Is perception a word

Perception is a great word. Besides “the ability to see and hear” it also means a way in which something is understood or interpreted.

Let’s look at some examples:

Dopamine and Serotonin Play Key Role in Perception and Cognition (action or process of acquiring knowledge), Study Finds.  (ability to see and hear)

pers

Study of 2020 Protests Shows Difference Between Reality and Perception.  (a way people see or interpret something)

My Biggest Mistake with "Perception" and "Percept": How to Use Them Right

I like your perception of the whole COVID thing. (a way people see or interpret something)

It’s a cool, advanced level word.

It’s pronounced as /pərˈsɛpʃən/ in American English. In the British version the R is omitted /pəˈsep.ʃən/. The stress is on the second syllable in both dialects: pə(r)ˈsɛpʃən

And it sounds much cooler than simple “understanding”. I often use the word perception when speaking about the different types of mentality and differences between people from various cultures.

For instance:
My perception of the USA is different from Americans’ perception of their culture.

So, I liked to use this word and I did it quite often.

But at some point, I wanted to tell about perception using a verb and I found nothing better than just use the word “percept”. And I pronounced it as pərˈsɛpt stressing the second syllable.?

I was thinking if we can say translation (n) – translate (v) or interpretation (n) – interpret (v), we can say perception – percept.

It turned out that we can’t.

Percept: meaning

Although the word ‘percept’ does exist in English (thank God!) BUT it’s not a verb but a noun and it’s pronounced differently. The stress is on the first syllable in American English and on the second syllable in the British version. (Here you can listen to both).

Percept /ˈpɜrsɛpt/ has two meanings:

– an object of perception
– a mental concept that is developed as a consequence of the process of perception.

Let’s look at some examples:

“…. this is an ambiguous visual percept.” (an object of perception)

percept

Do you see a duck or a rabbit?

“So your brain is very good at merging what we receive, the view from the outside word from each eye, and merges them together to give our percept of the world.” (perception)

“Individuals claim they were lip-reading a vague visual percept but could not clearly see a face…” (voice hallucinations, an object of perception)

“So the invitation is to see if you can be with a percept without getting caught in your liking or disliking of the percept.” (an object of perception)

As you can see there’s no way to use “precept” as a verb.

So, what’s the right verb for perception?

Let’s get back to our first example.

Dopamine and serotonin play a key role in perception and cognition, the study finds.

It’s actually the title of the article. And if we start reading the article we’ll see which verb we can use to describe the perception.

perceive in a sentence

“Dopamine and serotonin — two brain chemicals involved in reward processing and whose pathways are treatment targets for diseases such as Parkinson’s — also play a key role in controlling the way people perceive the world and make decisions based on those perceptions.”  

Yes, it’s “perceive” /pərˈsiv/ in American English; the R is omitted in the British version /pəˈsiːv/.

It has several meanings and one of them is “to have an opinion or a belief about something.”

Here’re more examples with “perceive” in a sentence:

How do the French perceive the British?

Actresses and actors are often perceived to be superficial.

How to Use ‘Perception’ in a Sentence [Examples from Youglish]:

(So you can shadow them)

“In ordinary waking states, we cannot perceive the passing of the moment. And so this perception, this… “

“their partner the way that other people perceive them..”

“I’ve mentioned before that the way you perceive something makes or breaks you.”

“I don’t perceive what I’m drawing as ice or water.”

“Nothing in our experience actually exists outside of our perception of it, our cognition, … You know, the world as it actually might ontologically bear no relation to the way in which we perceive it.” 

(here’s the link for practice these sentences)

“… started to experiment with altering their perception using psychedelic, i.e mind-manifesting drugs.”

” …it expands our horizons, it transforms our perception, it opens our minds and it touches our heart.”

“… feeling, a perception that people don’t have full control over a rapidly changing world.”

“First is perception, so how do you look at this situation, ..”

“And that perception has been changing over the last few years in the direction of more ignorance.”

“I don’t perceive what I’m drawing as ice or water.”

(here’s the link for practice these sentences)

P.S.
Okay, that’s it for this week. Let me know in the comments if you made the same mistake with percept and perception or with any other tricky couple of English words. And of course, I’m waiting for your own sentences with any of these three words. 

Do you want Weekly Vocabulary Tips in your Inbox? Sign up here

My Biggest Mistake with "Perception" and "Percept": How to Use Them Right

Perception is defined as the process by which an individual selects, organizes and interprets stimuli into a meaningful and coherent picture of the world.

It is a cognitive process by which people attend to incoming stimuli, organise and interpret such stimuli into behaviour.

Table of Content

  • 1 What is Perception?
  • 2 Perception Meaning
  • 3 Perception Definition
  • 4 Perception in Organisational Behavior
  • 5 Nature of perception
  • 6 Factors that Influence Perception
    • 6.1 Characteristics of the Perceiver
      • 6.1.1 Attitude
      • 6.1.2 Motives
      • 6.1.3 Interest
      • 6.1.4 Experience
      • 6.1.5 Expectation
      • 6.1.6 Self-Concept
    • 6.2 Characteristics of the Target
      • 6.2.1 Physical appearance
      • 6.2.2 Verbal communication
      • 6.2.3 Non-verbal communication
      • 6.2.4 Objects
    • 6.3 Characteristics of the Situation
      • 6.3.1 Time
      • 6.3.2 Work setting
      • 6.3.3 Social setting
  • 7 Managerial Implications of Perception
    • 7.1 Employment Interview
    • 7.2 Performance Evaluation
    • 7.3 Performance Expectations
    • 7.4 Employee Loyalty
  • 8 Implications of Perception on Performance and Satisfaction
    • 8.1 Productivity
    • 8.2 Absenteeism and Turnover
    • 8.3 Job Satisfaction
  • 9 Perception in Consumer Behavior
    • 9.1 Selective Attention
    • 9.2 Selective Distortion
    • 9.3 Selective Retention
  • 10 Perception Bias
    • 10.1 Primacy effect
    • 10.2 Recency effect
    • 10.3 Central traits
    • 10.4 Implicit personality theories
    • 10.5 Projection
    • 10.6 Stereotyping
  • 11 Consumer Imagery

Stimulus is any unit of input to any of the senses; examples of stimuli (i.e. Sensory inputs) include products, packages, brand names; advertisement and commercials. Sensory receptors are the human organs (the eyes, ears, nose, mouth and skin) that receive sensory inputs. These sensory functions are to see, hear, smell, taste and feel respective.

