Download Article
Download Article
You can solve many real world problems with the help of math. In order to familiarize students with these kinds of problems, teachers include word problems in their math curriculum. However, word problems can present a real challenge if you don’t know how to break them down and find the numbers underneath the story. Solving word problems is an art of transforming the words and sentences into mathematical expressions and then applying conventional algebraic techniques to solve the problem.
-
1
Read the problem carefully.[1]
A common setback when trying to solve algebra word problems is assuming what the question is asking before you read the entire problem. In order to be successful in solving a word problem, you need to read the whole problem in order to assess what information is provided, and what information is missing.[2]
-
2
Determine what you are asked to find. In many problems, what you are asked to find is presented in the last sentence. This is not always true, however, so you need to read the entire problem carefully.[3]
Write down what you need to find, or else underline it in the problem, so that you do not forget what your final answer means.[4]
In an algebra word problem, you will likely be asked to find a certain value, or you may be asked to find an equation that represents a value.- For example, you might have the following problem: Jane went to a book shop and bought a book. While at the store Jane found a second interesting book and bought it for $80. The price of the second book was $10 less than three times the price of he first book. What was the price of the first book?
- In this problem, you are asked to find the price of the first book Jane purchased.
Advertisement
-
3
Summarize what you know, and what you need to know. Likely, the information you need to know is the same as what information you are asked to find. You also need to assess what information you already know. Again, underline or write out this information, so you can keep track of all the parts of the problem. For problems involving geometry, it is often helpful to draw a sketch at this point.[5]
- For example, you know that Jane bought two books. You know that the second book was $80. You also know that the second book cost $10 less than 3 times the price of the first book. You don’t know the price of the first book.
-
4
Assign variables to the unknown quantities. If you are being asked to find a certain value, you will likely only have one variable. If, however, you are asked to find an equation, you will likely have multiple variables. No matter how many variables you have, you should list each one, and indicate what they are equal to.[6]
-
5
Look for keywords.[7]
Word problems are full of keywords that give you clues about what operations to use. Locating and interpreting these keywords can help you translate the words into algebra.[8]
- Multiplication keywords include times, of, and factor.[9]
- Division keywords include per, out of, and percent.[10]
- Addition keywords include some, more, and together.[11]
- Subtraction keywords include difference, fewer, and decreased.[12]
- Multiplication keywords include times, of, and factor.[9]
Advertisement
-
1
Write an equation. Use the information you learn from the problem, including keywords, to write an algebraic description of the story.[13]
-
2
Solve an equation for one variable. If you have only one unknown in your word problem, isolate the variable in your equation and find which number it is equal to. Use the normal rules of algebra to isolate the variable. Remember that you need to keep the equation balanced. This means that whatever you do to one side of the equation, you must also do to the other side.[14]
-
3
Solve an equation with multiple variables. If you have more than one unknown in your word problem, you need to make sure you combine like terms to simplify your equation.
-
4
Interpret your answer. Look back to your list of variables and unknown information. This will remind you what you were trying to solve. Write a statement indicating what your answer means.[15]
Advertisement
-
1
Solve the following problem. This problem has more than one unknown value, so its equation will have multiple variables. This means you cannot solve for a specific numerical value of a variable. Instead, you will solve to find an equation that describes a variable.
- Robyn and Billy run a lemonade stand. They are giving all the money that they make to a cat shelter. They will combine their profits from selling lemonade with their tips. They sell cups of lemonade for 75 cents. Their mom and dad have agreed to double whatever amount they receive in tips. Write an equation that describes the amount of money Robyn and Billy will give to the shelter.
-
2
Read the problem carefully and determine what you are asked to find.[16]
You are asked to find how much money Robyn and Billy will give to the cat shelter. -
3
Summarize what you know, and what you need to know. You know that Robyn and Billy will make money from selling cups of lemonade and from getting tips. You know that they will sell each cup for 75 cents. You also know that their mom and dad will double the amount they make in tips. You don’t know how many cups of lemonade they sell, or how much tip money they get.
-
4
Assign variables to the unknown quantities. Since you have three unknowns, you will have three variables. Let equal the amount of money they will give to the shelter. Let equal the number of cups they sell. Let equal the number of dollars they make in tips.
-
5
Look for keywords. Since they will “combine” their profits and tips, you know addition will be involved. Since their mom and dad will “double” their tips, you know you need to multiply their tips by a factor of 2.
-
6
Write an equation. Since you are writing an equation that describes the amount of money they will give to the shelter, the variable will be alone on one side of the equation.
-
7
Interpret your answer. The variable equals the amount of money Robyn and Billy will donate to the cat shelter. So, the amount they donate can be found by multiplying the number of cups of lemonade they sell by .75, and adding this product to the product of their tip money and 2.
Advertisement
Add New Question
-
Question
How do you solve an algebra word problem?
Daron Cam is an Academic Tutor and the Founder of Bay Area Tutors, Inc., a San Francisco Bay Area-based tutoring service that provides tutoring in mathematics, science, and overall academic confidence building. Daron has over eight years of teaching math in classrooms and over nine years of one-on-one tutoring experience. He teaches all levels of math including calculus, pre-algebra, algebra I, geometry, and SAT/ACT math prep. Daron holds a BA from the University of California, Berkeley and a math teaching credential from St. Mary’s College.
Academic Tutor
Expert Answer
Carefully read the problem and figure out what information you’re given and what that information should be used for. Once you know what you need to do with the values they’ve given you, the problem should be a lot easier to solve.
-
Question
If Deborah and Colin have $150 between them, and Deborah has $27 more than Colin, how much money does Deborah have?
Let x = Deborah’s money. Then (x — 27) = Colin’s money. That means that (x) + (x — 27) = 150. Combining terms: 2x — 27 = 150. Adding 27 to both sides: 2x = 177. So x = 88.50, and (x — 27) = 61.50. Deborah has $88.50, and Colin has $61.50, which together add up to $150.
-
Question
Karl is twice as old Bob. Nine years ago, Karl was three times as old as Bob. How old is each now?
Let x be Bob’s current age. Then Karl’s current age is 2x. Nine years ago Bob’s age was x-9, and Karl’s age was 2x-9. We’re told that nine years ago Karl’s age (2x-9) was three times Bob’s age (x-9). Therefore, 2x-9 = 3(x-9) = 3x-27. Subtract 2x from both sides, and add 27 to both sides: 18 = x. So Bob’s current age is 18, and Karl’s current age is 36, twice Bob’s current age. (Nine years ago Bob would have been 9, and Karl would have been 27, or three times Bob’s age then.)
See more answers
Ask a Question
200 characters left
Include your email address to get a message when this question is answered.
Submit
Advertisement
Video
-
Word problems can have more than one unknown and more the one variable.
-
The number of variables is always equal to the number of unknowns.
-
While solving word problems you should always read every sentence carefully and try to extract all the numerical information.
Show More Tips
Advertisement
References
About This Article
Article SummaryX
To solve word problems in algebra, start by reading the problem carefully and determining what you’re being asked to find. Next, summarize what information you know and what you need to know. Then, assign variables to the unknown quantities. For example, if you know that Jane bought 2 books, and the second book cost $80, which was $10 less than 3 times the price of the first book, assign x to the price of the 1st book. Use this information to write your equation, which is 80 = 3x — 10. To learn how to solve an equation with multiple variables, keep reading!
Did this summary help you?
Thanks to all authors for creating a page that has been read 56,462 times.
Reader Success Stories
-
James Carson
Sep 13, 2019
«I think this is amazing because it explains how and what you need to do.This helped me in algebra, and I recommend…» more
Did this article help you?
Solving Word Problems in Mathematics
What Is a Word Problem? (And How to Solve It!)
Learn what word problems are and how to solve them in 7 easy steps.
Real life math problems don’t usually look as simple as 3 + 5 = ?. Instead, things are a bit more complex. To show this, sometimes, math curriculum creators use word problems to help students see what happens in the real world. Word problems often show math happening in a more natural way in real life circumstances.
As a teacher, you can share some tips with your students to show that in everyday life they actually solve such problems all the time, and it’s not as scary as it may seem.
As you know, word problems can involve just about any operation: from addition to subtraction and division, or even multiple operations simultaneously.
If you’re a teacher, you may sometimes wonder how to teach students to solve word problems. It may be helpful to introduce some basic steps of working through a word problem in order to guide students’ experience. So, what steps do students need for solving a word problem in math?
Steps of Solving a Word Problem
To work through any word problem, students should do the following:
1. Read the problem: first, students should read through the problem once.
2. Highlight facts: then, students should read through the problem again and highlight or underline important facts such as numbers or words that indicate an operation.
3. Visualize the problem: drawing a picture or creating a diagram can be helpful.
Students can start visualizing simple or more complex problems by creating relevant images, from concrete (like drawings of putting away cookies from a jar) to more abstract (like tape diagrams). It can also help students clarify the operations they need to carry out. (next step!)
4. Determine the operation(s): next, students should determine the operation or operations they need to perform. Is it addition, subtraction, multiplication, division? What needs to be done?
Drawing the picture can be a big help in figuring this out. However, they can also look for the clues in the words such as:
– Addition: add, more, total, altogether, and, plus, combine, in all;
– Subtraction: fewer, than, take away, subtract, left, difference;
– Multiplication: times, twice, triple, in all, total, groups;
– Division: each, equal pieces, split, share, per, out of, average.
These key words may be very helpful when learning how to determine the operation students need to perform, but we should still pay attention to the fact that in the end it all depends on the context of the wording. The same word can have different meanings in different word problems.
Another way to determine the operation is to search for certain situations, Jennifer Findley suggests. She has a great resource that lists various situations you might find in the most common word problems and the explanation of which operation applies to each situation.
