515/humr.1988.1.1.5. S2CID 143869276.
  • Apte, Mahadev L. (2002). «Author Review of Humor and Laughter: an Anthropological Approach». Retrieved 10 August 2015.
  • Attardo, Salvatore (1994). Linguistic Theories of Humor. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
  • Attardo, Salvatore (2001). Humorous Texts: A Semantic and Pragmatic Analysis. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter.
  • Attardo, Salvatore (2008). «A primer for the linguistics of humor». In Raskin, Victor (ed.). Primer of Humor Research. Humor Research. Vol. 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 101–156.
  • Attardo, Salvatore; Chabanne, Jean-Charles (1992). «Jokes as a text type». Humor: International Journal of Humor Research. 5 (1/2): 165–176. doi:10.1515/humr.1992.5.1-2.165. S2CID 144805109.
  • Azzolina, David (1987). Tale type- and motif-indices: An annotated bibliography. New York: Garland.
  • Beard, Mary (2014), Laughter in Ancient Rome: On Joking, Tickling, and Cracking Up, Berkeley, Los Angeles, and London: University of California Press, ISBN 978-0-520-95820-3
  • Bauman, Richard (1975). «Verbal Art as Performance». American Anthropologist. New Series. 77 (2): 290–311. doi:10.1525/aa.1975.77.2.02a00030. JSTOR 674535.
  • Berry, William (2013). «The Joke’s On Who?». Psychology Today (Feb 2013).
  • Bronner, Simon J., ed. (2007). The Meaning of folklore: the Analytical Essays of Alan Dundes. Logan, UT: Utah State University Press.
  • Brunvand, Jan Harald (1968). The Study of American Folklore. New York, London: W.W. Norton. ISBN 9780393098037.
  • Carrell, Amy (2008). «Historical Views of Humor» (PDF). In Raskin, Victor (ed.). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 303–332.
  • Cathcart, Thomas; Klein, Daniel (2007). Plato and a Platypus Walk into a Bar… Understanding Philosophy Through Jokes. New York: Penguin Books.
  • Coulson, Seana; Kutas, Marta (1998). «Frame-shifting and sentential integration». USCD Cognitive Science Technical Report. San Diego, CA: Technical Report CogSci.UCSD-98.03. 4 (3–4).
  • Coulson, Seana; Kutas, Marta (2001). «Getting it: Human event-related brain response to jokes in good and poor comprehenders». Neuroscience Letters. 316 (2): 71–74. doi:10.1016/s0304-3940(01)02387-4. PMID 11742718. S2CID 14789987.
  • Davies, Christie (1990). Ethnic Humor Around the World: A comparative Analysis. Bloomington: Indiana University Press.
  • Davies, Christie (1998). Jokes and Their Relation to Society. Walter de Gruyter. pp. 186–189. ISBN 978-3-11-016104-5.
  • Davies, Christie (1999). «Jokes on the Death of Diana». In Walter, Julian Anthony; Walter, Tony (eds.). The Mourning for Diana. Berg Publishers. p. 255. ISBN 978-1-85973-238-0.
  • Davies, Christie (2002). «Jokes about Newfies and Jokes told by Newfoundlanders». Mirth of Nations. Transaction Publishers. ISBN 978-0-7658-0096-1.
  • Davies, Christie (2008). «Undertaking the Comparative Study of Humor». In Raskin, Victor (ed.). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 157–182.
  • Dorst, John (1990). «Tags and Burners, Cycles and Networks: Folklore in the Telectronic Age». Journal of Folklore Research. Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press. 27 (3): 61–108.
  • Douglas, Mary (1975). «Jokes». In Mukerji, Chandra; Schudson, Michael (eds.). Rethinking Popular Culture: Contemporary Perspectives in Cultural Studies. Berkeley, CA: University of California.
  • Dundes, Alan (1962). «From Etic to Emic Units in the Structural Study of Folktales». Journal of American Folklore. 75 (296): 95–105. doi:10.2307/538171. JSTOR 538171.
  • Dundes, Alan (1971). «A Study of Ethnic Slurs: The Jew and the Polack in the United States». Journal of American Folklore. 84 (332): 186–203. doi:10.2307/538989. JSTOR 538989.
  • Dundes, Alan (1972). «Folk ideas as units of World View». In Bauman, Richard; Paredes, Americo (eds.). Toward New Perspectives in Folklore. Bloomington, IN: Trickster Press. pp. 120–134.
  • Dundes, Alan (July 1979). «The Dead Baby Joke Cycle». Western Folklore. 38 (3): 145–157. doi:10.2307/1499238. JSTOR 1499238. PMID 11633558.
  • Dundes, Alan (1980). «Texture, text and context». Interpreting Folklore. Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press. pp. 20–32.
  • Dundes, Alan (1981). «Many Hands Make Light Work or Caught in the Act of Screwing in Light Bulbs». Western Folklore. 40 (3): 261–266. doi:10.2307/1499697. JSTOR 1499697.
  • Dundes, Alan (October–December 1985). «The J. A. P. and the J. A. M. in American Jokelore». The Journal of American Folklore. 98 (390): 456–475. doi:10.2307/540367. JSTOR 540367.
  • Dundes, Alan (1987). Cracking jokes: Studies of Sick Humor Cycles & Stereotypes. Berkeley: Ten Speed Press.
  • Dundes, Alan, ed. (1991). «Folk Humor». Mother Wit from the Laughing Barrel: Readings in the Interpretation of Afro-American Folklore. University Press of Mississippi. p. 612. ISBN 978-0-87805-478-7.
  • Dundes, Alan (1997). «The Motif-Index and the Tale Type Index: A Critique». Journal of Folklore Research. Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press. 34 (3): 195–202. JSTOR 3814885.
  • Dundes, Alan; Hauschild, Thomas (October 1983). «Auschwitz Jokes». Western Folklore. 42 (4): 249–260. doi:10.2307/1499500. JSTOR 1499500.
  • Dundes, Alan; Pagter, Carl R. (1987). When You’re Up to Your Ass in Alligators: More Urban Folklore from the Paperwork Empire. Detroit: Wayne State University Press.
  • Dundes, Alan; Pagter, Carl R. (1991). «The mobile SCUD Missile Launcher and other Persian Gulf Warlore: An American Folk Image of Saddam Hussein’s Iraq». Western Folklore. 50 (3): 303–322. doi:10.2307/1499881. JSTOR 1499881.
  • Ellis, Bill (1991). «The Last Thing … Said: The Challenger Disaster Jokes and Closure». International Folklore Review. London (8): 110–124.
  • Ellis, Bill (2002). «Making a Big Apple Crumble». New Directions in Folklore (6). Archived from the original on 2016-10-22. Retrieved 2015-08-18.
  • Frank, Russel (2009). «The Forward as Folklore: Studying E-Mailed Humor». In Blank, Trevor J. (ed.). Folklore and the Internet. Logan, UT: Utah State University Press. pp. 98–122.
  • Freedman, Matt; Hoffman, Paul (1980). How Many Zen Buddhists Does It Take to Screw In a Light Bulb?. New York.
  • Freud, Sigmund (1905). Der Witz und seine Beziehung zum Unbewußten. Leipzig, Vienna: Franz Deuticke.
  • Furnham, Adrian (Oct 30, 2014). «The Surprising Psychology of Smiling: Natural or fake, each smile tells you something important about its wearer». Psychology Today.[dead link]
  • Georges, Robert A. (1997). «The Centrality in Folkloristics of Motif and Tale Type». Journal of Folklore Research. Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press. 34 (3): 195–202. JSTOR 3814885.
  • Georges, Robert A.; Jones, Michael Owen (1995). Folkloristics : an Introduction. Bloomington and Indianapolis: Indiana University Press.
  • Giles, H.; Oxford, G.S. (1970). «Towards a multidimensional theory of laughter causation and its social implications». Bulletin of British Psychology Society. 23: 97–105.
  • Goldberg, Harriet (1998). «Motif-Index of Medieval Spanish Folk Narratives». Medieval & Renaissance Texts & Studies. Tempe, AZ.