The study of perception is largely the study of what we subconsciously add to or subtract from raw sensory inputs to produce a private picture of the world.

Sensation is the immediate and direct response of the sensory organs to simple stimuli and advertisement, a package, a brand name. Human sensitivity refers to the experiences of sensation.

What is Perception

Perception

Perception Meaning

The word Perception comes from the Latin words, “percipio” meaning “receiving, collecting, action of taking possession, apprehension with the mind or senses”.


Perception Definition

a process by which individuals organize and interpret their sensory impressions in order to give meaning to their environment

Stephen P. Robbins

Perception is an important meditative cognitive process through which persons make interpretations of the stimuli’s or situation they are faced with

Fred Luthans


Perception in Organisational Behavior

Why is perception important in the study of Organisational Behavior?

Simply because people’s behavior is based on their perception of what reality is, not on reality itself. The world as it is perceived is the world that is behaviorally important.

For example, in appraising performance, managers use their perceptions of an employee’s behaviour as a basis for evaluation. One work situation that highlights the importance of perception is the selection interview. Perception is also culturally determined. Based on our cultural backgrounds, we tend to perceive things in certain ways.

Read: What is Organizational Behavior?


Nature of perception

  1. Perception is the process by which an individual gives meaning to the environment.
  2. People‘s actions, emotions, thoughts and feelings are triggered by their perceptions of their surroundings.
  3. Perception has been defined in a variety of ways; it basically refers to the manner in which a person experiences the world.
  4. Perception is an almost automatic process and works in the same way within each individual, yet it typically yields different perceptions.

Read: Theories of Personality


Factors that Influence Perception

  1. Characteristics of the Perceiver
  2. Characteristics of the Target
  3. Characteristics of the Situation
Factors that Influence Perception
Factors that Influence Perception

Characteristics of the Perceiver

Several characteristics of the perceiver can affect perception. When an individual looks at a target and attempts to interpret what he or she stands for, that interpretation is heavily influenced by personal characteristics of the individual perceiver.

The major characteristics of the perceiver influencing perception are:

  1. Attitude
  2. Motives
  3. Interest
  4. Experience
  5. Expectation
  6. Self-Concept
Characteristics of the Perceiver
Characteristics of the Perceiver

Attitude

The attitude and aptitude of employees influence perception formation. If they have positive attitudes towards the management, they directly perceive the stimuli given by management. In the case of negative attitudes, the employees suspect the management’s approach. Employees of high aptitude have a desire and attitude for growth. They behave positively toward the management of an organization.

Motives

The motives and desires of employees cause them to view
stimuli differently as per their level and angle. Helpful motives of the
employees will always assist the management. If they desire to develop
themselves and the organization, they will perceive objects and situations
positively. Employees having low motives will not work sincerely. The
perception will differ depending on different types of motives.

Interest

The interest of individuals draws more attention and
recognition to stimuli. Less attention and recognition lowers the impact of
stimuli or objects on behaviour. If employees lack interest, behaviour pattern
will be less effective, and the perception will be weak.

Experience

The experience of employees results in different levels of
perception. A young employee takes time to understand the object and situation.
Experienced employees generally understand objects quickly and correctly.
However, in contradictory situations, it is difficult to correct aged persons,
whereas the young are easily moulded towards achieving the objectives of the
organization.

Expectation

Expectations distort perceptions. People see what they
expect to see. If they see the object and the situation differently from their
expectations, they get frustrated. They are unable to modify their behaviour.
The employees may expect more pay and so they perceive the management from that
angle. The real stimuli are not properly perceived if expectations exist there
on. The management has to evolve expectations for proper perception.

Self-Concept

Another factor that can affect social perception is the perceivers’
selfconcept. An individual with a positive self-concept tends to notice
positive attributes in another person. In contrast, a negative self-concept can
lead a perceiver to pick out negative traits in another person. Greater
understanding of self allows us to have more accurate perceptions of others.

Read: What is Learning?


Characteristics of the Target

Characteristics in the target that is being observed can
affect what is perceived. Physical appearance plays a big role in our
perception of others. Extremely attractive or unattractive individuals are more
likely to be noticed in a group than ordinary looking individuals.

Motion, sound, size and other attributes of a target shape the way we see it.

  1. Physical appearance
  2. Verbal communication
  3. Non-verbal communication
  4. Objects
characteristics of the target in perception
Characteristics of the Target in Perception

Physical appearance

Physical appearance plays a big role in our perception of
others. The perceiver will notice the target’s physical features like height,
weight, estimated age, race and gender. Perceivers tend to notice physical
appearance characteristics that contrast with the norm, that are intense, or
that are new or unusual.

Verbal communication

Verbal communication from targets also affects our
perception of them. We listen to the topics they speak about, their voice tone,
and their accent and make judgements based on this input.

Non-verbal communication

Non-verbal communication conveys a great deal of information
about the target. The perceiver deciphers eye contact, facial expressions, body
movements, and posture all in an attempt to form an impression of the target.

Targets are not looked at in isolation; the relationship of
a target to its background influences perception because of our tendency to
group close things and similar things together.

Objects

Objects that are close to each other will tend to be
perceived together rather than separately. As a result of physical or time
proximity, we often put together objects or events that are unrelated.

People, objects or events that are similar to each other
also tend to be grouped together. The greater the similarity, the greater the probability
we will tend to perceive them as a group.

Read: What is Attitude?


Characteristics of the Situation

Change in situation leads to incorrect perception about a person.

The factor that influence the perception are:

  1. Time
  2. Work setting
  3. Social setting
Characteristics of the Situation
Characteristics of the Situation

Time

The situation in which the interaction between the perceiver
and the target takes place has an influence on the perceiver’s impression of
the target.

For Example, a person decked up for a party may not be noticeable but the same dress in office would be noticed distinctly, though the person has not changed.

Work setting

You would have very frequently heard people say that their manager is different during working hours and 1800 opposite while in a social setting.