5. Make a math sentence: next, students should try to translate the word problem and drawings into a math or number sentence. This means students might write a sentence such as 3 + 8 =.
Here they should learn to identify the steps they need to perform first to solve the problem, whether it’s a simple or a complex sentence.
6. Solve the problem: then, students can solve the number sentence and determine the solution. For example, 3 + 8 = 11.
7. Check the answer: finally, students should check their work to make sure that the answer is correct.
These 7 steps will help students get closer to mastering the skill of solving word problems. Of course, they still need plenty of practice. So, make sure to create enough opportunities for that!
At Happy Numbers, we gradually include word problems throughout the curriculum to ensure math flexibility and application of skills. Check out how easy it is to learn how to solve word problems with our visual exercises!
Word problems can be introduced in Kindergarten and be used through all grades as an important part of an educational process connecting mathematics to real life experience.
Happy Numbers introduces young students to the first math symbols by first building conceptual understanding of the operation through simple yet engaging visuals and key words. Once they understand the connection between these keywords and the actions they represent, they begin to substitute them with math symbols and translate word problems into number sentences. In this way, students gradually advance to the more abstract representations of these concepts.
For example, during the first steps, simple wording and animation help students realize what action the problem represents and find the connection between these actions and key words like “take away” and “left” that may signal them.
From the beginning, visualization helps the youngest students to understand the concepts of addition, subtraction, and even more complex operations. Even if they don’t draw the representations by themselves yet, students learn the connection between operations they need to perform in the problem and the real-world process this problem describes.
Next, students organize data from the word problem and pictures into a number sentence. To diversify the activity, you can ask students to match a word problem with the number sentence it represents.
Solving measurement problems is also a good way of mastering practical math skills. This is an example where students can see that math problems are closely related to real-world situations. Happy Numbers applies this by introducing more complicated forms of word problems as we help students advance to the next skill. By solving measurement word problems, students upgrade their vocabulary, learning such new terms as “difference” and “sum,” and continue mastering the connection between math operations and their word problem representations.
Later, students move to the next step, in which they learn how to create drawings and diagrams by themselves. They start by distributing light bulbs equally into boxes, which helps them to understand basic properties of division and multiplication. Eventually, with the help of Dino, they master tape diagrams!
To see the full exercise, follow this link.
The importance of working with diagrams and models becomes even more apparent when students move to more complex word problems. Pictorial representations help students master conceptual understanding by representing a challenging multi-step word problem in a visually simple and logical form. The ability to interact with a model helps students better understand logical patterns and motivates them to complete the task.
Having mentioned complex word problems, we have to show some of the examples that Happy Numbers uses in its curriculum. As the last step of mastering word problems, it is not the least important part of the journey. It’s crucial for students to learn how to solve the most challenging math problems without being intimidated by them. This only happens when their logical and algorithmic thinking skills are mastered perfectly, so they easily start talking in “math” language.
These are the common steps that may help students overcome initial feelings of anxiety and fear of difficulty of the task they are given. Together with a teacher, they can master these foundational skills and build their confidence toward solving word problems. And Happy Numbers can facilitate this growth, providing varieties of engaging exercises and challenging word problems!
The techniques and methods we apply to solve word problems in math will vary from problem to problem.
The techniques and methods we apply to solve a word problem in a particular topic in math will not work for another word problem found in some other topic.
For example, the methods we apply to solve the word problems in algebra will not work for the word problems in trigonometry.
Because, in algebra, we will solve most of the problems without any diagram. But, in trigonometry, for each word problem, we have to draw a diagram. Without diagram, always it is bit difficult to solve word problems in trigonometry.
Even though we have different techniques to solve word problems in different topics of math, let us see the steps which are most commonly used.
The following steps would be useful to solve word problems in Mathematics.
Step 1 :
Understanding the question is more important than any other thing. That is, always it is very important to understand the information given in the question rather than solving.
Step 2 :
If it is possible, we have to split the given information. Because, when we split the given information in to parts, we can understand them easily.
Step 3 :
Once we understand the given information clearly, solving the word problem would not be a challenging work.
Step 4 :
When we try to solve the word problems, we have to introduce «x» or «y» or some other alphabet for unknown value (=answer for our question). Finally we have to get value for the alphabet which was introduced for the unknown value.
Step 5 :
If it is required, we have to draw picture for the given information. Drawing picture for the given information will give us a clear understanding about the question.
Step 6 :
Using the alphabet introduced for unknown value, we have to translate the English statement (information) given in the question as mathematical equation.
In translation, we have to translate the following English words as the corresponding mathematical symbols.
of —-> x (multiplication)
am, is, are, was, were, will be, would be —-> = (equal)
Step 7 :
Once we have translated the English Statement (information) given in the question as mathematical equation correctly, 90% of the work will be over. The remaining 10% is just getting the answer. That is solving for the unknown.
Example :
The age of a man is three times the sum of the ages of his two sons and 5 years hence his age will be double the sum of their ages. Find the present age of the man.
Answer :
Step 1 :
Let us understand the given information. There are two information given in the question.
1. The age of a man is three times the sum of the ages of his two sons. (At present)
2. After 5 years, his age would be double the sum of their ages. (After 5 years)
Step 2 :
Target of the question :
Present age of the man = ?
Step 3 :
Introduce required variables for the information given in the question.
Let x be the present age of the man.
Let y be the sum of present ages of two sons.
Clearly, the value of x to be found.
Because that is the target of the question.
Step 4 :
Translate the given information as mathematical equation using x and y.
First information :
The age of a man is three times the sum of the ages of his two sons.
Translation :
The Age of a man —-> x
is —-> =
Three times sum of the ages of his two sons —-> 3y
Equation related to the first information using x and y is
x = 3y —-(1)
Second Information :
After 5 years, his age would be double the sum of their ages.
Translation :
Age of the man after 5 years —-> (x + 5)
Sum of the ages of his two sons after 5 years is
y + 5 + 5 = y + 10
(Because there are two sons, 5 is added twice)
Double the sum of ages of two sons —-> 2(y + 10)
would be —-> =
Equations related to the second information using x and y is
x + 5 = 2(y + 10) —-(2)
Step 5 :
Solve equations (1) & (2).
From (1), substitute 3y for x in (2).
3y + 5 = 2(y + 10)
3y + 5 = 2y + 20
y = 15
Substitute 15 for y in (1).
x = 3(15)
x = 45
So, the present age of the man is 45 years.
Kindly mail your feedback to v4formath@gmail.com
We always appreciate your feedback.
©All rights reserved. onlinemath4all.com
This article is about algorithmic word problems in mathematics and computer science. For other uses, see Word problem.
In computational mathematics, a word problem is the problem of deciding whether two given expressions are equivalent with respect to a set of rewriting identities. A prototypical example is the word problem for groups, but there are many other instances as well. A deep result of computational theory is that answering this question is in many important cases undecidable.[1]
Background and motivationEdit
In computer algebra one often wishes to encode mathematical expressions using an expression tree. But there are often multiple equivalent expression trees. The question naturally arises of whether there is an algorithm which, given as input two expressions, decides whether they represent the same element. Such an algorithm is called a solution to the word problem. For example, imagine that are symbols representing real numbers — then a relevant solution to the word problem would, given the input , produce the output EQUAL
, and similarly produce NOT_EQUAL
from .
The most direct solution to a word problem takes the form of a normal form theorem and algorithm which maps every element in an equivalence class of expressions to a single encoding known as the normal form — the word problem is then solved by comparing these normal forms via syntactic equality.[1] For example one might decide that is the normal form of , , and , and devise a transformation system to rewrite those expressions to that form, in the process proving that all equivalent expressions will be rewritten to the same normal form.[2] But not all solutions to the word problem use a normal form theorem — there are algebraic properties which indirectly imply the existence of an algorithm.[1]
While the word problem asks whether two terms containing constants are equal, a proper extension of the word problem known as the unification problem asks whether two terms containing variables have instances that are equal, or in other words whether the equation has any solutions. As a common example, is a word problem in the integer group ℤ,
while is a unification problem in the same group; since the former terms happen to be equal in ℤ, the latter problem has the substitution as a solution.