  • Gruner, Charles R. (1997). The Game of Humor: A Comprehensive Theory of Why We Laugh. Piscataway, NJ: Transaction Publishers. ISBN 978-0-7658-0659-8.
  • Hempelmann, Christian; Samson, Andrea C. (2008). «Cartoons: Drawn jokes?». In Raskin, Victor (ed.). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 609–640.
  • Hetzron, Robert (1991). «On the structure of punchlines». Humor: International Journal of Humor Research. 4 (1): 61–108. doi:10.1515/humr.1991.4.1.61. S2CID 143907462.
  • Hirsch, K.; Barrick, M.E. (1980). «The Helen Keller Joke Cycle». Journal of American Folklore. 93 (370): 441–448. doi:10.2307/539874. JSTOR 539874.
  • Hirsch, Robin (1964). «Wind-Up Dolls». Western Folklore. 23 (2): 107–110. doi:10.2307/1498259. JSTOR 1498259.
  • Jason, Heda (2000). «Motif, type, and genre: a manual for compilation of indices & a bibliography of indices and indexing». FF Communications. Helsinki: Suomalainen Tiedeakatemia. 273.
  • Jolles, André (1930). Einfache Formen. Legende, Sage, Mythe, Rätsel, Spruch, Kasus, Memorabile, Märchen, Witz. Halle (Saale): Forschungsinstitut für Neuere Philologie Leipzig: Neugermanistische Abteilung; 2.
  • Joseph, John (July 31, 2008). «World’s oldest joke traced back to 1900 BC». Reuters. Retrieved May 21, 2017.
  • Kerman, Judith B. (1980). «The Light-Bulb Jokes: Americans Look at Social Action Processes». Journal of American Folklore. 93 (370): 454–458. doi:10.2307/539876. JSTOR 539876.
  • Kitchener, Amy (1991). Explosive Jokes: A collection of Persian Gulf War Humor. Unpublished Manuscript.
  • Lane, William Coolidge, ed. (1905). Catalogue of English and American chapbooks and broadside ballads in Harvard University Library. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.
  • Laszlo, Kurti (July–September 1988). «The Politics of Joking: Popular Response to Chernobyl». The Journal of American Folklore. 101 (401): 324–334. doi:10.2307/540473. JSTOR 540473.
  • Legman, Gershon (1968). Rationale of the Dirty Joke: an Analysis of Sexual Humor. New York: Simon & Schuster.
  • Lew, Robert (April 1996). An Ambiguity-based theory of the linguistic verbal joke in English (PDF) (PhD thesis). Poznań, Poland: Adam Mickiewicz University. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2015-09-24. Retrieved 2015-09-08.
  • Marcus, Adam (2001). «Laughter Shelved in Medicine Cabinet: America’s sense of humor blunted by week of shock». Healingwell.com (Sept. 19).
  • Mason, Bruce Lionel (1998). «E-Texts: The Orality and Literacy Issue Revisited». Oral Traditions. Vol. 13. Columbia, MO: Center for Studies in Oral Tradition.
  • Mintz, Lawrence E. (2008). «Humor and Popular Culture». In Raskin, Victor (ed.). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 281–302.
  • Morreall, John (2008). «Philosophy and Religion». In Raskin, Victor (ed.). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 211–242.
  • Mulder, M.P.; Nijholt, A. (September 2002). «Humour Research: State of the Art» (PDF). University of Twente, Netherlands: Center of Telematics and Information Technology. Retrieved 10 August 2015.
  • Nilsen, Alleen; Nilsen, Don C. (2008). «Literature and Humor». In Raskin, Victor (ed.). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. –––.
  • Oring, Elliott (1984). The Jokes of Sigmund Freud: a Study in Humor and Jewish Identity. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. ISBN 9780812279108.
  • Oring, Elliott (July–September 1987). «Jokes and the Discourse on Disaster». The Journal of American Folklore. 100 (397): 276–286. doi:10.2307/540324. JSTOR 540324.
  • Oring, Elliott (Spring 2000). «Review of Jokes and Their Relation to Society by Christie Davies». The Journal of American Folklore. 113 (448): 220–221. doi:10.2307/541299. JSTOR 541299.
  • Oring, Elliott (2008). «Humor in Anthropology and Folklore». In Raskin, Victor (ed.). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 183–210.
  • Preston, Cathy Lynn (1997). «Joke». In Green, Thomas (ed.). Folklore An Encyclopedia of Beliefs, Customs, Tales, Music, and Art. Santa Barbara, CA: ABC-CLIO.
  • Radcliffe-Brown, A. R. (1940). «On Joking Relationships». Journal of the International African Institute. 13 (332): 195–210. doi:10.2307/1156093. JSTOR 1156093. S2CID 11011777.
  • Rahkonen, Carl (2000). «No Laughing Matter: The Viola Joke Cycle as Musicians’ Folklore». Western Folklore. 59 (1): 49–63. doi:10.2307/1500468. JSTOR 1500468.
  • Raskin, Victor (1985). Semantic Mechanisms of Humor. Dordrecht, Boston, Lancaster: D. Reidel.
  • Raskin, Victor (1992). «Humor as a Non-Bona-Fide Mode of Communication». In E. L. Pedersen (ed.). Proceedings of the 1992 annual meeting of the Deseret Language and Linguistic Society. Provo, UT: Brigham Young University. pp. 87–92. S2CID 152033221.
  • Raskin, Victor, ed. (2008). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter.
  • Raskin, Victor; Attardo, Salvatore (1991). «Script theory revis(it)ed: joke similarity and joke representation model». Humor: International Journal of Humor Research. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. 4 (3–4): 293–348.
  • Ruch, Willibald (2008). «Psychology of humor». In Raskin, Victor (ed.). Primer of Humor Research: Humor Research 8. Berlin, New York: Mouton de Gruyter. pp. 17–100.
  • Sacks, Harvey (1974). «An Analysis of the Course of a Joke’s telling in Conversation». In Bauman, Richard; Sherzer, Joel (eds.). Explorations in the Ethnography of Speaking. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. pp. 337–353.
  • Shultz, Thomas R. (1976). «A cognitive-developmental analysis of humour». Humour and Laughter: Theory, Research and Applications. London: John Wiley: 11–36.
  • Simons, Elizabeth Radin (1986). «The NASA Joke Cycle: The Astronauts and the Teacher». Western Folklore. 45 (4): 261–277. doi:10.2307/1499821. JSTOR 1499821.
  • Sims, Martha; Stephens, Martine (2005). Living Folklore: Introduction to the Study of People and their Traditions. Logan, UT: Utah State University Press.
  • Smyth, Willie (October 1986). «Challenger Jokes and the Humor of Disaster». Western Folklore. 45 (4): 243–260. doi:10.2307/1499820. JSTOR 1499820.
  • Sykes, A.J.M. (1966). «Joking Relationships in an Industrial Setting». American Anthropologist. New Series. 68 (1): 188–193. doi:10.1525/aa.1966.68.1.02a00250. JSTOR 668081.
  • Toelken, Barre (1996). The Dynamics of Folklore. Logan, UT: Utah State University Press.
  • Walle, Alf H. (1976). «Getting Picked up without Being Put down: Jokes and the Bar Rush». Journal of the Folklore Institute. 13 (332): 201–217. doi:10.2307/3813856. JSTOR 3813856.
  • Ward, A.W.; Waller, A.R., eds. (2000). «V. The Progress of Social Literature in Tudor Times. § 9. Jest-books». The Cambridge History of English and American Literature in 18 Volumes (1907–21). Volume III. Renascence and Reformation. New York: BARTLEBY.COM.
  • Weeks, Linton (March 3, 2015). «The Secret History Of Knock-Knock Jokes». npr.org.
  • Wild, Barbara; Rodden, Frank A.; Grodd, Wolfgang; Ruch, Willibald (2003). «Neural correlates of laughter and humour». Brain. 126 (10): 2121–2138. doi:10.1093/brain/awg226. PMID 12902310.