The strength of the situational cues also affects social perception. Some situations provide strong cues as to appropriate behaviour. In these situations, we assume that the individual’s behaviour can be accounted for by the situation and that it may not reflect the individual’s disposition. This is the discounting principle in social perception.

For example, you may encounter an automobile salesperson who has a warm and personable manner, asks you about your work and hobbies, and seems genuinely interested in your taste in cars. Can you assume that this behaviour reflects the salesperson’s personality? You probably cannot, because of the influence of the situation. This person is trying to sell you a car, and in this particular situation, he probably treats all customers in this manner.


Go to Section

What is Perception? | Perception Meaning | Perception Definition | Perception in Organisational Behavior | Nature of perception | Factors that Influence Perception | Importance of Perception | Perception in Consumer Behavior | Perception Bias


Managerial Implications of Perception

People in organisations are always judging each other. Managers must appraise their subordinate’s performance. Let us look at the more obvious applications of perceptions in organisations.

  1. Employment Interview
  2. Performance Evaluation
  3. Performance Expectations
  4. Employee Loyalty
Managerial Implications of Perception
Managerial Implications of Perception

Employment Interview

A major input into who is hired and who is rejected in any organisation is the employment interview. Evidence indicates that interviewers often make inaccurate perceptual judgements. Interviewers generally draw early impressions that become very quickly entrenched.

Performance Evaluation

An employee’s performance appraisal very much depends on the perceptual process. The performance appraisal represents an assessment of an employee’s work. While this can be objective, many jobs are evaluated in subjective terms. Subjective measures are, by definition, judgemental.

Performance Expectations

A manager’s expectations of an individual affect both the manager’s behaviour towards the individual and the individual’s response.

Employee Loyalty

Another important judgement that managers make about employees is whether they are loyal to the organisation. Few organisations appreciate employees, especially those in the managerial ranks openly disparaging the firm.

Perception is an important process in an organisation. It plays a vital role in forming the basis of one’s behaviour by which one formulates a view of the world.


Implications of Perception on Performance and Satisfaction

  1. Productivity
  2. Absenteeism and Turnover
  3. Job Satisfaction
Implications of Perception
Implications of Perception

Productivity

What individuals perceive from their work situation will influence their productivity. More than the situation itself than whether a job is actually interesting or challenging is not relevant. How a manager successfully plans and organizes the work of his subordinates and actually helps them in structuring their work is far less important than how his subordinates perceive his efforts. Therefore, to be abl

Absenteeism and Turnover

Absence and Turnover are some of the reactions to the individual’s perception. Managers must understand how each individual interprets his job. and where there is a significant difference between what is seen and what exists and try to eliminate the distortions.

Job Satisfaction

Job satisfaction is a highly subjective, and feeling of the benefits that derive from the job. Clearly his variable is critically linked to perception. Clearly his variable is critically linked to perception. If job satisfaction is to be improved, the worker’s perception of the job characteristics, supervision and the organisation as a whole must be positive.

Read: What is Motivation?


Perception in Consumer Behavior

Different Perceptions of Consumers: People can emerge with different perceptions of the same object because of these perceptual processes

  1. Selective Attention
  2. Selective Distortion
  3. Selective Retention
Perception in Consumer
Perception in Consumer

Selective Attention

People are exposed to a huge amount of daily stimuli in the form of advertisement but a person cannot possibly give attention to all the advertisements.

Most of the advertisements (stimuli) are screened out. A person perceives only that stimulus (ad of the product) which he needs or which have a unique selling proposition. People would like to notice the following types of stimuli:

  • People are more likely to notice stimuli that relate to the current need.
  • People are more likely to notice stimuli that they anticipate.
  • People are more likely to notice stimuli whose deviations are large in relation to the normal size of stimuli. Ads that are larger in size or that use four colours or are novel and provide contrast are more likely to be noticed.

People differ in terms of the kind of information they prefer. They also have varying preference for media. Some are more interested in appearance, some in social prestige and some in price. Some prefer elaborate ads where as some like simple messages. Consumers show wide variation in their selective attention to commercial stimuli.

Selective Distortion

It describes the tendency of people to twist information into personal meaning. People interpret information in a way that will support rather than challenge their preconceptions.

Selective Retention

People tend to retain information that supports their attitudes and beliefs (selective retention). It has been found that consumers are much more likely to recall seeing advertisements of those brands that they are using or want to use.

Because of these perceptual factors (selective attention, selective distortion, selective retention), marketers use drama and repetition in sending messages to their target market.


Perception Bias

Below are the list of 6 perception bias:

  1. Primacy effect
  2. Recency effect
  3. Central traits
  4. Implicit personality theories
  5. Projection
  6. Stereotyping
Perception Bias
Perception Bias

Primacy effect

The tendency for a perceiver to rely on early cues or first impressions.

Recency effect

The tendency for a perceiver to rely on recent cues or last impressions.

Central traits

Personal characteristics of a target person that are of particular interest to a perceiver

Implicit personality theories

Personal theories that people have about which personality characteristics go together.

Projection

The tendency for perceivers to attribute their own thoughts and feelings to others.

Stereotyping

The tendency to generalize about people in a certain social category and ignore variations among them.


Consumer Imagery

An image is a total perception of something that individuals form by processing all the information they are exposed to over time. Research indicates that consumers develop enduring perceptions or images about brands, prices, stores and companies. These inferences are consumers’ beliefs about products or services.

Consumers may associate an Omega or Rolex watch with quality because of their advertising or word-of-mouth communications from friends. Individuals develop a self-image of themselves and certain brands carry a symbolic value for them. Some products seem to match this self-image of an individual while others do not.

According to Russell W Belk, consumers attempt to enhance or preserve their self-images by purchasing products that they believe correspond to or agree with their self-images and avoid buying products that do not fit their self-images.

Consumers also tend to buy from those outlets that seem to be consistent with their self-image. Many large retail stores and chains in India have started focusing on the need to build their identity to attract certain classes of consumers and create store loyalty among them.