HistoryEdit
One of the most deeply studied cases of the word problem is in the theory of semigroups and groups. A timeline of papers relevant to the Novikov-Boone theorem is as follows:[3][4]
- 1910: Axel Thue poses a general problem of term rewriting on tree-like structures. He states «A solution of this problem in the most general case may perhaps be connected with unsurmountable difficulties».[5][6]
- 1911: Max Dehn poses the word problem for finitely presented groups.[7]
- 1912: Dehn presents Dehn’s algorithm, and proves it solves the word problem for the fundamental groups of closed orientable two-dimensional manifolds of genus greater than or equal to 2.[8] Subsequent authors have greatly extended it to a wide range of group-theoretic decision problems.[9][10][11]
- 1914: Axel Thue poses the word problem for finitely presented semigroups.[12]
- 1930 – 1938: The Church-Turing thesis emerges, defining formal notions of computability and undecidability.[13]
- 1947: Emil Post and Andrey Markov Jr. independently construct finitely presented semigroups with unsolvable word problem.[14][15] Post’s construction is built on Turing machines while Markov’s uses Post’s normal systems.[3]
- 1950: Alan Turing shows the word problem for cancellation semigroups is unsolvable,[16] by furthering Post’s construction. The proof is difficult to follow but marks a turning point in the word problem for groups.[3]: 342
- 1955: Pyotr Novikov gives the first published proof that the word problem for groups is unsolvable, using Turing’s cancellation semigroup result.[17][3]: 354 The proof contains a «Principal Lemma» equivalent to Britton’s Lemma.[3]: 355
- 1954 – 1957: William Boone independently shows the word problem for groups is unsolvable, using Post’s semigroup construction.[18][19]
- 1957 – 1958: John Britton gives another proof that the word problem for groups is unsolvable, based on Turing’s cancellation semigroups result and some of Britton’s earlier work.[20] An early version of Britton’s Lemma appears.[3]: 355
- 1958 – 1959: Boone publishes a simplified version of his construction.[21][22]
- 1961: Graham Higman characterises the subgroups of finitely presented groups with Higman’s embedding theorem,[23] connecting recursion theory with group theory in an unexpected way and giving a very different proof of the unsolvability of the word problem.[3]
- 1961 – 1963: Britton presents a greatly simplified version of Boone’s 1959 proof that the word problem for groups is unsolvable.[24] It uses a group-theoretic approach, in particular Britton’s Lemma. This proof has been used in a graduate course, although more modern and condensed proofs exist.[25]
- 1977: Gennady Makanin proves that the existential theory of equations over free monoids is solvable.[26]
The word problem for semi-Thue systemsEdit
The accessibility problem for string rewriting systems (semi-Thue systems or semigroups) can be stated as follows: Given a semi-Thue system and two words (strings) , can be transformed into by applying rules from ? Note that the rewriting here is one-way. The word problem is the accessibility problem for symmetric rewrite relations, i.e. Thue systems.[27]
The accessibility and word problems are undecidable, i.e. there is no general algorithm for solving this problem.[28] This even holds if we limit the systems to have finite presentations, i.e. a finite set of symbols and a finite set of relations on those symbols.[27] Even the word problem restricted to ground terms is not decidable for certain finitely presented semigroups.[29][30]
The word problem for groupsEdit
Given a presentation for a group G, the word problem is the algorithmic problem of deciding, given as input two words in S, whether they represent the same element of G. The word problem is one of three algorithmic problems for groups proposed by Max Dehn in 1911. It was shown by Pyotr Novikov in 1955 that there exists a finitely presented group G such that the word problem for G is undecidable.[31]
The word problem in combinatorial calculus and lambda calculusEdit
One of the earliest proofs that a word problem is undecidable was for combinatory logic: when are two strings of combinators equivalent? Because combinators encode all possible Turing machines, and the equivalence of two Turing machines is undecidable, it follows that the equivalence of two strings of combinators is undecidable. Alonzo Church observed this in 1936.[32]
Likewise, one has essentially the same problem in (untyped) lambda calculus: given two distinct lambda expressions, there is no algorithm which can discern whether they are equivalent or not; equivalence is undecidable. For several typed variants of the lambda calculus, equivalence is decidable by comparison of normal forms.
The word problem for abstract rewriting systemsEdit
Solving the word problem: deciding if usually requires heuristic search (red, green), while deciding is straightforward (grey).
The word problem for an abstract rewriting system (ARS) is quite succinct: given objects x and y are they equivalent under ?[29] The word problem for an ARS is undecidable in general. However, there is a computable solution for the word problem in the specific case where every object reduces to a unique normal form in a finite number of steps (i.e. the system is convergent): two objects are equivalent under if and only if they reduce to the same normal form.[33]
The Knuth-Bendix completion algorithm can be used to transform a set of equations into a convergent term rewriting system.
The word problem in universal algebraEdit
In universal algebra one studies algebraic structures consisting of a generating set A, a collection of operations on A of finite arity, and a finite set of identities that these operations must satisfy. The word problem for an algebra is then to determine, given two expressions (words) involving the generators and operations, whether they represent the same element of the algebra modulo the identities. The word problems for groups and semigroups can be phrased as word problems for algebras.[1]
The word problem on free Heyting algebras is difficult.[34]
The only known results are that the free Heyting algebra on one generator is infinite, and that the free complete Heyting algebra on one generator exists (and has one more element than the free Heyting algebra).
The word problem for free latticesEdit
|
|
The word problem on free lattices and more generally free bounded lattices has a decidable solution. Bounded lattices are algebraic structures with the two binary operations ∨ and ∧ and the two constants (nullary operations) 0 and 1. The set of all well-formed expressions that can be formulated using these operations on elements from a given set of generators X will be called W(X). This set of words contains many expressions that turn out to denote equal values in every lattice. For example, if a is some element of X, then a ∨ 1 = 1 and a ∧ 1 = a. The word problem for free bounded lattices is the problem of determining which of these elements of W(X) denote the same element in the free bounded lattice FX, and hence in every bounded lattice.
The word problem may be resolved as follows. A relation ≤~ on W(X) may be defined inductively by setting w ≤~ v if and only if one of the following holds:
- w = v (this can be restricted to the case where w and v are elements of X),
- w = 0,
- v = 1,
- w = w1 ∨ w2 and both w1 ≤~ v and w2 ≤~ v hold,
- w = w1 ∧ w2 and either w1 ≤~ v or w2 ≤~ v holds,
- v = v1 ∨ v2 and either w ≤~ v1 or w ≤~ v2 holds,
- v = v1 ∧ v2 and both w ≤~ v1 and w ≤~ v2 hold.
This defines a preorder ≤~ on W(X), so an equivalence relation can be defined by w ~ v when w ≤~ v and v ≤~ w. One may then show that the partially ordered quotient set W(X)/~ is the free bounded lattice FX.[35][36] The equivalence classes of W(X)/~ are the sets of all words w and v with w ≤~ v and v ≤~ w. Two well-formed words v and w in W(X) denote the same value in every bounded lattice if and only if w ≤~ v and v ≤~ w; the latter conditions can be effectively decided using the above inductive definition. The table shows an example computation to show that the words x∧z and x∧z∧(x∨y) denote the same value in every bounded lattice. The case of lattices that are not bounded is treated similarly, omitting rules 2 and 3 in the above construction of ≤~.
Example: A term rewriting system to decide the word problem in the free groupEdit
Bläsius and Bürckert
[37]
demonstrate the Knuth–Bendix algorithm on an axiom set for groups.
The algorithm yields a confluent and noetherian term rewrite system that transforms every term into a unique normal form.[38]
The rewrite rules are numbered incontiguous since some rules became redundant and were deleted during the algorithm run.
The equality of two terms follows from the axioms if and only if both terms are transformed into literally the same normal form term. For example, the terms
- , and
share the same normal form, viz. ; therefore both terms are equal in every group.
As another example, the term and has the normal form and , respectively. Since the normal forms are literally different, the original terms cannot be equal in every group. In fact, they are usually different in non-abelian groups.
A1 | ||
A2 | ||
A3 |
R1 | ||
R2 | ||
R3 | ||
R4 | ||
R8 | ||
R11 | ||
R12 | ||
R13 | ||
R14 | ||
R17 |
See alsoEdit
- Conjugacy problem
- Group isomorphism problem
ReferencesEdit
- ^ a b c d Evans, Trevor (1978). «Word problems». Bulletin of the American Mathematical Society. 84 (5): 790. doi:10.1090/S0002-9904-1978-14516-9.
- ^ Cohen, Joel S. (2002). Computer algebra and symbolic computation: elementary algorithms. Natick, Mass.: A K Peters. pp. 90–92. ISBN 1568811586.
- ^ a b c d e f g Miller, Charles F. (2014). Downey, Rod (ed.). «Turing machines to word problems» (PDF). Turing’s Legacy: 330. doi:10.1017/CBO9781107338579.010. hdl:11343/51723. ISBN 9781107338579. Retrieved 6 December 2021.
- ^ Stillwell, John (1982). «The word problem and the isomorphism problem for groups». Bulletin of the American Mathematical Society. 6 (1): 33–56. doi:10.1090/S0273-0979-1982-14963-1.
- ^ Müller-Stach, Stefan (12 September 2021). «Max Dehn, Axel Thue, and the Undecidable». p. 13. arXiv:1703.09750 [math.HO].
- ^ Steinby, Magnus; Thomas, Wolfgang (2000). «Trees and term rewriting in 1910: on a paper by Axel Thue». Bulletin of the European Association for Theoretical Computer Science. 72: 256–269. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.32.8993. MR 1798015.
- ^ Dehn, Max (1911). «Über unendliche diskontinuierliche Gruppen». Mathematische Annalen. 71 (1): 116–144. doi:10.1007/BF01456932. ISSN 0025-5831. MR 1511645. S2CID 123478582.
- ^ Dehn, Max (1912). «Transformation der Kurven auf zweiseitigen Flächen». Mathematische Annalen. 72 (3): 413–421. doi:10.1007/BF01456725. ISSN 0025-5831. MR 1511705. S2CID 122988176.
- ^ Greendlinger, Martin (June 1959). «Dehn’s algorithm for the word problem». Communications on Pure and Applied Mathematics. 13 (1): 67–83. doi:10.1002/cpa.3160130108.
- ^ Lyndon, Roger C. (September 1966). «On Dehn’s algorithm». Mathematische Annalen. 166 (3): 208–228. doi:10.1007/BF01361168. hdl:2027.42/46211. S2CID 36469569.
- ^ Schupp, Paul E. (June 1968). «On Dehn’s algorithm and the conjugacy problem». Mathematische Annalen. 178 (2): 119–130. doi:10.1007/BF01350654. S2CID 120429853.
- ^ Power, James F. (27 August 2013). «Thue’s 1914 paper: a translation». arXiv:1308.5858 [cs.FL].
- ^ See History of the Church–Turing thesis. The dates are based on On Formally Undecidable Propositions of Principia Mathematica and Related Systems and Systems of Logic Based on Ordinals.