External links

  • The dictionary definition of joke at Wiktionary

Noun



She meant it as a joke, but many people took her seriously.



They played a harmless joke on him.



They are always making jokes about his car.



I heard a funny joke yesterday.



the punch line of a joke



I didn’t get the joke.



That exam was a joke.



Their product became a joke in the industry.



He’s in danger of becoming a national joke.

Verb



My friends would joke about the uniform I had to wear at work.



She joked about the possibility of losing her job.



I thought he was joking when he said he might quit, but it turned out that he really meant it.



Don’t take it seriously: I was only joking.



She spent a few minutes joking with reporters after giving her speech.



She joked that she could always get work as a truck driver if she lost her job.

See More

Recent Examples on the Web



Gold prices are back above $2,000 and the price of dogecoin—the joke cryptocurrency—is still elevated.


WSJ, 4 Apr. 2023





It was first published as an April Fools’ Day joke in 2006 by a French fishing magazine.


Eleanor Mccrary, USA TODAY, 4 Apr. 2023





The first cryptocurrency based on an internet meme, Dogecoin launched in December 2013 as a joke.


Stephanie Mlot, PCMAG, 4 Apr. 2023





The two hosts’ annual joke swap has painted Jost as a white-supremacist bigot, and Sarah Sherman routinely refers to Jost as an antisemitic child molester.


Vulture, 3 Apr. 2023





In July 2018, right-wing trolls unearthed some of his old, deliberately edgy joke tweets, leading Disney to fire him from the already-in-the-works third chapter.


Brian Hiatt, Rolling Stone, 3 Apr. 2023





Dogecoin was the first memecoin, or a cryptocurrency that lacks any underlying value aside from its status as a joke.


Leo Schwartz, Fortune Crypto, 3 Apr. 2023





Dogecoin was created December 6, 2013, by a pair of software engineers — as a joke.


Allison Morrow, CNN, 3 Apr. 2023





Enjoy the day by pranking – or being pranked – and take a look back at Cincinnati’s April Fools’ Day jokes over the years.


Victoria Moorwood, The Enquirer, 1 Apr. 2023




In 2019, Adam joked about the common mix-up of the two Naomi Scotts when the women finally met in person at a birthday party for actress Elizabeth Banks.


Kaitlin Stevens, Peoplemag, 6 Apr. 2023





One professor has joked that there should be a philosophy-and-weight-lifting club called Will to Power.


Emma Green, The New Yorker, 3 Apr. 2023





Mathematicians have joked that this unusual challenge is akin to finding hay in a haystack.


Ben Brubaker, Quanta Magazine, 3 Apr. 2023





Swift joked prior to the track.


Daniel Kreps, Rolling Stone, 1 Apr. 2023





While presenting at the Oscars earlier this month, Hugh Grant joked that his face looks like a scrotum.


Emlyn Travis, EW.com, 30 Mar. 2023





Erin Murphy joked that fandom, evidently, has a price tag.


Jr Radcliffe, Journal Sentinel, 29 Mar. 2023





Mining researchers in South Africa sometimes joke that the story of gold mining runs from AA to ZZ — from multinationals like Anglo American to zama-zamas.


Seyward Darby, Longreads, 29 Mar. 2023





Here for all the bang jokes 21.


Town & Country, 4 Apr. 2023



See More

These examples are programmatically compiled from various online sources to illustrate current usage of the word ‘joke.’ Any opinions expressed in the examples do not represent those of Merriam-Webster or its editors. Send us feedback about these examples.

joke
1. [dʒəʋk]

1. анекдот, шутка, острота; смешной случай

in joke — в шутку, шутя

current [dirty] joke — злободневные [непристойные] анекдоты

practical joke — шутка, розыгрыш

the best of the joke — соль /смысл/ остроты /шутки/

to see the joke — понять соль /смысл/ остроты /анекдота/

to make a joke about smb., smth. — подшутить над кем-л., чем-л.

to turn smth. into a joke, to make a joke of smth. — обернуть что-л. в шутку, свести что-л. к шутке

to cut /to crack/ a joke about smb. — отпустить шутку на чей-л. счёт

to put the joke on smb. — подшутить над кем-л., пройтись на чей-л. счёт

he can’t take a joke — он не понимает шуток

the joke is on him — это он в дураках остался

2. объект шуток, посмешище

standing joke — неистощимый объект для шуток

he is the joke of the town — над ним весь город смеётся

3. 1) шуточное дело, пустяк

no joke — не шутка, дело серьёзное

the loss was no joke — убытки были нешуточные

this goes beyond a joke — это уже не смешно; это становится серьёзным

2) работа, не требующая усилий; ≅ раз плюнуть

that exam was a joke — сдать такой экзамен было легче лёгкого

2. [dʒəʋk]

1. шутить; острить

he is always joking — он вечно шутит

joking apart — шутки в сторону

2. подшучивать (), дразнить ()

Новый большой англо-русский словарь.
2001.

Полезное

Смотреть что такое «joke» в других словарях:

  • Joke — est un groupe de musique français créé en 1995 en banlieue parisienne. Ses membres fusionnent différents styles musicaux allant du punk rock, au hip hop en passant par le raggamuffin. Activites scéniques aux textes socialement impliqués, les… …   Wikipédia en Français

  • joke — joke, jest, jape, quip, witticism, wisecrack, crack, gag are comparable when they mean a remark, story, or action intended to evoke laughter. Joke, when applied to a story or remark, suggests something designed to promote good humor and… …   New Dictionary of Synonyms

  • Joke — Joke, n. [L. jocus. Cf {Jeopardy}, {Jocular}, {Juggler}.] [1913 Webster] 1. Something said for the sake of exciting a laugh; something witty or sportive (commonly indicating more of hilarity or humor than jest); a jest; a witticism; as, to crack… …   The Collaborative International Dictionary of English

  • joke — [jōk] n. [L jocus, a joke, game < IE base * jek , to speak > OHG jehan] 1. anything said or done to arouse laughter; specif., a) a funny anecdote with a punch line b) an amusing trick played on someone 2. the humorous element in a situation …   English World dictionary

  • Joke — 〈[ dʒoʊk] m. 6; umg.〉 Witz, witzige Geschichte ● er machte einen Joke nach dem anderen [engl.] * * * Joke [ʤoʊk], der; s, s [engl. joke < lat. iocus = Scherz] (ugs.): Witz: er macht gern mal einen J …   Universal-Lexikon

  • joke — joke·less; joke·let; joke·ster; joke; …   English syllables

  • Joke — Joke, v. t. [imp. & p. p. {Joked}; p. pr. & vb. n. {Joking}.] To make merry with; to make jokes upon; to rally; to banter; as, to joke a comrade. [1913 Webster] …   The Collaborative International Dictionary of English

  • Joke — Joke, v. i. [L. jocari.] To do something for sport, or as a joke; to be merry in words or actions; to jest. [1913 Webster] He laughed, shouted, joked, and swore. Macaulay. Syn: To jest; sport; rally; banter. See {Jest}. [1913 Webster] …   The Collaborative International Dictionary of English

  • joke — [n1] fun, quip antic, bon mot, buffoonery, burlesque, caper, caprice, chestnut*, clowning, drollery, epigram, escapade, farce, frolic, gag, gambol, game, ha ha*, hoodwinking*, horseplay*, humor, jape, jest, lark, laugh, mischief, monkeyshine*,… …   New thesaurus

  • Joke — 〈 [dʒoʊk] m.; Gen.: s, Pl.: s; umg.〉 Witz, witzige Geschichte; er machte einen Joke nach dem anderen [Etym.: engl.] …   Lexikalische Deutsches Wörterbuch

  • Joke — [dʒouk] der; s, s <aus gleichbed. engl. joke, dies aus lat. iocus> (ugs.) Witz, spaßige Geschichte …   Das große Fremdwörterbuch

English[edit]

Etymology[edit]

From Latin iocus (joke, jest, pastime), from Proto-Italic *jokos (word, (playful?) saying), from Proto-Indo-European *yokos (word, utterance), from ultimate root Proto-Indo-European *yek- (to speak, utter) (of which distant cognates include Proto-Celtic *yextis (language) (Breton yezh (language) and Welsh iaith (language)) and German Beichte (confession)). Cognate with French jeu, Italian gioco, Portuguese jogo, Spanish juego, Romanian juca, English Yule, Danish Jule, Norwegian Bokmål Jul, Swedish Jul, and Norwegian Nynorsk jol.