Read Complete: Consumer Imagery


Go to Section:

What is Perception? | Perception Meaning | Perception Definition | Perception in Organisational Behavior | Nature of perception | Factors that Influence Perception | Importance of Perception | Perception in Consumer Behavior | Perception Bias


Reference

  1. Robbins, Stephen P. 2010. Organizational Behaviour. New Delhi, Prentice-Hall.
  2. Furnham, A. (1997). The Psychology of Behaviour at Work. Sussex: Taylor & Francis.
  3. Zalkind, S.S. and Costello, T.W. (1962). Perception: Some Recent Research and Implications for Administration. Administrative Science Quarterly, 7,218-235.

Go On, Share & Help your Friend

Did we miss something in Organizational Behavior Tutorial or You want something More? Come on! Tell us what you think about our post on Perception, Definition, Nature, Factors Influence Perception| Organisational Behavior in the comments section and Share this post with your friends.

Ezoic

Humans are able to have a very good guess on the underlying 3D shape category/identity/geometry given a silhouette of that shape. Computer vision researchers have been able to build computational models for perception that exhibit a similar behavior and are capable of generating and reconstructing 3D shapes from single or multi-view depth maps or silhouettes.[1]

Perception (from Latin perceptio ‘gathering, receiving’) is the organization, identification, and interpretation of sensory information in order to represent and understand the presented information or environment.[2] All perception involves signals that go through the nervous system, which in turn result from physical or chemical stimulation of the sensory system.[3] Vision involves light striking the retina of the eye; smell is mediated by odor molecules; and hearing involves pressure waves.

Perception is not only the passive receipt for of these signals, but it is also shaped by the recipient’s learning, memory, expectation, and attention.[4][5] Sensory input is a process that transforms this low-level information to higher-level information (e.g., extracts shapes for object recognition).[5] The process that follows connects a person’s concepts and expectations (or knowledge), restorative and selective mechanisms (such as attention) that influence perception.

Perception depends on complex functions of the nervous system, but subjectively seems mostly effortless because this processing happens outside conscious awareness.[3] Since the rise of experimental psychology in the 19th century, psychology’s understanding of perception has progressed by combining a variety of techniques.[4] Psychophysics quantitatively describes the relationships between the physical qualities of the sensory input and perception.[6] Sensory neuroscience studies the neural mechanisms underlying perception. Perceptual systems can also be studied computationally, in terms of the information they process. Perceptual issues in philosophy include the extent to which sensory qualities such as sound, smell or color exist in objective reality rather than in the mind of the perceiver.[4]

Although people traditionally viewed the senses as passive receptors, the study of illusions and ambiguous images has demonstrated that the brain’s perceptual systems actively and pre-consciously attempt to make sense of their input.[4] There is still active debate about the extent to which perception is an active process of hypothesis testing, analogous to science, or whether realistic sensory information is rich enough to make this process unnecessary.[4]

The perceptual systems of the brain enable individuals to see the world around them as stable, even though the sensory information is typically incomplete and rapidly varying. Human and other animal brains are structured in a modular way, with different areas processing different kinds of sensory information. Some of these modules take the form of sensory maps, mapping some aspect of the world across part of the brain’s surface. These different modules are interconnected and influence each other. For instance, taste is strongly influenced by smell.[7]

Process and terminology[edit]

The process of perception begins with an object in the real world, known as the distal stimulus or distal object.[3] By means of light, sound, or another physical process, the object stimulates the body’s sensory organs. These sensory organs transform the input energy into neural activity—a process called transduction.[3][8] This raw pattern of neural activity is called the proximal stimulus.[3] These neural signals are then transmitted to the brain and processed.[3] The resulting mental re-creation of the distal stimulus is the percept.

To explain the process of perception, an example could be an ordinary shoe. The shoe itself is the distal stimulus. When light from the shoe enters a person’s eye and stimulates the retina, that stimulation is the proximal stimulus.[9] The image of the shoe reconstructed by the brain of the person is the percept. Another example could be a ringing telephone. The ringing of the phone is the distal stimulus. The sound stimulating a person’s auditory receptors is the proximal stimulus. The brain’s interpretation of this as the «ringing of a telephone» is the percept.

The different kinds of sensation (such as warmth, sound, and taste) are called sensory modalities or stimulus modalities.[8][10]

Bruner’s model of the perceptual process[edit]

Psychologist Jerome Bruner developed a model of perception, in which people put «together the information contained in» a target and a situation to form «perceptions of ourselves and others based on social categories.»[11][12] This model is composed of three states:

  1. When people encounter an unfamiliar target, they are very open to the informational cues contained in the target and the situation surrounding it.
  2. The first stage does not give people enough information on which to base perceptions of the target, so they will actively seek out cues to resolve this ambiguity. Gradually, people collect some familiar cues that enable them to make a rough categorization of the target.
  3. The cues become less open and selective. People try to search for more cues that confirm the categorization of the target. They actively ignore and distort cues that violate their initial perceptions. Their perception becomes more selective and they finally paint a consistent picture of the target.

Saks and John’s three components to perception[edit]

According to Alan Saks and Gary Johns, there are three components to perception:[13]

  1. The Perceiver: a person whose awareness is focused on the stimulus, and thus begins to perceive it. There are many factors that may influence the perceptions of the perceiver, while the three major ones include (1) motivational state, (2) emotional state, and (3) experience. All of these factors, especially the first two, greatly contribute to how the person perceives a situation. Oftentimes, the perceiver may employ what is called a «perceptual defense,» where the person will only see what they want to see.
  2. The Target: the object of perception; something or someone who is being perceived. The amount of information gathered by the sensory organs of the perceiver affects the interpretation and understanding about the target.
  3. The Situation: the environmental factors, timing, and degree of stimulation that affect the process of perception. These factors may render a single stimulus to be left as merely a stimulus, not a percept that is subject for brain interpretation.

Multistable perception[edit]

Stimuli are not necessarily translated into a percept and rarely does a single stimulus translate into a percept. An ambiguous stimulus may sometimes be transduced into one or more percepts, experienced randomly, one at a time, in a process termed multistable perception. The same stimuli, or absence of them, may result in different percepts depending on subject’s culture and previous experiences.