- ^ Post, Emil L. (March 1947). «Recursive Unsolvability of a problem of Thue» (PDF). Journal of Symbolic Logic. 12 (1): 1–11. doi:10.2307/2267170. JSTOR 2267170. S2CID 30320278. Retrieved 6 December 2021.
- ^ Mostowski, Andrzej (September 1951). «A. Markov. Névožmoinost’ nékotoryh algoritmov v téorii associativnyh sistém (Impossibility of certain algorithms in the theory of associative systems). Doklady Akadémii Nauk SSSR, vol. 77 (1951), pp. 19–20». Journal of Symbolic Logic. 16 (3): 215. doi:10.2307/2266407. JSTOR 2266407.
- ^ Turing, A. M. (September 1950). «The Word Problem in Semi-Groups With Cancellation». The Annals of Mathematics. 52 (2): 491–505. doi:10.2307/1969481. JSTOR 1969481.
- ^ Novikov, P. S. (1955). «On the algorithmic unsolvability of the word problem in group theory». Proceedings of the Steklov Institute of Mathematics (in Russian). 44: 1–143. Zbl 0068.01301.
- ^ Boone, William W. (1954). «Certain Simple, Unsolvable Problems of Group Theory. I». Indagationes Mathematicae (Proceedings). 57: 231–237. doi:10.1016/S1385-7258(54)50033-8.
- ^ Boone, William W. (1957). «Certain Simple, Unsolvable Problems of Group Theory. VI». Indagationes Mathematicae (Proceedings). 60: 227–232. doi:10.1016/S1385-7258(57)50030-9.
- ^ Britton, J. L. (October 1958). «The Word Problem for Groups». Proceedings of the London Mathematical Society. s3-8 (4): 493–506. doi:10.1112/plms/s3-8.4.493.
- ^ Boone, William W. (1958). «The word problem» (PDF). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 44 (10): 1061–1065. Bibcode:1958PNAS…44.1061B. doi:10.1073/pnas.44.10.1061. PMC 528693. PMID 16590307. Zbl 0086.24701.
- ^ Boone, William W. (September 1959). «The Word Problem». The Annals of Mathematics. 70 (2): 207–265. doi:10.2307/1970103. JSTOR 1970103.
- ^ Higman, G. (8 August 1961). «Subgroups of finitely presented groups». Proceedings of the Royal Society of London. Series A. Mathematical and Physical Sciences. 262 (1311): 455–475. Bibcode:1961RSPSA.262..455H. doi:10.1098/rspa.1961.0132. S2CID 120100270.
- ^ Britton, John L. (January 1963). «The Word Problem». The Annals of Mathematics. 77 (1): 16–32. doi:10.2307/1970200. JSTOR 1970200.
- ^ Simpson, Stephen G. (18 May 2005). «A Slick Proof of the Unsolvability of the Word Problem for Finitely Presented Groups» (PDF). Retrieved 6 December 2021.
- ^ «Subgroups of finitely presented groups». Mathematics of the USSR-Sbornik. 103 (145): 147–236. 13 February 1977. doi:10.1070/SM1977v032n02ABEH002376.
- ^ a b Matiyasevich, Yuri; Sénizergues, Géraud (January 2005). «Decision problems for semi-Thue systems with a few rules». Theoretical Computer Science. 330 (1): 145–169. doi:10.1016/j.tcs.2004.09.016.
- ^ Davis, Martin (1978). «What is a Computation?» (PDF). Mathematics Today Twelve Informal Essays: 257–259. doi:10.1007/978-1-4613-9435-8_10. ISBN 978-1-4613-9437-2. Retrieved 5 December 2021.
- ^ a b Baader, Franz; Nipkow, Tobias (5 August 1999). Term Rewriting and All That. Cambridge University Press. pp. 59–60. ISBN 978-0-521-77920-3.
- ^
- Matiyasevich, Yu. V. (1967). «Простые примеры неразрешимых ассоциативных исчислений» [Simple examples of undecidable associative calculi]. Doklady Akademii Nauk SSSR (in Russian). 173 (6): 1264–1266. ISSN 0869-5652.
- Matiyasevich, Yu. V. (1967). «Simple examples of undecidable associative calculi». Soviet Mathematics. 8 (2): 555–557. ISSN 0197-6788.
- ^ Novikov, P. S. (1955). «On the algorithmic unsolvability of the word problem in group theory». Trudy Mat. Inst. Steklov (in Russian). 44: 1–143.
- ^ Statman, Rick (2000). «On the Word Problem for Combinators». Rewriting Techniques and Applications. Lecture Notes in Computer Science. 1833: 203–213. doi:10.1007/10721975_14. ISBN 978-3-540-67778-9.
- ^ Beke, Tibor (May 2011). «Categorification, term rewriting and the Knuth–Bendix procedure». Journal of Pure and Applied Algebra. 215 (5): 730. doi:10.1016/j.jpaa.2010.06.019.
- ^ Peter T. Johnstone, Stone Spaces, (1982) Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, ISBN 0-521-23893-5. (See chapter 1, paragraph 4.11)
- ^ Whitman, Philip M. (January 1941). «Free Lattices». The Annals of Mathematics. 42 (1): 325–329. doi:10.2307/1969001. JSTOR 1969001.
- ^ Whitman, Philip M. (1942). «Free Lattices II». Annals of Mathematics. 43 (1): 104–115. doi:10.2307/1968883. JSTOR 1968883.
- ^ K. H. Bläsius and H.-J. Bürckert, ed. (1992). Deduktionsssysteme. Oldenbourg. p. 291.; here: p.126, 134
- ^ Apply rules in any order to a term, as long as possible; the result doesn’t depend on the order; it is the term’s normal form.
Introduction
In the last decades, mathematical word problem solving has gained much attention from both researchers and educational practitioners (Campbell, 1992; Hegarty et al., 1995; Hajer, 1996; Depaepe et al., 2010; Hickendorff, 2011, 2013; Moreno et al., 2011; Boonen et al., 2013; Swanson et al., 2013). Mathematical word problems refer to mathematical exercises that present relevant information on a problem as text, rather than in the form of mathematical notation (Rasmussen and King, 2000; Timmermans et al., 2007). Hence, effectively solving a mathematical word problem is assumed to depend not only on students’ ability to perform the required mathematical operations, but also on the extent to which they are able to accurately understand the text of the word problem (Lewis and Mayer, 1987; Hegarty et al., 1995; Van der Schoot et al., 2009; Jitendra and Star, 2012). Both of these aspects are related in such a way that developing a deeper understanding of the text of the word problem serves as a crucial step before the correct mathematical computations can be performed. Hence, a key challenge for word problem solvers is to get an adequate understanding of the problem statement (Lee et al., 2009; Thevenot, 2010; Boonen et al., 2013).
Two individual skills are relevant in this regard. First, an important factor contributing to a deeper understanding of the text of the word problem is the ability to construct a rich and coherent mental representation containing all (the relations between the) solution-relevant elements that are derived from the text base of the word problem (De Corte et al., 1985; Hegarty et al., 1995; Pape, 2003). That is, word problem solvers have to use a problem-model strategy in which they translate the problem statement into a qualitative mental representation of the problem situation hidden in the text (Pape, 2003; Van der Schoot et al., 2009). This mental representation subsequently allows them to make a solution plan and execute the required mathematical operations. Although successful word problem solvers appear to employ such a problem-model strategy by drawing on their mental representation skills, less successful problem solvers often adopt an impulsive, superficial direct translation strategy, in which they only focus on selecting the presented numbers that, in turn, form the basis for their mathematical calculations (Verschaffel et al., 1992; Hegarty et al., 1995).
The second important individual skill in word problem solving success substantiated by research evidence is the influence of a student’s reading comprehension abilities (Pape, 2003; Van der Schoot et al., 2009; Boonen et al., 2013). It has been suggested that reading comprehension abilities are especially helpful in dealing with semantic-linguistic word problem characteristics such as the sequence of the known elements in the text of the word problem, the degree to which the semantic relations between the given and unknown quantities of the problem are made explicit, and the relevance of the information in the text of the word problem (De Corte et al., 1985, 1990; Verschaffel et al., 1992; Marzocchi et al., 2002).
Moreover, reading comprehension skills appear to be more important in overcoming such textual complexities than being able to use one’s mental representation skills (De Corte et al., 1985, 1990). This might explain why the use of a problem-model strategy is not sufficient in all circumstances. That is, word problems containing semantically complex features require both accurate mental representation skills and reading comprehension skills, whereas for word problems with a lower semantic-linguistic complexity, well-developed mental representational skills might be sufficient.
These findings suggest that, to teach students how to effectively solve mathematical word problems, mental representation skills and reading comprehension skills should both be part of the mathematics education program. Particularly, paying attention to semantic-linguistic features of word problems is relevant to help students improve their word problem solving success, as word problems become semantically more complex as students progress in their educational career, for example, when they make the transition to secondary education. Word problems offered in secondary school subjects like geometry, physics and biology, include more verbal information and generally contain more complex semantic-linguistic text features (Silver and Cai, 1996; Helwig et al., 1999).