Pronunciation[edit]

  • (UK) IPA(key): /d͡ʒəʊk/
  • (US) IPA(key): /d͡ʒoʊk/
  • Rhymes: -əʊk

Noun[edit]

joke (plural jokes)

  1. An amusing story.
    • 1708, John Gay, Wine
      Or witty joke our airy senses moves / To pleasant laughter.
  2. Something said or done for amusement, not in seriousness.
    It was a joke!
  3. (figuratively) The root cause or main issue, especially an unexpected one
  4. (figuratively) A laughably worthless thing or person; a sham.

    Your effort at cleaning your room is a joke.

    The president was a joke.

    • 1943 September and October, T. Lovatt Williams, “Some Reminiscences of the Footplate—II”, in Railway Magazine, page 272:

      The other wheel on the tender of the L.N.W.R. engines operated the tender brake, and this was always rather a joke. Sometimes it operated with good results and on other occasions it did not.

  5. (figuratively) Something that is far easier or far less challenging than expected.
    The final exam was a joke.

Usage notes[edit]

  • Adjectives often applied to «joke»: old, bad, inside, poor, silly, funny, lame, hilarious, stupid, offensive.

Synonyms[edit]

  • See also Thesaurus:joke

Coordinate terms[edit]

  • comedy
  • limerick
  • parody
  • pun

Derived terms[edit]

terms derived from joke (noun)

[edit]

  • jocular

Descendants[edit]

  • Danish: joke
  • French: joke
  • Persian: جوک
  • Japanese: ジョーク, Japanese: 冗句
  • Welsh: jôc

Translations[edit]

amusing story

  • Arabic: طُرْفَة‎ f (ṭurfa), نُكْتَة‎ f (nukta), نَادِرَة‎ f (nādira), لَطِيفَة‎ f (laṭīfa)
    Egyptian Arabic: نكتة‎ f (nukta)
    Hejazi Arabic: نكتة‎ f (nukta)
    Moroccan Arabic: نكتة(nukta)
  • Armenian: անեկդոտ (hy) (anekdot), կատակ (hy) (katak)
  • Azerbaijani: lətifə (az)
  • Belarusian: жарт m (žart), анекдо́т m (anjekdót), до́сціп m (dóscip)
  • Bulgarian: шега́ (bg) f (šegá)
  • Catalan: acudit (ca) m
  • Chechen: забар (zabar), бегаш (begaš)
  • Chinese:
    Cantonese: 笑話笑话 (siu3 waa6-2)
    Hakka: 笑話笑话 (seu-fa)
    Mandarin: 笑話笑话 (zh) (xiàohua)
    Min Nan: 笑詼笑诙 (zh-min-nan) (chhiò-khoe, chhiò-khe), 笑話笑话 (zh-min-nan) (chhiò-ōe)
  • Coptic: ⲥⲁϫⲓ ⲛ̀ⲥⲱⲃⲓ m (sači ǹsōbi)
  • Czech: vtip (cs) m
  • Danish: vittighed (da) c, vits c, joke c
  • Dutch: mop (nl) m, grap (nl) f, grol (nl) f
  • Esperanto: ŝerco, amuzaĵo
  • Estonian: nali, anekdoot
  • Faroese: skemtisøga f, krønika f
  • Finnish: vitsi (fi), pila (fi), (dated) kasku (fi), (slang) läppä (fi)
  • French: plaisanterie (fr) f, blague (fr) f, joke (fr) f
  • Georgian: ხუმრობა (xumroba), ანეკდოტი (aneḳdoṭi)
  • German: Witz (de) m
  • Greek: ανέκδοτο (el) n (anékdoto)
    Ancient: σκῶμμα n (skômma), χλεύη f (khleúē)
  • Guaraní: pukarã
  • Hebrew: בְּדִיחָה (he) f (b’dikhá)
  • Hindi: चुटकुला (hi) m (cuṭkulā), लतीफ़ा m (latīfā)
  • Hungarian: vicc (hu)
  • Icelandic: brandari (is) m
  • Indonesian: candaan
  • Italian: barzelletta (it) f, battuta (it) f, scherzo (it) m, celia (it) f
  • Japanese: 冗談 (ja) (じょうだん, jōdan)
  • Khmer: ហាសវត្ថុ (haasaʼvŏətthoʼ)
  • Korean: 농담(弄談) (ko) (nongdam), 롱담(弄談) (ko) (rongdam) (North Korea)
  • Kurdish:
    Central Kurdish: نوکتە(nukte)
    Northern Kurdish: henek (ku)
  • Kyrgyz: тамаша (ky) (tamaşa)
  • Latin: iocus m, nūgae f pl
  • Latvian: joks (lv) m
  • Macedonian: шега f (šega)
  • Malay: jenaka, seloroh
  • Malayalam: തമാശ (ml) (tamāśa)
  • Mongolian:
    Cyrillic: онигоо (mn) (onigoo)
  • Norwegian:
    Bokmål: vits (no) m
    Nynorsk: vits m
  • Oromo: qoosaa
  • Persian: لطیفه (fa) (latife), جوک (fa) (jok)
  • Plautdietsch: Spos n
  • Polish: dowcip (pl) m, kawał (pl), żart (pl)
  • Portuguese: piada (pt) f
  • Romani:
    Balkan Romani: pherǎs
  • Romanian: banc (ro) n, glumă (ro) f, șagă (ro) f
  • Russian: анекдо́т (ru) m (anekdót), шу́тка (ru) f (šútka)
  • Rusyn: фіґель m (figelʹ), анекдо́т m (anekdót), смішо́к m (smišók)
  • Scottish Gaelic: fealla-dhà m
  • Serbo-Croatian:
    Cyrillic: ша́ла f
    Roman: šála (sh) f
  • Slovak: vtip m
  • Slovene: šala (sl) f
  • Spanish: broma (es) f, chiste (es) m, chascarrillo (es)
  • Swahili: kasheshe (sw)
  • Swedish: skämt (sv) n, rolig historia (sv) c, vits (sv) c
  • Tagalog: biro
  • Tajik: аския (askiya), латифа (tg) (latifa)
  • Thai: มุก (th) (múk)
  • Tibetan: ཀྱལ (kyal)
  • Turkish: fıkra (tr)
  • Turkmen: degişme
  • Ukrainian: жарт m (žart), до́теп (uk) m (dótep), анекдо́т (uk) m (anekdót)
  • Urdu: لطیفہ‎ m (latīfā)
  • Uyghur: لەتىپە(letipe), شاڭخو(shangxo)
  • Uzbek: latifa (uz)
  • Vietnamese: chuyện tiếu lâm
  • Waray-Waray: ti-aw
  • Welsh: jôc f
  • Yiddish: וויץ‎ m (vits)