Ambiguous figures demonstrate that a single stimulus can result in more than one percept. For example, the Rubin vase can be interpreted either as a vase or as two faces. The percept can bind sensations from multiple senses into a whole. A picture of a talking person on a television screen, for example, is bound to the sound of speech from speakers to form a percept of a talking person.

Types of perception[edit]

Vision[edit]

In many ways, vision is the primary human sense. Light is taken in through each eye and focused in a way which sorts it on the retina according to direction of origin. A dense surface of photosensitive cells, including rods, cones, and intrinsically photosensitive retinal ganglion cells captures information about the intensity, color, and position of incoming light. Some processing of texture and movement occurs within the neurons on the retina before the information is sent to the brain. In total, about 15 differing types of information are then forwarded to the brain proper via the optic nerve.[14]

The timing of perception of a visual event, at points along the visual circuit, have been measured. A sudden alteration of light at a spot in the environment first alters photoreceptor cells in the retina, which send a signal to the retina bipolar cell layer which, in turn, can activate a retinal ganglion neuron cell. A retinal ganglion cell is a bridging neuron that connects visual retinal input to the visual processing centers within the central nervous system.[15] Light-altered neuron activation occurs within about 5–20 milliseconds in a rabbit retinal ganglion,[16] although in a mouse retinal ganglion cell the initial spike takes between 40 and 240 milliseconds before the initial activation.[17] The initial activation can be detected by an action potential spike, a sudden spike in neuron membrane electric voltage.

A perceptual visual event measured in humans was the presentation to individuals of an anomalous word. If these individuals are shown a sentence, presented as a sequence of single words on a computer screen, with a puzzling word out of place in the sequence, the perception of the puzzling word can register on an electroencephalogram (EEG). In an experiment, human readers wore an elastic cap with 64 embedded electrodes distributed over their scalp surface.[18] Within 230 milliseconds of encountering the anomalous word, the human readers generated an event-related electrical potential alteration of their EEG at the left occipital-temporal channel, over the left occipital lobe and temporal lobe.

Sound[edit]

Anatomy of the human ear. (The length of the auditory canal is exaggerated in this image).

Hearing (or audition) is the ability to perceive sound by detecting vibrations (i.e., sonic detection). Frequencies capable of being heard by humans are called audio or audible frequencies, the range of which is typically considered to be between 20 Hz and 20,000 Hz.[19] Frequencies higher than audio are referred to as ultrasonic, while frequencies below audio are referred to as infrasonic.

The auditory system includes the outer ears, which collect and filter sound waves; the middle ear, which transforms the sound pressure (impedance matching); and the inner ear, which produces neural signals in response to the sound. By the ascending auditory pathway these are led to the primary auditory cortex within the temporal lobe of the human brain, from where the auditory information then goes to the cerebral cortex for further processing.

Sound does not usually come from a single source: in real situations, sounds from multiple sources and directions are superimposed as they arrive at the ears. Hearing involves the computationally complex task of separating out sources of interest, identifying them and often estimating their distance and direction.[20]

Touch[edit]

The process of recognizing objects through touch is known as haptic perception. It involves a combination of somatosensory perception of patterns on the skin surface (e.g., edges, curvature, and texture) and proprioception of hand position and conformation. People can rapidly and accurately identify three-dimensional objects by touch.[21] This involves exploratory procedures, such as moving the fingers over the outer surface of the object or holding the entire object in the hand.[22] Haptic perception relies on the forces experienced during touch.[23]

Gibson defined the haptic system as «the sensibility of the individual to the world adjacent to his body by use of his body.»[24] Gibson and others emphasized the close link between body movement and haptic perception, where the latter is active exploration.

The concept of haptic perception is related to the concept of extended physiological proprioception according to which, when using a tool such as a stick, perceptual experience is transparently transferred to the end of the tool.

Taste[edit]

Taste (formally known as gustation) is the ability to perceive the flavor of substances, including, but not limited to, food. Humans receive tastes through sensory organs concentrated on the upper surface of the tongue, called taste buds or gustatory calyculi.[25] The human tongue has 100 to 150 taste receptor cells on each of its roughly-ten thousand taste buds.[26]

Traditionally, there have been four primary tastes: sweetness, bitterness, sourness, and saltiness. The recognition and awareness of umami, which is considered the fifth primary taste, is a relatively recent development in Western cuisine.[27][28] Other tastes can be mimicked by combining these basic tastes,[26][29] all of which contribute only partially to the sensation and flavor of food in the mouth. Other factors include smell, which is detected by the olfactory epithelium of the nose;[7] texture, which is detected through a variety of mechanoreceptors, muscle nerves, etc.;[29][30] and temperature, which is detected by thermoreceptors.[29] All basic tastes are classified as either appetitive or aversive, depending upon whether the things they sense are harmful or beneficial.[31]

Smell[edit]

Smell is the process of absorbing molecules through olfactory organs, which are absorbed by humans through the nose. These molecules diffuse through a thick layer of mucus; come into contact with one of thousands of cilia that are projected from sensory neurons; and are then absorbed into a receptor (one of 347 or so).[32] It is this process that causes humans to understand the concept of smell from a physical standpoint.

Smell is also a very interactive sense as scientists have begun to observe that olfaction comes into contact with the other sense in unexpected ways.[33] It is also the most primal of the senses, as it is known to be the first indicator of safety or danger, therefore being the sense that drives the most basic of human survival skills. As such, it can be a catalyst for human behavior on a subconscious and instinctive level.[34]

[edit]

Social perception is the part of perception that allows people to understand the individuals and groups of their social world. Thus, it is an element of social cognition.[35]

Though the phrase «I owe you» can be heard as three distinct words, a spectrogram reveals no clear boundaries.

Speech[edit]

Speech perception is the process by which spoken language is heard, interpreted and understood. Research in this field seeks to understand how human listeners recognize the sound of speech (or phonetics) and use such information to understand spoken language.

Listeners manage to perceive words across a wide range of conditions, as the sound of a word can vary widely according to words that surround it and the tempo of the speech, as well as the physical characteristics, accent, tone, and mood of the speaker. Reverberation, signifying the persistence of sound after the sound is produced, can also have a considerable impact on perception. Experiments have shown that people automatically compensate for this effect when hearing speech.[20][36]

The process of perceiving speech begins at the level of the sound within the auditory signal and the process of audition. The initial auditory signal is compared with visual information—primarily lip movement—to extract acoustic cues and phonetic information. It is possible other sensory modalities are integrated at this stage as well.[37] This speech information can then be used for higher-level language processes, such as word recognition.