The Netherlands, like many other countries, currently places great emphasis on the teaching of word problem solving in contemporary mathematics education (Ruijssenaars et al., 2004; Elia et al., 2009). The teaching of mathematics in the Netherlands takes place within the context of a domain-specific instructional approach, called Realistic Mathematics Education (RME, Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, 2003), where the process of mathematical word problem solving plays an important role (Van den Boer, 2003; Barnes, 2005; Prenger, 2005; Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, 2005; Hickendorff, 2011). Studies investigating the educational practice of RME show that the teaching of mental representation skills receives much attention in word problem solving instruction (Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, 2003; Van Dijk et al., 2003; Elia et al., 2009). However, reading comprehension skills enabling students to become sensitive to semantic-linguistic complexities in a word problem appear to be trained fewer and less explicitly in the instructional practice of RME, in spite of its proven importance in previous studies (e.g., De Corte et al., 1985, 1990; Hegarty et al., 1992). This is presumably because teachers may underestimate or are not aware of the importance of reading comprehension skills for solving word problems (Hajer, 1996; Van Eerde, 2009). Thus, the current approach to teaching word problem solving appears to emphasize the development of mental representation skills, but seems to pay less attention to the role of reading comprehension skills. In this respect, the way in which word problem solving is taught in the RME curriculum does not seem to be aligned with what is currently known from research about the factors involved in effective word problem solving.
Based on the above analysis of the RME curriculum it seems legitimate to assume that students attending such a curriculum may be at a disadvantage when semantic-linguistic characteristics of a word problem have to be taken into account. That is, students from an RME curriculum are likely to experience difficulties when ask to solve mathematical word problems with a high semantic-linguistic complexity. To test this assumption, we compared students’ performance on word problems obtained while following the RME curriculum to their performances on an independent word problem solving task. First, we classified students as successful or less successful word problem solvers with the help of a mathematics test that is part of the RME curriculum, viz., the CITO Mathematics test. This test can be considered a method-specific (i.e., RME-specific) mathematics test of students’ word problem solving performance, as it builds upon the currently used instructional method for word problem solving. Hence, this test reflects the skills that students learn in the RME classroom, in order to solve word problems (Doorman et al., 2007; Hickendorff, 2011). Second, we examined students’ performance on an independent word problem solving test, which contained either word problems that they could solve by only using their mental representation skills, or word problems that required them to also rely on their reading comprehension skills for handling semantic-linguistic complexities in the word problems. This procedure provides an advantage over prior studies of, among others, Hegarty et al. (1995), Pape (2003), and Van der Schoot et al. (2009), which typically used the main dependent variable of the study (i.e., problem solving success) as an outcome measure as well as a means to classify students into successful and less successful word problem solvers. The classification used in the present study, on the other hand, is based on an external, well-established measure of mathematical word problem solving, which is independent of the main dependent variable of the study (i.e., word problem solving success). This allowed us to make more meaningful group comparisons.
As previously mentioned, a key aspect that differentiates successful from less successful word problem solvers concerns their ability to construct an accurate mental representation of the problem text. Previous studies have shown that asking students to solve compare problems, especially inconsistent compare problems (see Example 1), is a suitable method for investigating whether or not they have effectively constructed an accurate mental representation of the problem statement (e.g., Pape, 2003; Van der Schoot et al., 2009).
[Example 1 – inconsistent word problem]
At the grocery store, a bottle of olive oil costs 7 euro.
That is 2 euro more than at the supermarket.
If you need to buy seven bottles of olive oil, how much will it cost at the supermarket?
[Example 2 – consistent word problem]
At the grocery store, a bottle of olive oil costs 7 euro.
At the supermarket, a bottle of olive oil costs 2 euro more than at the grocery store.
If you need to buy 7 bottles of olive oil, how much will you pay at the supermarket?
In inconsistent word problems like the one presented in Example 1, the translation process requires the identification of the pronominal reference ‘that is’ as the indicator of the relation between the value of the first variable (‘the price of a bottle of olive oil at the grocery store’) to the second (‘the price of a bottle of olive oil at the supermarket’). This identification is necessary to become cognizant of the fact that, in an inconsistent compare problem, the relational term ‘more than’ refers to a subtraction operation rather than to an addition operation. So, inconsistent word problems create greater cognitive complexity than consistent word problems (see Example 2), requiring students to ignore the well-established association between more with increases and addition, and less with decreases and subtraction (Schumacher and Fuchs, 2012). Empirical evidence corroborates this interpretation by showing that word problem solvers make more (reversal) errors on inconsistent than on consistent word problems (i.e., consistency effect, Lewis and Mayer, 1987; Pape, 2003; Van der Schoot et al., 2009). Especially students who fail to build an accurate mental representation of the problem statement, and thus immediately start calculating with the given numbers and relational term, seem to be less successful on inconsistent word problems (Hegarty et al., 1995).
In the present study, we expected neither successful nor less successful problem solvers to experience difficulties with solving consistent compare word problems. However, we did assume that successful word problem solvers in the RME curriculum would experience less difficulties with correctly solving inconsistent compare problems as a result of their reliance on mental representation skills (acquired during word problem solving instruction in RME), than less successful problem solvers who employ a more superficial problem solving approach (Verschaffel et al., 1992; Van der Schoot et al., 2009).
It is important to keep in mind that this only holds for consistent and inconsistent compare problems with a low semantic complexity; that is, problems that only tap into students’ ability to construct an accurate mental representation. If the semantic complexity of compare problems increases, we expected that even students classified as successful word problem solvers (according to our classification based on the RME instruction) may come to experience difficulties with correctly solving inconsistent compare problems. In this case, correctly solving a word problem requires students to use both mental representational skills and reading comprehension skills, while word problem solving instruction in RME (presumably) has provided students only with considerable training in the first of these two skills.
A relatively well-studied and accepted way to increase the semantic complexity of (inconsistent) compare problems is to manipulate the relational term (Lewis and Mayer, 1987; Van der Schoot et al., 2009). According to the lexical marking principle (Clark, 1969), it is more difficult to process marked relations terms (such as ‘less’ in the antonym pair ‘more-less,’ ‘narrow’ in ‘wide-narrow’ or ‘short’ in ‘tall-short’) than unmarked relational terms (e.g., more, wide, tall). Consistent with this, research has shown that students find it easier to convert the unmarked relational term ‘more than’ into a subtraction operation than the marked relational term ‘less than’ into an addition operation (Clark, 1969; Lewis and Mayer, 1987; Kintsch, 1998; Pape, 2003; Van der Schoot et al., 2009). In the present study, we therefore refer to word problems containing a marked relational term (‘more than’) as semantically more complex word problems, whereas word problems with an unmarked relational term (‘less than’) are referred to as semantically less complex word problems (see Examples 3 and 4 for examples of marked and unmarked word problems respectively). Importantly, the difficulties experienced with solving marked inconsistent word problems lie in the fact that these problems draw on students’ use of their mental representation skills as well as on their reading comprehension skills. Accordingly, the influence of reading comprehension skills on word problem solving can only be studied for students who mentally represent the problem statement accurately, that is, the group of successful problem solvers in our study. So, although our group of successful word problem solvers may draw upon their mental representation skills, the insufficient attention to reading comprehension skills in the educational practice of RME is likely to cause them to experience difficulties with correctly solving (semantically complex) marked inconsistent word problems.
[Example 3 – marked word problem]
At the grocery store, a bottle of olive oil costs 7 euro.
At the supermarket, a bottle of olive oil costs 2 euro less than at the grocery store.
If you need to buy seven bottles of olive oil, how much will you pay at the supermarket?
[Example 4 – unmarked word problem]
At the grocery store, a bottle of olive oil costs 7 euro.
That is 2 euro less than at the supermarket.
If you need to buy seven bottles of olive oil, how much will it cost at the supermarket?
According to several researchers, the extent to which successful word problem solvers might be able to overcome difficulties with correctly solving marked inconsistent word problems is related to their reading comprehension skills (e.g., Lee et al., 2004; Van der Schoot et al., 2009). Translating a marked relational term like ‘less than’ into an addition operation is found to be closely associated with general measures of reading comprehension (Lee et al., 2004; Van der Schoot et al., 2009). This suggests that reading comprehension skills, together with mental representation skills, might be necessary to deal with semantically complex word problems. The present study therefore also takes into account students’ general reading comprehension ability.
In sum, the present study aimed to test the following hypotheses:
1. We hypothesized that, as a result of difficulties with constructing a coherent mental representation of word problems, less successful word problem solvers in the RME curriculum would make more errors on both unmarked and marked inconsistent word problems than on unmarked and marked consistent word problems.
2. We hypothesized that, as a result of paying insufficient attention to reading comprehension skills in the teaching of word problem solving, successful word problem solvers in the RME curriculum would experience difficulties with solving semantically complex, marked inconsistent word problems, but not with solving semantically less complex, unmarked, inconsistent word problems.
3. We hypothesized that, as a result of the alleged relation between reading comprehension ability and the ability to overcome the semantic-linguistic complexities of a word problem, a positive relation for successful problem solvers exists between reading comprehension ability and the number of correctly solved marked inconsistent word problems.
Materials and Methods
Selection of Participants
Data from 80 Dutch sixth-grade students (42 boys, 38 girls) from eight elementary schools in the Netherlands were collected. These students had a mean age of 11.72 years (SD = 0.40). They were almost equally divided in two groups (by means of the median split method) on the basis of their score on the CITO (Institute for Educational Measurement) Mathematics test (2008). This selection procedure resulted in a group of less successful word problem solvers (N = 41) and a group of successful word problems solvers (N = 39). The CITO Mathematics test is a nationwide standardized test that reflects the way in which word problem solving is instructed in Realistic Mathematics Education. The test contains elements like mental arithmetic (addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division), complex applications (problems involving multiple operations) and measurement and geometry (knowledge of measurement situations), all of which are offered as mathematical word problems. The internal consistency of this test was high (Cronbach’s α = 0.95, Janssen et al., 2010).
Parents provided written informed consent based on printed information about the purpose of the study. This study was carried out in accordance with the ethical procedures of the Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam.
Instruments and Procedure
The two measurement instruments that were used in this study were administrated to the students by three trained independent research assistants in a session of approximately 45 min.