something said or done for amusement

  • Albanian: shaka (sq) f
  • Arabic: نُكْتَة‎ f (nukta), هَزْل‎ m (hazl)
  • Armenian: կատակ (hy) (katak)
  • Azerbaijani: zarafat (az)
  • Belarusian: жарт m (žart)
  • Bengali: প্রহসন (prohośon), রসিকতা (rośikota)
  • Bulgarian: шега́ (bg) f (šegá)
  • Burmese: အသော (my) (a.sau:)
  • Catalan: broma (ca) f
  • Chechen: бегаш (begaš), забар (zabar)
  • Chinese:
    Mandarin: 玩笑 (zh) (wánxiào)
  • Coptic: ⲥⲁϫⲓ ⲛ̀ⲥⲱⲃⲓ m (sači ǹsōbi)
  • Czech: vtip (cs) m, vtípek (cs) m, žert (cs) m, žertík m
  • Danish: spøg (da) c
  • Dutch: mop (nl) m, grap (nl) f, geintje (nl) n, lol (nl) f
  • Esperanto: amuzaĵo
  • Estonian: nali
  • Faroese: skemt
  • Finnish: pila (fi), kuje (fi), kepponen (fi)
  • French: plaisanterie (fr) f, blague (fr) f
  • Georgian: ხუმრობა (xumroba)
  • German: Scherz (de) m, Streich (de) m, Witz (de) m, Spaß (de) m
  • Greek: αστείο (el) n (asteío)
    Ancient: σκῶμμα n (skômma), χλεύη f (khleúē)
  • Hebrew: בְּדִיחָה (he) f (b’dikhá)
  • Hindi: मज़ाक़ m (mazāq)
  • Hungarian: tréfa (hu)
  • Icelandic: grín (is) n, spaug n
  • Indonesian: lelucon (id), lawak (id)
  • Interlingua: burla
  • Irish: cleas m
  • Italian: battuta (it) f, scherzo (it) m, celia (it) f
  • Japanese: 冗談 (ja) (じょうだん, jōdan), ジョーク (ja) (jōku)
  • Kabuverdianu: txakóta, txokóta
  • Kazakh: әзіл (kk) (äzıl), қалжың (qaljyñ), ажуа (ajua), ойын (oiyn)
  • Khmer: ពាក្យនិយាយលេង (piək niyiəy leing), ល្បែងលេងសើច (lbaeng leing saəc)
  • Korean: 농담(弄談) (ko) (nongdam), 롱담(弄談) (ko) (rongdam) (North Korea)
  • Kurdish:
    Central Kurdish: گاڵتە (ckb) (gallte)
    Northern Kurdish: henek (ku), galte (ku)
  • Kyrgyz: тамаша (ky) (tamaşa), азил (ky) (azil)
  • Lao: ເລື່ອງຕະຫຼົກ (lư̄ang ta lok)
  • Latin: iocus m, facetia f
  • Latvian: joks (lv) m
  • Lithuanian: juokas (lt) m, pokštas (lt) m
  • Macedonian: шега f (šega)
  • Malay: lawak (ms)
  • Malayalam: തമാശ (ml) (tamāśa)
  • Mongolian:
    Cyrillic: хошигнол (mn) (xošignol)
  • Norwegian:
    Bokmål: spøk (no) m
    Nynorsk: spøk m
  • Pashto: ټوکه (ps) f (ṭoka), انک (ps) m (anák)
  • Persian: شوخی (fa) (šuxi)
  • Polish: dowcip (pl) m, żart (pl) m, kawał (pl) m
  • Portuguese: brincadeira (pt) f
  • Romani:
    Balkan Romani: pherǎs
  • Romanian: glumă (ro)
  • Russian: шу́тка (ru) f (šútka), остро́та (ru) f (ostróta)
  • Sanskrit: विनोदकणिका f (vinodakaṇikā), उपहासगिर् (sa) f (upahāsagir), अभिहास (sa) m (abhihāsa)
  • Scottish Gaelic: cleas m, fealla-dhà m
  • Serbo-Croatian:
    Cyrillic: ша́ла f
    Roman: šála (sh) f
  • Slovak: vtip m, žart m
  • Slovene: šala (sl) f
  • Spanish: broma (es) f, cuchufleta (es)
  • Swahili: kasheshe (sw), mzaha (sw)
  • Swedish: skämt (sv) n
  • Tagalog: biro
  • Tajik: шӯхӣ (šüxī), ҳазл (hazl)
  • Thai: มุก (th) (múk), เรื่องตลก (rʉ̂ʉang-dtà-lòk)
  • Tibetan: བསྟན་ཤིག་སློང (bstan shig slong)
  • Turkish: şaka (tr)
  • Turkmen: oýun
  • Ukrainian: жарт m (žart)
  • Urdu: مذاق‎ m (mazāq)
  • Uyghur: چاقچاق(chaqchaq), مايداڭ(maydang), ھەزىل(hezil)
  • Uzbek: xazil (uz)
  • Vietnamese: đùa (vi), lời nói đùa, câu nói đùa
  • Volapük: cog (vo), (diminutive) cogil
  • Yiddish: וויץ‎ m (vits), שפּאַס‎ m (shpas)

worthless thing or person

  • Czech: šašek (cs) m, klaun m
  • Dutch: grap (nl) f, aanfluiting (nl) f
  • French: raté (fr) m, ratée (fr) f
  • Georgian: დასაცინი (dasacini), სასაცილო (sasacilo), მასხარა (masxara)
  • German: Witz (de) m
  • Hungarian: vicc (hu)
  • Icelandic: brandari (is) m
  • Italian: burla (it) f
  • Kurdish:
    Central Kurdish: گاڵتە (ckb) (gallte)
  • Norwegian:
    Bokmål: vits (no) m
  • Persian: مسخره (fa) (masxare), (person) شوت (fa) (šut)
  • Polish: żart (pl) m
  • Portuguese: piada (pt) f
  • Romani:
    Balkan Romani: devruś
  • Romanian: glumă (ro)
  • Russian: (person) кло́ун (ru) m (klóun), (person) шут (ru) m (šut), посме́шище (ru) n (posméšišče)
  • Spanish: tomadura de pelo (es) f
  • Swedish: skämt (sv) n
  • Tagalog: gago (derogatory), gaguhan, panggagago

Verb[edit]

joke (third-person singular simple present jokes, present participle joking, simple past and past participle joked)

  1. (intransitive) To do or say something for amusement rather than seriously.

    I didn’t mean what I said — I was only joking.

  2. (intransitive, followed by with) To dupe in a friendly manner for amusement; to mess with, play with.

    Relax, man, I’m just joking with you.

  3. (transitive, dated) To make merry with; to make jokes upon; to rally.

    to joke a comrade

    • 1848 November – 1850 December, William Makepeace Thackeray, chapter 21, in The History of Pendennis. [], volume (please specify |volume=I or II), London: Bradbury and Evans, [], published 1849–1850, →OCLC:

      He made more than one visit to Oxbridge, where the young fellows were amused by entertaining the old gentleman, and gave parties and breakfasts and fêtes, partly to joke him and partly to do him honour.

[edit]

  • joker

Translations[edit]