Speech perception is not necessarily uni-directional. Higher-level language processes connected with morphology, syntax, and/or semantics may also interact with basic speech perception processes to aid in recognition of speech sounds.[38] It may be the case that it is not necessary (maybe not even possible) for a listener to recognize phonemes before recognizing higher units, such as words. In an experiment, Richard M. Warren replaced one phoneme of a word with a cough-like sound. His subjects restored the missing speech sound perceptually without any difficulty. Moreover, they were not able to accurately identify which phoneme had even been disturbed.[39]

Faces[edit]

Facial perception refers to cognitive processes specialized in handling human faces (including perceiving the identity of an individual) and facial expressions (such as emotional cues.)

[edit]

The somatosensory cortex is a part of the brain that receives and encodes sensory information from receptors of the entire body.[40]

Affective touch is a type of sensory information that elicits an emotional reaction and is usually social in nature. Such information is actually coded differently than other sensory information. Though the intensity of affective touch is still encoded in the primary somatosensory cortex, the feeling of pleasantness associated with affective touch is activated more in the anterior cingulate cortex. Increased blood oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) contrast imaging, identified during functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), shows that signals in the anterior cingulate cortex, as well as the prefrontal cortex, are highly correlated with pleasantness scores of affective touch. Inhibitory transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) of the primary somatosensory cortex inhibits the perception of affective touch intensity, but not affective touch pleasantness. Therefore, the S1 is not directly involved in processing socially affective touch pleasantness, but still plays a role in discriminating touch location and intensity.[41]

Multi-modal perception[edit]

Multi-modal perception refers to concurrent stimulation in more than one sensory modality and the effect such has on the perception of events and objects in the world.[42]

Time (chronoception)[edit]

Chronoception refers to how the passage of time is perceived and experienced. Although the sense of time is not associated with a specific sensory system, the work of psychologists and neuroscientists indicates that human brains do have a system governing the perception of time,[43][44] composed of a highly distributed system involving the cerebral cortex, cerebellum, and basal ganglia. One particular component of the brain, the suprachiasmatic nucleus, is responsible for the circadian rhythm (commonly known as one’s «internal clock»), while other cell clusters appear to be capable of shorter-range timekeeping, known as an ultradian rhythm.

One or more dopaminergic pathways in the central nervous system appear to have a strong modulatory influence on mental chronometry, particularly interval timing.[45]

Agency[edit]

Sense of agency refers to the subjective feeling of having chosen a particular action. Some conditions, such as schizophrenia, can cause a loss of this sense, which may lead a person into delusions, such as feeling like a machine or like an outside source is controlling them. An opposite extreme can also occur, where people experience everything in their environment as though they had decided that it would happen.[46]

Even in non-pathological cases, there is a measurable difference between the making of a decision and the feeling of agency. Through methods such as the Libet experiment, a gap of half a second or more can be detected from the time when there are detectable neurological signs of a decision having been made to the time when the subject actually becomes conscious of the decision.

There are also experiments in which an illusion of agency is induced in psychologically normal subjects. In 1999, psychologists Wegner and Wheatley gave subjects instructions to move a mouse around a scene and point to an image about once every thirty seconds. However, a second person—acting as a test subject but actually a confederate—had their hand on the mouse at the same time, and controlled some of the movement. Experimenters were able to arrange for subjects to perceive certain «forced stops» as if they were their own choice.[47][48]

Familiarity[edit]

Recognition memory is sometimes divided into two functions by neuroscientists: familiarity and recollection.[49] A strong sense of familiarity can occur without any recollection, for example in cases of deja vu.

The temporal lobe (specifically the perirhinal cortex) responds differently to stimuli that feel novel compared to stimuli that feel familiar. Firing rates in the perirhinal cortex are connected with the sense of familiarity in humans and other mammals. In tests, stimulating this area at 10–15 Hz caused animals to treat even novel images as familiar, and stimulation at 30–40 Hz caused novel images to be partially treated as familiar.[50] In particular, stimulation at 30–40 Hz led to animals looking at a familiar image for longer periods, as they would for an unfamiliar one, though it did not lead to the same exploration behavior normally associated with novelty.

Recent studies on lesions in the area concluded that rats with a damaged perirhinal cortex were still more interested in exploring when novel objects were present, but seemed unable to tell novel objects from familiar ones—they examined both equally. Thus, other brain regions are involved with noticing unfamiliarity, while the perirhinal cortex is needed to associate the feeling with a specific source.[51]

Sexual stimulation[edit]

Sexual stimulation is any stimulus (including bodily contact) that leads to, enhances, and maintains sexual arousal, possibly even leading to orgasm. Distinct from the general sense of touch, sexual stimulation is strongly tied to hormonal activity and chemical triggers in the body. Although sexual arousal may arise without physical stimulation, achieving orgasm usually requires physical sexual stimulation (stimulation of the Krause-Finger corpuscles[52] found in erogenous zones of the body.)

Other senses[edit]

Other senses enable perception of body balance, acceleration, gravity, position of body parts, temperature, and pain. They can also enable perception of internal senses, such as suffocation, gag reflex, abdominal distension, fullness of rectum and urinary bladder, and sensations felt in the throat and lungs.

Reality[edit]

In the case of visual perception, some people can see the percept shift in their mind’s eye.[53] Others, who are not picture thinkers, may not necessarily perceive the ‘shape-shifting’ as their world changes. This esemplastic nature has been demonstrated by an experiment that showed that ambiguous images have multiple interpretations on the perceptual level.

The confusing ambiguity of perception is exploited in human technologies such as camouflage and biological mimicry. For example, the wings of European peacock butterflies bear eyespots that birds respond to as though they were the eyes of a dangerous predator.