Inconsistency Task
The inconsistency task contained eight two-step compare problems (see Appendix in Supplementary Material) that were selected from the study of Hegarty et al. (1992) and were translated into Dutch. All of the word problems consisted of three sentences. The first sentence of each compare problem was an assignment statement expressing the value of the first variable, namely the price of a product at a well-known Dutch store or supermarket (e.g., At Aldi a bottle of wine costs 4 euro). The second sentence contained a relational statement, expressing the value of the second variable (i.e., the price of this product at another store or supermarket) in relation to the first (e.g., At Boni, a bottle of wine costs 3 euro more than at Aldi). In the third sentence, the problem solver was asked to find a multiple of the value of the second variable (e.g., If you need to buy three bottles of wine, how much will you pay at Boni?). The answer to these compare problems always involved first computing the value of the second variable (e.g., 4 + 3 = 7), and then multiplying this solution by the quantity given in the third sentence (e.g., 7 times 3 = 21).
The eight compare problems were separated in four different word problem types (see Appendix in Supplementary Material) by crossing the following two within-subject factors: Consistency (consistent vs. inconsistent) and Markedness (unmarked vs. marked). Consistency referred to whether the relational term in the second sentence was consistent or inconsistent with the required arithmetic operation. A consistent sentence explicitly expressed the value of the second variable (e.g., At Boni a bottle of wine costs 3 euro [more/less] than at Aldi) introduced in the prior sentence (e.g., At Aldi a bottle of wine costs 4 euro). An inconsistent sentence related the value of the second variable to the first by using a pronominal reference (e.g., That is 3 euro [more/less] than at Aldi). Consequently, the relational term in a consistent compare problem primed the appropriate arithmetic operation (‘more than’ when the required operation is addition, and ‘less than’ when the required operation is subtraction). The relational term in an inconsistent compare problem primed the inappropriate arithmetic operation (‘more than’ when the required operation is subtraction, and ‘less than’ when the required operation is addition). Markedness referred to whether the relational term was a marked (i.e., less than) or an unmarked (i.e., more than) member of the antonym pair ‘more-less.’ As mentioned earlier, markedness was used to manipulate the semantic complexity of the relational term. A marked relational term (i.e., less than) is semantically more complex than an unmarked relational term (i.e., more than). Hence, marked and unmarked word problems were considered as semantically more complex and semantically less complex word problems respectively.
The stimuli were arranged in four material sets. Each participant was presented with eight word problems, two from each word problem type. The order in which the word problems were presented in each set was pseudorandomized. Each set was presented to 20 participants. Across sets and across participants, each word problem occurred equally often in the unmarked/consistent, marked/consistent, unmarked/inconsistent and marked/ inconsistent version to ensure full combination of conditions and materials. Across word problems, we controlled for the difficulty of the required calculations, and for the number of letters in the names of the variables (i.e., stores) and products. To ensure that the execution of the required arithmetic operations would not be a determining factor in students’ word problem solving performance, the operations were selected on the basis of the following rules: (1) the answers to the first step of the operation were below 10; (2) the final answers were between 14 and 40; (3) none of the first steps or final answers contained a fraction of a number or negative number; (4) no numerical value occurred twice in the same problem; and (5) none of the (possible) answers were 1. The numerical values used in consistent and inconsistent problems of each word problem type were matched for magnitude (see Van der Schoot et al., 2009).
For the analyses, we looked at students’ accuracy (i.e., the amount of correct answers) on each of the four word problem types: (1) unmarked/consistent; (2) marked/consistent; (3) unmarked/inconsistent; and (4) marked/inconsistent. The internal consistency of this measure in the present study was high (Cronbach’s α = 0.90).
Reading Comprehension
The (Grade 6 version of the) normed standardized CITO (Institute for Educational Measurement) Test for Reading Comprehension (2010) of the Dutch National Institute for Educational Measurement was used to assess children’s reading comprehension level. This test is part of the standard Dutch CITO pupil monitoring system and is designed to determine general reading comprehension level in elementary school children. This test consists of two modules, each involving a text and 25 multiple choice questions. The questions pertained to the word, sentence or text level, and tapped both the text base and situational representation that the reader constructed from the text (Kintsch, 1998). On this test, children’s reading comprehension level is expressed by a reading proficiency score, which, in this study, ranged from 15 to 95 (M = 40.51, SD = 13.94). The internal consistency of this test was high with a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.89 (Weekers et al., 2011).
Data Analysis
A 2 × 2 × 2 analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted with Consistency (consistent vs. inconsistent) and Markedness (unmarked vs. marked) as within-subject factors and Group (less successful vs. successful word problem solvers) as the between-subject factor. Follow-up tests were performed using paired sample t-tests. The partial eta-squared (ηp2) was calculated as a measure of effect size (Pierce et al., 2004). According to Pierce et al. (2004), values of 0.02, 0.13, and 0.26 represent small, medium, and large effect sizes respectively.
In the present study, the role of reading comprehension in the four word problem types was examined by calculating the product-moment correlations (Pearson’s r) between reading comprehension and the difference score between the unmarked inconsistent and consistent word problem types, and the correlation between reading comprehension and the difference score between the marked inconsistent and consistent word problem types. These difference scores reflect the differences in performance between the consistent and inconsistent word problem types, and can be taken as a measure of the extent to which students are able to construct a mental representation of the described problem situation. The lower the difference score, the less word problem solvers suffer from the inconsistency. The correlations were first calculated for the less successful and successful word problem solvers together, and then, to test the third hypothesis, for each of these groups separately.
Our approach deviates from, but provides an important advantage over, the study by Van der Schoot et al. (2009), who added reading comprehension as a covariate in the repeated measures ANOVA. That is, the results obtained by Van der Schoot et al. (2009) could provide only limited insight into the exact locus of the covariate’s effect, as it was not known which group (less successful or successful word problem solvers) or in which word problem type (consistent unmarked/marked or inconsistent unmarked/marked) reading comprehension played a role. Moreover, it turns out that the repeated measures ANCOVA does change the main effects of the repeated measures compared to assessing the main effects via a simple repeated measures ANOVA (see Thomas et al., 2009). So, the approach used in the present study enabled us to obtain more specific insight into the precise role of reading comprehension in word problem solving. In all analyses an alpha of 0.05 was used to test the significance of the results.
Results
The overall means (M) and standard deviations (SD) for the main factors in this study, as well as their intercorrelations, are displayed in Table 1. As can be seen, there was a significant main effect of Consistency [F(1,78) = 23.84, p = 0.00, ηp2 = 0.23], indicating that consistent word problems were completed more accurately than inconsistent word problems (i.e., consistency effect). There was no significant main effect of Markedness [F(1,78) = 2.64, p = 0.11], suggesting that overall not more errors were made on marked than on unmarked word problems. The main effect of Group was also not significant [(1,78) = 1.15, p = 0.29)], indicating that overall successful problem solvers did not show a higher problem solving performance than less successful problem solvers.
TABLE 1. Overall means, standard deviations, and correlations of the main variables.
Regarding the interacting effects between Consistency and Markedness, the analysis revealed a significant interaction [F(1,78) = 7.64, p = 0.01, ηp2 = 0.09] showing that overall the consistency effect was present for marked word problems but absent for unmarked word problems. Of more interest, in light of our hypotheses, is that, as expected, the Consistency × Markedness interaction differed for less successful and successful word problem solvers. This was evidenced by a significant three-way interaction between Consistency, Markedness, and Group [F(1,78) = 4.32, p = 0.03, ηp2 = 0.05]. In Figure 1, word problem solving performance is presented as a function of consistency (consistent vs. inconsistent) and markedness (marked vs. unmarked) for less successful problem solvers (Figure 1A), and for successful problem solvers (Figure 1B), respectively.
FIGURE 1. Performance on the four types of word problems for the less successful (A) and successful problem solvers (B).
As shown in Figure 1A, the main effect of Consistency [F(1,38) = 8.16, p = 0.01, ηp2 = 0.18] indicates that less successful word problem solvers showed the consistency effect. Given the non-significant Consistency × Markedness interaction [F(1,38) = 0.25, p = 0.62], the consistency effect was present for both marked and unmarked word problems. No significant main effect of Markedness was found [F(1,38) = 0.12, p = 0.74]. So, less successful word problem solvers performed significantly lower on both the unmarked and marked inconsistent word problem types, compared to the consistent unmarked and marked word problem types [t(38) = 1.86, p = 0.04; t(38) = 2.57, p = 0.01 respectively].
As can be seen in Figure 1B, the group of successful problem solvers resembled the less successful problem solvers in that there was a main effect of Consistency [F(1,40) = 16.29, p = 0.00, ηp2 = 0.29], but no significant main effect of Markedness [F(1,40) = 0.27, p = 0.61]. However, in contrast to the group of less successful problem solvers, the consistency effect in the group of successful problem solvers was present for marked but absent for unmarked word problems [Consistency × Markedness interaction: F(1,40) = 17.44, p = 0.00, ηp2 = 0.30]. This indicates that successful word problem solvers performed significantly lower on marked inconsistent compared to marked consistent word problems [t(40) = 5.07, p = 0.00], whereas performance on unmarked consistent and unmarked inconsistent word problem types did not differ significantly [t(40) = 1.52, p = 0.13].
In sum, these findings show that less successful word problem solvers demonstrated the consistency effect on both semantic-linguistically simple (i.e., unmarked) and complex (i.e., marked) word problems, whereas successful word problem solvers only demonstrated the consistency effect when the word problem text contained complex semantic-linguistic features (i.e., marked).