do for amusement

  • Arabic: مَزَحَ(mazaḥa)
    Hijazi Arabic: مزح(mazaḥ), نَكَّت(nakkat)
  • Armenian: կատակել (hy) (katakel), զվարճախոսել (hy) (zvarčaxosel)
  • Azerbaijani: zarafat etmək
  • Belarusian: жартава́ць impf (žartavácʹ)
  • Bulgarian: шегувам се (šeguvam se)
  • Burmese: ရွှတ် (my) (hrwat)
  • Cherokee: ᎦᏪᏟᎭ (gawetliha)
  • Chinese:
    Mandarin: 開玩笑开玩笑 (zh) (kāi wánxiào)
  • Czech: žertovat (cs)
  • Danish: spøge
  • Dutch: grappen (nl), grappenmaken
  • Esperanto: ŝerci
  • Estonian: naljatama
  • Faroese: skemta
  • Finnish: vitsailla (fi), pilailla (fi)
  • French: plaisanter (fr), blaguer (fr)
  • Georgian: ხუმრობა (xumroba), ოხუნჯობა (oxunǯoba), ლაზღანდარობა (lazɣandaroba)
  • German: scherzen (de), Witze machen, Spaß machen, witzeln (de)
  • Greek: ατειεύομαι (ateiévomai)
    Ancient: σκώπτω (skṓptō)
  • Hebrew: התלוצץ (he) (hitlotséts), התבדח(hitbadéakh)
  • Hungarian: tréfál (hu), viccel (hu)
  • Icelandic: grínast, spauga (is)
  • Italian: scherzare (it)
  • Japanese: 冗談を言う (じょうだんをいう, jōdan o iu), 冷やかす (ひやかす, hiyakasu), 巫山戯る (ja) (ふざける, fuzakeru)
  • Korean: 농담하다 (ko) (nongdamhada), 롱담하다 (rongdamhada) (North Korea)
  • Kurdish:
    Central Kurdish: گالتە کردن(galte kirdin)
  • Kyrgyz: тамашалоо (ky) (tamaşaloo), калжың кылуу (ky) (kaljıŋ kıluu)
  • Latin: iocor, ioculor
  • Latvian: jokot
  • Neapolitan: pazzià
  • Norwegian: tulle (no), tulle bort, kødde
  • Old English: gamenian
  • Polish: żartować (pl)
  • Portuguese: gracejar (pt), contar piada, brincar (pt) m
  • Romani:
    Balkan Romani: kerel pherǎs, asavel, marel pherǎs, marel muj
  • Romanian: glumi (ro)
  • Russian: шути́ть (ru) impf (šutítʹ), пошути́ть (ru) pf (pošutítʹ)
  • Serbo-Croatian:
    Cyrillic: шалити се impf
    Roman: šaliti se (sh) impf
  • Sicilian: babbiàri
  • Slovak: žartovať impf
  • Spanish: bromear (es)
  • Swedish: skämta (sv), skoja (sv)
  • Tagalog: biro
  • Turkish: şaka yapmak (tr)
  • Ukrainian: жартува́ти (uk) impf (žartuváty)
  • Vietnamese: nói đùa (vi)
  • Volapük: cogön (vo)

See also[edit]

  • jeer
  • mock

Anagrams[edit]

  • ojek

Danish[edit]

Etymology 1[edit]

Borrowed from English joke.

Pronunciation[edit]

  • IPA(key): [ˈd̥jɔwɡ̊]

Noun[edit]

joke c (singular definite joken, plural indefinite jokes)

  1. joke
Declension[edit]
Synonyms[edit]
  • spøg
  • vits
  • vittighed

Etymology 2[edit]

Borrowed from English joke.

Pronunciation[edit]

  • IPA(key): [ˈd̥jɔwɡ̊ə], (imperative) IPA(key): [ˈd̥jɔwˀɡ̊]

Verb[edit]

joke (past tense jokede, past participle joket)

  1. joke
Conjugation[edit]
Synonyms[edit]
  • spøge

French[edit]

Etymology[edit]

From English joke.

Pronunciation[edit]

  • IPA(key): /dʒok/
  • (Quebec) IPA(key): /dʒoʊk/

Noun[edit]

joke f (plural jokes)

  1. (Louisiana, Quebec) joke
    • 2009, Robert Maltais, Le Curé du Mile End, page 195:

      Non, non, c’est juste une joke. Garde-lé, ton vingt piastres.

      No, no, that was a joke. Keep it, your twenty bucks.

Derived terms[edit]

  • faire une joke


Also found in: Thesaurus, Medical, Acronyms, Idioms, Encyclopedia, Wikipedia.

joke

 (jōk)

n.

1. Something said or done to evoke laughter or amusement, especially an amusing story with a punch line.

2. A mischievous trick; a prank: played a joke on his roommate.

3. Something that is of ludicrously poor quality: Their delivery service is a joke.

4. Informal

a. Something not to be taken seriously; a triviality: The accident was no joke.

b. An object of amusement or laughter; a laughingstock: His loud tie was the joke of the office.

v. joked, jok·ing, jokes

v.intr.

1. To tell or play jokes; jest.

2. To speak in fun; be facetious: You have to be joking.

v.tr.

To say or write as a joke.



jok′ing·ly adv.

Synonyms: joke, witticism, quip, crack, wisecrack, gag
These nouns refer to something that is said or done in order to evoke laughter or amusement. Joke especially denotes an amusing story with a punch line at the end: She told jokes at the party. A witticism is a witty, usually cleverly phrased remark: «He said one should treat heavy things lightly and light things with gravity, which became a handy witticism» (Natalie Dykstra).
A quip is a clever, pointed, often sarcastic remark: He responded to the tough questions with quips. Crack and wisecrack refer less formally to flippant or sarcastic retorts: She made a crack about my driving ability; he was punished for making wisecracks in class. Gag is principally applicable to a broadly comic remark or to comic byplay in a theatrical routine: It was one of the most memorable gags in the history of vaudeville.

American Heritage® Dictionary of the English Language, Fifth Edition. Copyright © 2016 by Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company. Published by Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company. All rights reserved.

joke

(dʒəʊk)

n

1. a humorous anecdote

2. something that is said or done for fun; prank

3. a ridiculous or humorous circumstance

4. a person or thing inspiring ridicule or amusement; butt

5. a matter to be joked about or ignored

6. joking apart seriously: said to recall a discussion to seriousness after there has been joking

7. no joke something very serious

vb

8. (intr) to tell jokes

9. (intr) to speak or act facetiously or in fun

10. to make fun of (someone); tease; kid

[C17: from Latin jocus a jest]

ˈjokingly adv

Collins English Dictionary – Complete and Unabridged, 12th Edition 2014 © HarperCollins Publishers 1991, 1994, 1998, 2000, 2003, 2006, 2007, 2009, 2011, 2014

joke

(dʒoʊk)

n., v. joked, jok•ing. n.

1. a short humorous anecdote with a punch line.

2. anything said or done to provoke laughter or cause amusement.

3. something amusing or ridiculous: I don’t see the joke in that.

4. an object of laughter or ridicule, esp. because of being inadequate or sham.

5. a trifling matter: The loss was no joke.

v.i.

7. to speak or act in a playful way.

8. to say something in fun or teasing: I was only joking.

v.t.

9. to subject to jokes; make fun of.

[1660–70; < Latin jocus jest]

jok′ing•ly, adv.

Random House Kernerman Webster’s College Dictionary, © 2010 K Dictionaries Ltd. Copyright 2005, 1997, 1991 by Random House, Inc. All rights reserved.

joke

— Latin jocus, «jest, joke,» gave us joke.

See also related terms for jest.

Farlex Trivia Dictionary. © 2012 Farlex, Inc. All rights reserved.

joke

When someone makes or cracks a joke, they say something in order to make people laugh.

She would make jokes about her appearance.

We stayed up for hours, laughing and cracking jokes.

A joke is also a clever or funny story that you repeat in order to make people laugh. When joke has this meaning, you say that someone tells a joke.

Tell Uncle Henry the joke you told us.

A joke is also something that is done to make someone appear foolish. When joke has this meaning, you say that someone plays a joke on someone else.

They’re playing a joke on you.

Be Careful!
Don’t say that someone ‘says a joke‘ or ‘does a joke‘.