There is also evidence that the brain in some ways operates on a slight «delay» in order to allow nerve impulses from distant parts of the body to be integrated into simultaneous signals.[54]

Perception is one of the oldest fields in psychology. The oldest quantitative laws in psychology are Weber’s law, which states that the smallest noticeable difference in stimulus intensity is proportional to the intensity of the reference; and Fechner’s law, which quantifies the relationship between the intensity of the physical stimulus and its perceptual counterpart (e.g., testing how much darker a computer screen can get before the viewer actually notices). The study of perception gave rise to the Gestalt School of Psychology, with an emphasis on holistic approach.

Physiology[edit]

A sensory system is a part of the nervous system responsible for processing sensory information. A sensory system consists of sensory receptors, neural pathways, and parts of the brain involved in sensory perception. Commonly recognized sensory systems are those for vision, hearing, somatic sensation (touch), taste and olfaction (smell), as listed above. It has been suggested that the immune system is an overlooked sensory modality.[55] In short, senses are transducers from the physical world to the realm of the mind.

The receptive field is the specific part of the world to which a receptor organ and receptor cells respond. For instance, the part of the world an eye can see, is its receptive field; the light that each rod or cone can see, is its receptive field.[56] Receptive fields have been identified for the visual system, auditory system and somatosensory system, so far. Research attention is currently focused not only on external perception processes, but also to «interoception», considered as the process of receiving, accessing and appraising internal bodily signals. Maintaining desired physiological states is critical for an organism’s well-being and survival. Interoception is an iterative process, requiring the interplay between perception of body states and awareness of these states to generate proper self-regulation. Afferent sensory signals continuously interact with higher order cognitive representations of goals, history, and environment, shaping emotional experience and motivating regulatory behavior.[57]

Features[edit]

Constancy[edit]

Perceptual constancy is the ability of perceptual systems to recognize the same object from widely varying sensory inputs.[5]: 118–120 [58] For example, individual people can be recognized from views, such as frontal and profile, which form very different shapes on the retina. A coin looked at face-on makes a circular image on the retina, but when held at angle it makes an elliptical image.[20] In normal perception these are recognized as a single three-dimensional object. Without this correction process, an animal approaching from the distance would appear to gain in size.[59][60] One kind of perceptual constancy is color constancy: for example, a white piece of paper can be recognized as such under different colors and intensities of light.[60] Another example is roughness constancy: when a hand is drawn quickly across a surface, the touch nerves are stimulated more intensely. The brain compensates for this, so the speed of contact does not affect the perceived roughness.[60] Other constancies include melody, odor, brightness and words.[61] These constancies are not always total, but the variation in the percept is much less than the variation in the physical stimulus.[60] The perceptual systems of the brain achieve perceptual constancy in a variety of ways, each specialized for the kind of information being processed,[62] with phonemic restoration as a notable example from hearing.

Law of Closure. The human brain tends to perceive complete shapes even if those forms are incomplete.

Grouping (Gestalt)[edit]

The principles of grouping (or Gestalt laws of grouping) are a set of principles in psychology, first proposed by Gestalt psychologists, to explain how humans naturally perceive objects as organized patterns and objects. Gestalt psychologists argued that these principles exist because the mind has an innate disposition to perceive patterns in the stimulus based on certain rules. These principles are organized into six categories:

  1. Proximity: the principle of proximity states that, all else being equal, perception tends to group stimuli that are close together as part of the same object, and stimuli that are far apart as two separate objects.
  2. Similarity: the principle of similarity states that, all else being equal, perception lends itself to seeing stimuli that physically resemble each other as part of the same object and that are different as part of a separate object. This allows for people to distinguish between adjacent and overlapping objects based on their visual texture and resemblance.
  3. Closure: the principle of closure refers to the mind’s tendency to see complete figures or forms even if a picture is incomplete, partially hidden by other objects, or if part of the information needed to make a complete picture in our minds is missing. For example, if part of a shape’s border is missing people still tend to see the shape as completely enclosed by the border and ignore the gaps.
  4. Good Continuation: the principle of good continuation makes sense of stimuli that overlap: when there is an intersection between two or more objects, people tend to perceive each as a single uninterrupted object.
  5. Common Fate: the principle of common fate groups stimuli together on the basis of their movement. When visual elements are seen moving in the same direction at the same rate, perception associates the movement as part of the same stimulus. This allows people to make out moving objects even when other details, such as color or outline, are obscured.
  6. The principle of good form refers to the tendency to group together forms of similar shape, pattern, color, etc.[63][64][65][66]

Later research has identified additional grouping principles.[67]

Contrast effects[edit]

A common finding across many different kinds of perception is that the perceived qualities of an object can be affected by the qualities of context. If one object is extreme on some dimension, then neighboring objects are perceived as further away from that extreme.

«Simultaneous contrast effect» is the term used when stimuli are presented at the same time, whereas successive contrast applies when stimuli are presented one after another.[68]

The contrast effect was noted by the 17th Century philosopher John Locke, who observed that lukewarm water can feel hot or cold depending on whether the hand touching it was previously in hot or cold water.[69] In the early 20th Century, Wilhelm Wundt identified contrast as a fundamental principle of perception, and since then the effect has been confirmed in many different areas.[69] These effects shape not only visual qualities like color and brightness, but other kinds of perception, including how heavy an object feels.[70] One experiment found that thinking of the name «Hitler» led to subjects rating a person as more hostile.[71] Whether a piece of music is perceived as good or bad can depend on whether the music heard before it was pleasant or unpleasant.[72] For the effect to work, the objects being compared need to be similar to each other: a television reporter can seem smaller when interviewing a tall basketball player, but not when standing next to a tall building.[70] In the brain, brightness contrast exerts effects on both neuronal firing rates and neuronal synchrony.[73]

Theories[edit]

Perception as direct perception (Gibson)[edit]

Cognitive theories of perception assume there is a poverty of stimulus. This is the claim that sensations, by themselves, are unable to provide a unique description of the world.[74] Sensations require ‘enriching’, which is the role of the mental model.