Regarding the role of reading comprehension skills in word problem solving the following findings were obtained. Overall, there was a significant correlation between reading comprehension and mathematics scores obtained from the curriculum-specific RME test (r = 0.59, p = 0.00). This suggests that students with higher reading comprehension scores also showed higher scores on the RME mathematics test. To obtain more detailed insight into the role of reading comprehension skills in solving marked and unmarked word problems, reading comprehension scores were correlated with the difference scores (inconsistent – consistent) computed for the marked and unmarked word problem types. Results showed that reading comprehension was significantly correlated with the difference score for unmarked word problems (r = 0.19, p = 0.04) and had a marginally significant correlation with the difference score for marked word problems (r = 0.17, p = 0.06). This suggests that overall reading comprehension abilities are relevant to solving both marked and unmarked word problems.
When looking at the successful and less successful problem solvers separately, the results showed, similar to the overall findings, that reading comprehension was significantly correlated with the scores on the RME-specific mathematics test for both successful (r = 0.48, p = 0.00) and less successful problem solvers (r = 0.64, p = 0.00). So, for successful and less successful problem solvers higher reading comprehension abilities were associated with higher RME mathematics scores. Furthermore, successful word problem solvers (M = 46.42, SD = 2.66) scored significantly higher on the standardized reading comprehension test than less successful word problem solvers (M = 35.02, SD = 1.27) [t(53.32) = 3.87, p = 0.00].
More specific analyses focusing on the hypothesized relation between reading comprehension skills and solving marked inconsistent word problems revealed the following pattern of findings. In line with our expectations, the results of the correlational analyses between reading comprehension and the difference scores for marked and unmarked word problems showed that only in the group of successful word problem solvers the difference score for the marked word problem type was significantly related to reading comprehension (r = -0.40, p = 0.01). Importantly, reading comprehension was not correlated with the successful word problem solvers’ difference scores for unmarked word problems (r = -0.27, p = 0.10). Furthermore, in the group of less successful word problem solvers, reading comprehension was also not correlated with the difference scores computed for either unmarked (r = -0.04, p = 0.76) or marked word problems (r = -0.04, p = 0.83).
So, only in the group of successful word problem solvers, a higher reading comprehension score was associated with a smaller difference score. That is, the vulnerability for the consistency effect on marked word problems was lower for students who have higher reading comprehension abilities. This suggests that students with higher reading comprehension abilities appear to suffer less from being primed to an inconsistent arithmetic operation (i.e., being directed toward a subtraction operation by ‘less than’ while addition is required) in solving marked inconsistent word problems.
Discussion
This study was motivated by the observation that contemporary RME primarily teaches students to use their mental representation skills, and focuses much less on using reading comprehension skills, to solve mathematical word problems. Against this background, we set out to investigate the assumption that students from an RME curriculum experience difficulties when having to solve mathematical word problems that are semantic-linguistically complex. We therefore designed a study in which we not only manipulated the extent to which mental representation skills were required, but also varied the semantic complexity of the word problems by using a marked (i.e., high semantic complexity) or unmarked (i.e., low semantic complexity) relational term in the word problem text. Moreover, we classified students as successful and less successful word problem solvers on the basis of their performance on an independent and well-established RME-specific mathematics test.
Using this classification procedure, it was hypothesized that less successful word problem solvers would experience difficulties with correctly solving inconsistent word problems irrespective of their semantic complexity (Hypothesis 1). This hypothesis was confirmed by our analyses, which showed that less successful word problem solvers performed poorly on both marked and unmarked inconsistent word problems. Successful word problem solvers, on the other hand, were able to effectively solve inconsistent word problems that had a low semantic complexity. So, these findings show that the RME-based classification in successful and less successful problem solvers was also reflected in our experimental word problem solving task.
However, on semantically complex word problems even the successful problem solvers experienced difficulties, as indicated by the large number of errors they made on marked inconsistent word problems (Hypothesis 2). More concretely, successful word problem solvers found it more difficult to translate a marked relational term (‘less than’) into an addition operation, than to translate an unmarked relational term (‘more than’) into a subtraction operation.
These findings once again support prior observations that (subtle) semantic-linguistic elements of a word problem, more specifically the marked relational term, influence word problem solving success (Clark, 1969; Lewis and Mayer, 1987; Kintsch, 1998; Pape, 2003; Van der Schoot et al., 2009). Moreover, they are in line with empirical work consistently reporting processing problems with marked terms, which are suggested to be caused by the semantic representation of negative poles of antonym pairs (e.g., more than vs. less than) like ‘less than’ being more fixed and complex, and therefore less likely to be reversed, than that of the positive poles like ‘more than’ (e.g., Lewis and Mayer, 1987; for a detailed explanation of the underlying mechanism, see, e.g., Clark, 1969). For example, earlier studies have shown that students are less able to recall marked terms accurately in memory tasks (Clark and Card, 1969), have slower naming responses for marked terms in naming tasks (Schriefers, 1990), have slower solution times for problems with marked adjectives in reasoning problems (French, 1979), and, the finding replicated in this study, experience problems with reversing a marked inconsistent word problem (e.g., Pape, 2003; Van der Schoot et al., 2009).
Importantly, our results reveal the interesting situation that students classified as successful word problem solvers in an RME curriculum are unsuccessful in solving semantically complex (inconsistent) word problems. The fact that successful problem solvers were able to solve inconsistent word problems with a lower semantic complexity suggests that this poor performance on semantically complex word problems is not due to shortcomings in their mental representation skills. Rather, it seems that successful problem solvers particularly have difficulties to effectively handle semantic-linguistic complexities in word problems. This suggests that students lack the reading comprehension skills required for identifying and translating a primed mathematical operation to the ‘word problem appropriate’ mathematical operation. In the case of marked inconsistent word problems, this means that even successful students find it difficult to convert ‘less than’ into an addition operation. Although it could be argued that this is likely the result of the relatively little attention to the development of reading comprehension skills in the context of mathematical word problem solving in RME (e.g., Elia et al., 2009), this speculative interpretation needs to be further substantiated in future research.
Building upon prior studies (e.g., Lee et al., 2004; Van der Schoot et al., 2009), another aim of this study was to investigate whether reading comprehension skills could help (successful) word problem solvers to overcome the semantically complex marked relational term in an inconsistent word problem. In line with our expectations, reading comprehension was positively related to the performance on marked (but not unmarked) inconsistent word problems for the group of successful word problem solvers, whereas for the less successful group no significant relations were found between reading comprehension and word problem solving (Hypothesis 3).
These results provide corroborating evidence that general reading comprehension skills play an important role in students’ ability to correctly solve semantically complex word problems. Moreover, our findings represent an advance over prior work by more specifically delineating which types of word problems and for which students reading comprehension ability might have an effect. This study shows that reading comprehension skills are especially helpful when it comes to improving the performance on semantically complex word problems by successful word problem solvers (as classified by the RME mathematics test). More specifically, reading comprehension skills are relevant for word problem solving primarily in helping students to effectively translate complex (i.e., marked) relations terms encountered in inconsistent word problems to the correct mathematical operation (i.e., addition). From this, it is evident that reading comprehension skills provide a valuable addition to mental representation skills for word problem solving, and that simply relying on mental representation skills is not sufficient to correctly solve semantically complex word problems. This suggests that in addition to teaching students to use their mental representation skills to solve word problems, word problem solving instruction should have sufficient attention for developing and using reading comprehension skills related to identifying and dealing with semantic-linguistic features in the word problem statement.
It is important to start developing such skills early in elementary school, as word problems get semantically more complex when students progress in their educational career, for example when making the transition from elementary to secondary education (Silver and Cai, 1996; Helwig et al., 1999). Particularly in instructional approaches focused on word problem solving that show an imbalance between the amount of instruction time being devoted to the teaching of mental representation skills and reading comprehension skills, such as in RME, it is important to make teachers aware of this unequal distribution. Encouraging them to pay more attention to reading comprehension skills and teaching students how to deal with semantic-linguistic characteristics in word problems would then provide a good starting point to work toward more equally balanced word problem solving instructions. Moreover, it is useful to make a distinction between learning to process more subtle semantic-linguistic text features (like a marked relation term) and dealing with more general semantic text complexities (like the relevance of the information in the word problem text, the explicitness of the described relations, and the sequence of the known elements in the word problem text).
These and other practical aspects of the results, such as finding the optimal balance between the amount of instruction in strategic mental representation skills and reading comprehension skills, remain to be addressed in future research. Presumably, currently effective intervention programs that focus on both strategic mental representation skills and reading comprehension skills, such as schema-based instruction (e.g., Jitendra et al., 2002, 2011), and the Solve It! instruction method (Montague et al., 2000; Krawec et al., 2013), could provide a fruitful starting point in pursuing this challenge.
Author Contributions
All authors listed, have made substantial, direct and intellectual contribution to the work, and approved it for publication.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00191
References
Barnes, H. (2005). The theory of Realistic Math Education as a theoretical framework for teaching low attainers mathematics. Pythagoras 61, 42–57.
Google Scholar
Boonen, A. J. H., Van der Schoot, M., Van Wesel, F., De Vries, M. H., and Jolles, J. (2013). What underlies successful word problem solving? A path analysis in sixth grade students. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 38, 271–279. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2013.05.001
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Campbell, J. I. D. (ed.) (1992). The Nature and Origins of Mathematical Skills. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science Publishers.