Collins COBUILD English Usage © HarperCollins Publishers 1992, 2004, 2011, 2012

joke

Past participle: joked
Gerund: joking

Imperative
joke
joke
Present
I joke
you joke
he/she/it jokes
we joke
you joke
they joke
Preterite
I joked
you joked
he/she/it joked
we joked
you joked
they joked
Present Continuous
I am joking
you are joking
he/she/it is joking
we are joking
you are joking
they are joking
Present Perfect
I have joked
you have joked
he/she/it has joked
we have joked
you have joked
they have joked
Past Continuous
I was joking
you were joking
he/she/it was joking
we were joking
you were joking
they were joking
Past Perfect
I had joked
you had joked
he/she/it had joked
we had joked
you had joked
they had joked
Future
I will joke
you will joke
he/she/it will joke
we will joke
you will joke
they will joke
Future Perfect
I will have joked
you will have joked
he/she/it will have joked
we will have joked
you will have joked
they will have joked
Future Continuous
I will be joking
you will be joking
he/she/it will be joking
we will be joking
you will be joking
they will be joking
Present Perfect Continuous
I have been joking
you have been joking
he/she/it has been joking
we have been joking
you have been joking
they have been joking
Future Perfect Continuous
I will have been joking
you will have been joking
he/she/it will have been joking
we will have been joking
you will have been joking
they will have been joking
Past Perfect Continuous
I had been joking
you had been joking
he/she/it had been joking
we had been joking
you had been joking
they had been joking
Conditional
I would joke
you would joke
he/she/it would joke
we would joke
you would joke
they would joke
Past Conditional
I would have joked
you would have joked
he/she/it would have joked
we would have joked
you would have joked
they would have joked

Collins English Verb Tables © HarperCollins Publishers 2011

ThesaurusAntonymsRelated WordsSynonymsLegend:

Noun 1. joke - a humorous anecdote or remark intended to provoke laughterjoke — a humorous anecdote or remark intended to provoke laughter; «he told a very funny joke»; «he knows a million gags»; «thanks for the laugh»; «he laughed unpleasantly at his own jest»; «even a schoolboy’s jape is supposed to have some ascertainable point»

gag, jape, jest, laugh

humor, wit, witticism, wittiness, humour — a message whose ingenuity or verbal skill or incongruity has the power to evoke laughter

gag line, punch line, tag line, laugh line — the point of a joke or humorous story

howler, sidesplitter, thigh-slapper, wow, belly laugh, riot, scream — a joke that seems extremely funny

blue joke, blue story, dirty joke, dirty story — an indelicate joke

ethnic joke — a joke at the expense of some ethnic group

funny, funny remark, funny story, good story — an account of an amusing incident (usually with a punch line); «she told a funny story»; «she made a funny»

in-joke — a joke that is appreciated only by members of some particular group of people

one-liner — a one-line joke

shaggy dog story — a long rambling joke whose humor derives from its pointlessness

sick joke — a joke in bad taste

sight gag, visual joke — a joke whose effect is achieved by visual means rather than by speech (as in a movie)

2. joke — activity characterized by good humor

jest, jocularity

diversion, recreation — an activity that diverts or amuses or stimulates; «scuba diving is provided as a diversion for tourists»; «for recreation he wrote poetry and solved crossword puzzles»; «drug abuse is often regarded as a form of recreation»

drollery, waggery — a quaint and amusing jest

leg-pull, leg-pulling — as a joke: trying to make somebody believe something that is not true

pleasantry — an agreeable or amusing remark; «they exchange pleasantries»

3. joke - a ludicrous or grotesque act done for fun and amusementjoke — a ludicrous or grotesque act done for fun and amusement

antic, prank, put-on, trick, caper

diversion, recreation — an activity that diverts or amuses or stimulates; «scuba diving is provided as a diversion for tourists»; «for recreation he wrote poetry and solved crossword puzzles»; «drug abuse is often regarded as a form of recreation»

dirty trick — an unkind or aggressive trick

practical joke — a prank or trick played on a person (especially one intended to make the victim appear foolish)

4. joke — a triviality not to be taken seriously; «I regarded his campaign for mayor as a joke»

puniness, slightness, triviality, pettiness — the quality of being unimportant and petty or frivolous

Verb 1. joke — tell a joke; speak humorously; «He often jokes even when he appears serious»

jest

communicate, intercommunicate — transmit thoughts or feelings; «He communicated his anxieties to the psychiatrist»

quip, gag — make jokes or quips; «The students were gagging during dinner»

fool around, horse around, fool — indulge in horseplay; «Enough horsing around—let’s get back to work!»; «The bored children were fooling about»

pun — make a play on words; «Japanese like to pun—their language is well suited to punning»

2. joke — act in a funny or teasing way

jest

behave, act, do — behave in a certain manner; show a certain behavior; conduct or comport oneself; «You should act like an adult»; «Don’t behave like a fool»; «What makes her do this way?»; «The dog acts ferocious, but he is really afraid of people»

antic, clown, clown around — act as or like a clown

Based on WordNet 3.0, Farlex clipart collection. © 2003-2012 Princeton University, Farlex Inc.

joke

noun

1. jest, gag (informal), wisecrack (informal), witticism, crack (informal), sally, quip, josh (slang, chiefly U.S. & Canad.), pun, quirk, one-liner (informal), jape No one told worse jokes than Claus.

2. jest, laugh, fun, josh (slang, chiefly U.S. & Canad.), lark, sport, frolic, whimsy, jape It was probably just a joke to them, but it wasn’t funny to me.

verb

1. jest, kid (informal), fool, mock, wind up (Brit. slang), tease, ridicule, taunt, quip, josh (slang, chiefly U.S. & Canad.), banter, deride, frolic, chaff, gambol, play the fool, play a trick Don’t get defensive, Charlie. I was only joking.

Proverbs
«Many a true word is spoken in jest»

Collins Thesaurus of the English Language – Complete and Unabridged 2nd Edition. 2002 © HarperCollins Publishers 1995, 2002

joke

noun

1. Words or actions intended to excite laughter or amusement:

3. Informal. Something or someone uproariously funny or absurd:

4. An object of amusement or laughter:

verb

1. To make jokes; behave playfully:

2. To tease or mock good-humoredly:

The American Heritage® Roget’s Thesaurus. Copyright © 2013, 2014 by Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company. Published by Houghton Mifflin Harcourt Publishing Company. All rights reserved.

Translations

vtipvtipkovatžertžertovatdělat si legraci

vittighedfortælle vittighedergøre grin medlave sjovmorsomhed

kepponenkujepilavitsaillavitsi

šaliti sevic

mókáziktréfatréfálvicc

brandarigera aî gamni sínusegja brandara, grínast meîspaug, brandari

冗談冗談を言う

농담농담하다

iocus

džiokerisjuokai juokaisjuokingas dalykasjuoktisne juokai

jokotjoks

robiť si žarty

šalašaliti se

skämtskämtavitsrolig historia

เรื่องตลกพูดตลก

lời nói đùanói đùa

Collins Spanish Dictionary — Complete and Unabridged 8th Edition 2005 © William Collins Sons & Co. Ltd. 1971, 1988 © HarperCollins Publishers 1992, 1993, 1996, 1997, 2000, 2003, 2005

Collins English/French Electronic Resource. © HarperCollins Publishers 2005

joke

Collins German Dictionary – Complete and Unabridged 7th Edition 2005. © William Collins Sons & Co. Ltd. 1980 © HarperCollins Publishers 1991, 1997, 1999, 2004, 2005, 2007

Collins Italian Dictionary 1st Edition © HarperCollins Publishers 1995

joke

(dʒəuk) noun

1. anything said or done to cause laughter. He told/made the old joke about the elephant in the refrigerator; He dressed up as a ghost for a joke; He played a joke on us and dressed up as a ghost.

2. something that causes laughter or amusement. The children thought it a huge joke when the cat stole the fish.

verb

1. to make a joke or jokes. They joked about my mistake for a long time afterwards.

2. to talk playfully and not seriously. Don’t be upset by what he said – he was only joking.

ˈjoker noun

1. in a pack of playing-cards, an extra card (usually having a picture of a jester) used in some games.

2. a person who enjoys telling jokes, playing tricks etc.

ˈjokingly adverb

He looked out at the rain and jokingly suggested a walk.

it’s no joke

it is a serious or worrying matter. It’s no joke when water gets into the petrol tank.

joking apart/aside

let us stop joking and talk seriously. I feel like going to Timbuctoo for the weekend – but, joking apart, I do need a rest!

take a joke

to be able to accept or laugh at a joke played on oneself. The trouble with him is that he can’t take a joke.

Kernerman English Multilingual Dictionary © 2006-2013 K Dictionaries Ltd.

joke

نُكْتَةٌ, يـَمْزَحُ vtip, vtipkovat fortælle vittigheder, vittighed scherzen, Witz αστειεύομαι, αστείο broma, bromear vitsailla, vitsi blague, blaguer šaliti se, vic scherzare, scherzo 冗談, 冗談を言う 농담, 농담하다 grapje, grappen maken spøk, spøke żart, zażartować brincar, piada шутить, шутка skämt, skämta เรื่องตลก, พูดตลก şaka, şaka yapmak lời nói đùa, nói đùa 开玩笑, 笑话

Multilingual Translator © HarperCollins Publishers 2009

Britannica Dictionary definition of JOKE

[count]

:

something said or done to cause laughter

  • She meant it as a joke, but many people took her seriously.

  • Is this your idea of a joke? [=do you think that what you are saying/doing is funny?]

  • They played a harmless joke on him.

  • They are always making jokes about his car.

  • He thought he could embarrass us, but now the joke is on him. [=he is the one who looks foolish]

  • He just can’t take a joke. [=he does not like it when other people make jokes about him]

:

a brief story with a surprising and funny ending

  • I heard a funny joke yesterday.

  • the punch line of a joke

  • She’s always cracking/telling jokes.

  • I didn’t get/understand the joke.

  • an inside joke [=a joke that is understood only by people with special knowledge about something]




see also in-joke, practical joke

disapproving

:

someone or something that is not worth taking seriously

  • That exam was a joke.

  • Their product became a joke in the industry.

  • He’s in danger of becoming a national joke.

  • It’s no joke to be lost in the woods. = Being lost in the woods is no joke. [=being lost in the woods is a serious situation]

Britannica Dictionary definition of JOKE

:

to say things that are meant to cause laughter

:

to make jokes about someone or something

[no object]

  • My friends would joke about the uniform I had to wear at work.

  • She joked about the possibility of losing her job.

  • I thought he was joking when he said he might quit, but it turned out that he really meant it.

  • Don’t take it seriously: I was only joking.

  • She spent a few minutes joking with reporters after giving her speech.

  • “The report is to be done by tomorrow.” “You’re joking.” = “You must be joking.” [=you must be kidding; what you say is very surprising or hard to believe]

  • It’s no joking matter. [=it is no joke; it is a serious matter]

[+ object]

  • She joked that she could always get work as a truck driver if she lost her job.

joking aside

or British

joking apart

used to introduce a serious statement that follows a humorous statement

  • It looks like this is a job for Superman! But joking aside, this is a serious problem and we’re going to need help.

— jokingly

/ˈʤoʊkɪŋli/

adverb

[more jokingly; most jokingly]

  • She spoke jokingly about the possibility of losing her job.

  • Defenition of the word joke

    • Something said or done for amusement.
    • To say or do something for amusement; to speak humorously.
    • a ludicrous or grotesque act done for fun and amusement
    • activity characterized by good humor
    • a humorous anecdote or remark
    • act in a funny or teasing way
    • tell a joke; speak humorously; «He often jokes even when he appears serious»
    • a triviality not to be taken seriously; «I regarded his campaign for mayor as a joke»
    • a humorous anecdote or remark intended to provoke laughter; «he told a very funny joke»; «he knows a million gags»; «thanks for the laugh»; «he laughed unpleasantly at hisown jest»; «even a schoolboy»s jape is supposed to have some ascertainable point»
    • a triviality not to be taken seriously
    • a humorous anecdote or remark intended to provoke laughter
    • tell a joke; speak humorously

Synonyms for the word joke

    • antic
    • caper
    • comic story
    • funny story
    • gag
    • hoax
    • jape
    • jest
    • jocularity
    • kid
    • laugh
    • mess about
    • not be serious
    • practical joke
    • prank
    • pretend
    • pull somebody’s leg
    • shaggy dog story
    • story
    • stunt
    • tale
    • tall story
    • tease
    • trick
    • wheeze
    • yak
    • yarn

Similar words in the joke

    • blackguard
    • guy
    • jest at
    • joke
    • joke’s
    • joker
    • joker’s
    • jokers
    • laugh at
    • make fun
    • poke fun
    • rib
    • ridicule

Meronymys for the word joke

    • gag line
    • laugh line
    • punch line
    • tag line

Hyponyms for the word joke

    • antic
    • arse around
    • belly laugh
    • blue joke
    • blue story
    • clown
    • clown around
    • dirty joke
    • dirty story
    • dirty trick
    • drollery
    • ethnic joke
    • fool
    • fool around
    • funny
    • funny remark
    • funny story
    • gag
    • good story
    • horse around
    • howler
    • in-joke
    • leg-pull
    • leg-pulling
    • one-liner
    • pleasantry
    • practical joke
    • pun
    • quip
    • riot
    • scream
    • shaggy dog story
    • sick joke
    • sidesplitter
    • sight gag
    • thigh-slapper
    • visual joke
    • waggery
    • wow

Hypernyms for the word joke

    • act
    • behave
    • communicate
    • diversion
    • do
    • humor
    • humour
    • intercommunicate
    • pettiness
    • puniness
    • recreation
    • slightness
    • triviality
    • wit
    • witticism
    • wittiness

See other words

    • What is babysitting
    • The definition of babysitter
    • The interpretation of the word babysits
    • What is meant by dearest
    • The lexical meaning babysitters
    • The dictionary meaning of the word babyish
    • The grammatical meaning of the word babysat
    • Meaning of the word babylon
    • Literal and figurative meaning of the word babying
    • The origin of the word teenager
    • Synonym for the word legume
    • Antonyms for the word teen
    • Homonyms for the word kiddie
    • Hyponyms for the word kidders
    • Holonyms for the word kiddied
    • Hypernyms for the word kiddy
    • Proverbs and sayings for the word kiddies
    • Translation of the word in other languages prowling

Meaning joke

What does joke mean? Here you find 14 meanings of the word joke. You can also add a definition of joke yourself

1

0

 
0

1660s, «to make a joke,» from joke (n.) or else from Latin iocari «to jest, joke,» from iocus «joke, sport, pastime.» Related: Joked; joking.

2

0

 
0

joke

1660s, joque, «a jest, something done to excite laughter,» from Latin iocus «joke, jest, sport, pastime» (source also of French jeu, Spanish juego, Portuguese jogo, Italian gioco), [..]

3

0

 
0

joke

/ˈʤoʊk/ noun plural jokes 1 joke /ˈʤoʊk/ noun plural jokes Learner's definition of JOKE [count] 1  a  : something said or done to cause laughter She meant it as a joke, but many peopl [..]

4

0

 
0

joke

a humorous anecdote or remark intended to provoke laughter; &amp;quot;he told a very funny joke&amp;quot;; &amp;quot;he knows a million gags&amp;quot;; &amp;quot;thanks for the lau [..]

5

0

 
0

joke

something a person says or does to make you laugh

6

0

 
0

joke

khokhme

7

0

 
0

joke

shpas

8

0

 
0

joke

vits

9

0

 
0

joke

vitslen zikh

10

0

 
0

joke

A story that makes people of a certain culture laugh. Please add your jokes to to this collection of computer related jokes. Introduction Here is the beginning of a collection of computer related joke [..]

11

0

 
0

joke

This is not a virus

12

0

 
0

joke

iocus

13

0

 
0

joke

An amusing story.

* Gay

*: Or witty joke our airy senses moves / To pleasant laughter.

Something said or done for amusement, not in seriousness.

»It was a joke!»

* Alexander Pope

*: Enclos [..]

14

0

 
0

joke

female|lang=nl, diminutive of Jo

Dictionary.university is a dictionary written by people like you and me.
Please help and add a word. All sort of words are welcome!

Add meaning

Like this post? Please share to your friends:
  • The meaning of the word put out
  • The meaning of the word is called its
  • The meaning of the word punishment
  • The meaning of the word intelligent
  • The meaning of the word punctuation