Google Scholar
Clark, H. H., and Card, S. K. (1969). Role of semantics in remembering comparative sentences. J. Exp. Psychol. 82, 545–553. doi: 10.1037/h0028370
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
De Corte, E., Verschaffel, L., and De Win, L. (1985). Influence of rewording verbal problems on children’s problem representations and solutions. J. Educ. Psychol. 77, 460–470. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.77.4.460
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
De Corte, E., Verschaffel, L., and Pauwels, A. (1990). Influence of the semantic structure of word problems on second graders’ eye movements. J. Educ. Psychol. 82, 359–365. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.82.2.359
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Depaepe, F., De Corte, E., and Verschaffel, L. (2010). Teachers’ metacognitive and heuristic approaches to word problem solving: analysis and impact on students’ beliefs and performance. ZDM Math. Educ. 42, 205–218. doi: 10.1007/s11858-009-0221-5
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Doorman, M., Drijvers, P., Dekker, T., Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M., de Lange, J., and Wijers, M. (2007). Problem solving as a challenge for mathematics education in the Netherlands. ZDM Math. Educ. 39, 405–418. doi: 10.1007/s11858-007-0043-2
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Elia, I., Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M., and Kovolou, A. (2009). Exploring strategy use and strategy flexibility in non-routine problem solving in primary school high achievers in mathematics. ZDM Int. J. Math. Educ. 41, 605–618. doi: 10.1007/s11858-009-0184-6
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
French, P. L. (1979). Linguistic marking, strategy, and affect in syllogistic reasoning. J. Psycholinguist. Res. 8, 425–449.
Google Scholar
Hajer, M. (1996). Leren in Een Tweede Taal. Interactie in Een Meertalige Mavo-Klas [Learning a Second Language. Interaction in a Multilingual Classroom]. Groningen: Wolters Noordhoff.
Hegarty, M., Mayer, R. E., and Green, C. E. (1992). Comprehension of arithmetic word problems: evidence from students’ eye fixations. J. Educ. Psychol. 84, 76–84. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.84.1.76
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hegarty, M., Mayer, R. E., and Monk, C. A. (1995). Comprehension of arithmetic word problems: a comparison of successful and unsuccessful problem solvers. J. Educ. Psychol. 87, 18–32. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.87.1.18
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Helwig, R., Rozek-Tedesco, M. A., Tindal, G., Heath, B., and Almond, P. J. (1999). Reading as an access to mathematics problem solving on multiple-choice tests for sixth-grade students. J. Educ. Res. 93, 113–125. doi: 10.1080/00220679909597635
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hickendorff, M. (2011). Explanatory Latent Variable Modeling of Mathematical Ability in Primary School: Crossing the Border Between Psychometrics and Psychology. Doctoral dissertation, Leiden University, Leiden.
Google Scholar
Hickendorff, M. (2013). The effects of presenting multidigit mathematics problems in a realistic context on sixth graders’ problem solving. Cogn. Instr. 31, 314–344. doi: 10.1080/07370008.2013.799167
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Janssen, J., Verhelst, N., Engelen, R., and Scheltens, F. (2010). Wetenschappelijke Verantwoording Papieren Toetsen Rekenen-Wiskunde Groep 3 tot en met 8 [Scientific Justification of the. Mathematics Test]. Arnhem: Cito.
Jitendra, A., DiPipi, C. M., and Perron-Jones, N. (2002). An exploratory study of schema-based word problem solving instruction for middle school students with learning disabilities: an emphasis on conceptual and procedural understanding. J. Special Educ. 36, 23–38. doi: 10.1177/00224669020360010301
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Jitendra, A. H., and Star, J. R. (2012). An exploratory study contrasting high– and low achieving students’ percent word problem solving. Learn. Individ. Differ. 22, 151–158. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2011.11.003
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Jitendra, A. K., Star, J. R., Rodriguez, M., Lindell, M., and Someki, F. (2011). Improving students’ proportional thinking using schema-based instruction. Learn. Instr. 21, 731–745. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2011.04.002
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Kintsch, W. (1998). Comprehension: A Paradigm for Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Google Scholar
Krawec, J. L., Huang, J., Montague, M., Kressler, B., and Melia de Alba, A. (2013). The effects of cognitive strategy instruction on knowledge of math problem-solving processes of middle school students with learning disabilities. Learn. Disabil. Q. 36, 80–92. doi: 10.1177/0731948712463368
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lee, K., Ng, E. L., and Ng, S. F. (2009). The contributions of working memory and executive functioning to problem representation and solution generation in algebraic word problems. J. Educ. Psychol. 101, 373–387. doi: 10.1037/a0013843
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lee, K., Ng, S.-W., Ng, E.-L., and Lim, Z.-Y. (2004). Working memory and literacy as predictors of performance on algebraic word problems. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 89, 140–158. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2004.07.001
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lewis, A. B., and Mayer, R. E. (1987). Students’ miscomprehension of relational statements in arithmetic word problems. J. Educ. Psychol. 79, 363–371. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.79.4.363
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Marzocchi, G. M., Lucangeli, D., De Meo, T., Fini, F., and Cornoldi, C. (2002). The disturbing effect of irrelevant information on arithmetic problem solving in inattentive children. Dev. Neuropsychol. 21, 73–92. doi: 10.1207/S15326942DN2101_4
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Montague, M., Warger, C., and Morgan, T. H. (2000). Solve it! Strategy instruction to improve mathematical problem solving. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 15, 110–116. doi: 10.1207/SLDRP1502_7
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Moreno, R., Ozogul, G., and Reisslein, M. (2011). Teaching with concrete and abstract visual representations: effects on students’ problem solving, problem representations, and learning perceptions. J. Educ. Psychol. 103, 32–47. doi: 10.1037/a0021995
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Pape, S. J. (2003). Compare word problems: consistency hypothesis revisited. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 28, 396–421. doi: 10.1016/S0361-476X(02)00046-2
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Pierce, C. A., Block, R. A., and Aguinis, H. (2004). Cautionary note on reporting eta-squared values from multifactor anova designs. Educ. Psychol. Meas. 64, 916–924. doi: 10.1177/0013164404264848
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Prenger, J. (2005). Taal Telt! Een Onderzoek Naar de rol van Taalvaardigheid en Tekstbegrip in het Realistische Rekenonderwijs. [Language Counts! A Study into the Role of Linguistic Skill and Text Comprehension in Realistic Mathematics Education]. Doctoral dissertation, University of Groningen, Groningen.
Rasmussen, C. L., and King, K. D. (2000). Locating starting points in differential equations: a realistic mathematics education approach. Int. J. Math. Educ. Sci. Technol. 31, 161–172. doi: 10.1080/002073900287219
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ruijssenaars, A. J. J. M., Van Luit, H., and Van Lieshout, E. C. D. M. (eds.) (2004). Rekenproblemen en Dyscalculie [Arithmetic Problems and Dyscalculia]. Rotterdam: Lemniscaat.
Google Scholar
Schumacher, R. F., and Fuchs, L. S. (2012). Does understanding relational terminology mediate effects of intervention on compare word problem? J. Exp. Child Psychol. 111, 607–628. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2011.12.001
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Silver, E. A., and Cai, J. (1996). An analysis of arithmetic problem posing by middle school students. J. Res. Math. Educ. 27, 521–539. doi: 10.2307/749846
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Swanson, H. L., Lussler, C. M., and Orosco, M. J. (2013). Cognitive strategies, working memory, and growth in word problem solving in children with math difficulties. J. Learn. Disabil. XX, 1–20. doi: 10.1177/0022219413498771
PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Thomas, M. S. C., Annaz, D., Ansari, D., Scerif, G., Jarrold, C., and Karmiloff-Smith, A. (2009). Using developmental trajectories to understand developmental disorders. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 52, 336–358. doi: 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/07-0144)
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Timmermans, R. E., Van Lieshout, E. D. C. M., and Verhoeven, L. (2007). Gender related effects of contemporary math instruction for low performers on problem-solving behavior. Learn. Instr. 17, 42–54. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2006.11.005
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Van den Boer, C. (2003). Als je Begrijpt wat ik Bedoel. Een Zoektocht naar Verklaringen voor Achterblijvende Prestaties van Allochtone Leerlingen in het Wiskundeonderwijs [If You See What I Mean. A Quest for an Explanation of the Lower Achievement Levels of Minority Students in Mathematics Education]. Utrecht: CD-ß Press.
Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M. (2003). The didactical use of models in realistic mathematics education: an example from a longitudinal trajectory on percentage. Educ. Stud. Math. 54, 9–35. doi: 10.1023/B:EDUC.0000005212.03219.dc
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Van den Heuvel-Panhuizen, M. (2005). The role of contexts in assessment problems in mathematics. Learn. Math. 25, 2–9.
Google Scholar
Van der Schoot, M., Bakker Arkema, A. H., Horsley, T. M., and Van Lieshout, E. D. C. M. (2009). The consistency effect depends on markedness in less successful but not successful problem solvers: an eye movement study in primary school children. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 34, 58–66. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2008.07.002
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Van Dijk, I. M. A. W., Van Oers, H. J. M., and Terwel, J. (2003). Providing or designing? Constructing models in primary math education. Learn. Instr. 13, 53–72. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4752(01)00037-8
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Van Eerde, H. A. A. (2009). Rekenen-wiskunde en taal: een didactisch duo [Arithmetic and language: a didactical duo]. Panama Post Reken Wiskunde Onderwijs Onderzoek Ontwikkeling Praktijk 28, 19–32.
Verschaffel, L., De Corte, E., and Pauwels, A. (1992). Solving compare problems: an eye movement test of Lewis and Mayer’s consistency hypothesis. J. Educ. Psychol. 84, 85–94. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.84.1.85
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Weekers, A., Groenen, I., Kleintjes, F., and Feenstra, H. (2011). Wetenschappelijke Verantwoording Papieren Toetsen Begrijpend Lezen Voor Groep 7 en 8 [Scientific Justification of the Reading Comprehension Test]. Arnhem: Cito.
Google Scholar