The perceptual ecology approach was introduced by James J. Gibson, who rejected the assumption of a poverty of stimulus and the idea that perception is based upon sensations. Instead, Gibson investigated what information is actually presented to the perceptual systems. His theory «assumes the existence of stable, unbounded, and permanent stimulus-information in the ambient optic array. And it supposes that the visual system can explore and detect this information. The theory is information-based, not sensation-based.»[75] He and the psychologists who work within this paradigm detailed how the world could be specified to a mobile, exploring organism via the lawful projection of information about the world into energy arrays.[76] «Specification» would be a 1:1 mapping of some aspect of the world into a perceptual array. Given such a mapping, no enrichment is required and perception is direct.[77]

Perception-in-action[edit]

From Gibson’s early work derived an ecological understanding of perception known as perception-in-action, which argues that perception is a requisite property of animate action. It posits that, without perception, action would be unguided, and without action, perception would serve no purpose. Animate actions require both perception and motion, which can be described as «two sides of the same coin, the coin is action.» Gibson works from the assumption that singular entities, which he calls invariants, already exist in the real world and that all that the perception process does is home in upon them.

The constructivist view, held by such philosophers as Ernst von Glasersfeld, regards the continual adjustment of perception and action to the external input as precisely what constitutes the «entity,» which is therefore far from being invariant.[78] Glasersfeld considers an invariant as a target to be homed in upon, and a pragmatic necessity to allow an initial measure of understanding to be established prior to the updating that a statement aims to achieve. The invariant does not, and need not, represent an actuality. Glasersfeld describes it as extremely unlikely that what is desired or feared by an organism will never suffer change as time goes on. This social constructionist theory thus allows for a needful evolutionary adjustment.[79]

A mathematical theory of perception-in-action has been devised and investigated in many forms of controlled movement, and has been described in many different species of organism using the General Tau Theory. According to this theory, «tau information», or time-to-goal information is the fundamental percept in perception.

Evolutionary psychology[edit]

Many philosophers, such as Jerry Fodor, write that the purpose of perception is knowledge. However, evolutionary psychologists hold that the primary purpose of perception is to guide action.[80] They give the example of depth perception, which seems to have evolved not to aid in knowing the distances to other objects but rather to aid movement.[80] Evolutionary psychologists argue that animals ranging from fiddler crabs to humans use eyesight for collision avoidance, suggesting that vision is basically for directing action, not providing knowledge.[80] Neuropsychologists showed that perception systems evolved along the specifics of animals’ activities. This explains why bats and worms can perceive different frequency of auditory and visual systems than, for example, humans.

Building and maintaining sense organs is metabolically expensive. More than half the brain is devoted to processing sensory information, and the brain itself consumes roughly one-fourth of one’s metabolic resources. Thus, such organs evolve only when they provide exceptional benefits to an organism’s fitness.[80]

Scientists who study perception and sensation have long understood the human senses as adaptations.[80] Depth perception consists of processing over half a dozen visual cues, each of which is based on a regularity of the physical world.[80] Vision evolved to respond to the narrow range of electromagnetic energy that is plentiful and that does not pass through objects.[80] Sound waves provide useful information about the sources of and distances to objects, with larger animals making and hearing lower-frequency sounds and smaller animals making and hearing higher-frequency sounds.[80] Taste and smell respond to chemicals in the environment that were significant for fitness in the environment of evolutionary adaptedness.[80] The sense of touch is actually many senses, including pressure, heat, cold, tickle, and pain.[80] Pain, while unpleasant, is adaptive.[80] An important adaptation for senses is range shifting, by which the organism becomes temporarily more or less sensitive to sensation.[80] For example, one’s eyes automatically adjust to dim or bright ambient light.[80] Sensory abilities of different organisms often co-evolve, as is the case with the hearing of echolocating bats and that of the moths that have evolved to respond to the sounds that the bats make.[80]

Evolutionary psychologists claim that perception demonstrates the principle of modularity, with specialized mechanisms handling particular perception tasks.[80] For example, people with damage to a particular part of the brain are not able to recognize faces (prosopagnosia).[80] Evolutionary psychology suggests that this indicates a so-called face-reading module.[80]

Closed-loop perception[edit]

The theory of closed-loop perception proposes dynamic motor-sensory closed-loop process in which information flows through the environment and the brain in continuous loops.[81][82][83][84]

Feature integration theory[edit]

Anne Treisman’s feature integration theory (FIT) attempts to explain how characteristics of a stimulus such as physical location in space, motion, color, and shape are merged to form one percept despite each of these characteristics activating separate areas of the cortex. FIT explains this through a two part system of perception involving the preattentive and focused attention stages.[85][86][87][88][89]

The preattentive stage of perception is largely unconscious, and analyzes an object by breaking it down into its basic features, such as the specific color, geometric shape, motion, depth, individual lines, and many others.[85] Studies have shown that, when small groups of objects with different features (e.g., red triangle, blue circle) are briefly flashed in front of human participants, many individuals later report seeing shapes made up of the combined features of two different stimuli, thereby referred to as illusory conjunctions.[85][88]

The unconnected features described in the preattentive stage are combined into the objects one normally sees during the focused attention stage.[85] The focused attention stage is based heavily around the idea of attention in perception and ‘binds’ the features together onto specific objects at specific spatial locations (see the binding problem).[85][89]

Other theories of perception[edit]

  • Enactivism
  • The Interactive Activation and Competition Model
  • Recognition-By-Components Theory (Irving Biederman)

Effects on perception[edit]

Effect of experience[edit]

With experience, organisms can learn to make finer perceptual distinctions, and learn new kinds of categorization. Wine-tasting, the reading of X-ray images and music appreciation are applications of this process in the human sphere. Research has focused on the relation of this to other kinds of learning, and whether it takes place in peripheral sensory systems or in the brain’s processing of sense information.[90] Empirical research show that specific practices (such as yoga, mindfulness, Tai Chi, meditation, Daoshi and other mind-body disciplines) can modify human perceptual modality. Specifically, these practices enable perception skills to switch from the external (exteroceptive field) towards a higher ability to focus on internal signals (proprioception). Also, when asked to provide verticality judgments, highly self-transcendent yoga practitioners were significantly less influenced by a misleading visual context. Increasing self-transcendence may enable yoga practitioners to optimize verticality judgment tasks by relying more on internal (vestibular and proprioceptive) signals coming from their own body, rather than on exteroceptive, visual cues.[91]

Past actions and events that transpire right before an encounter or any form of stimulation have a strong degree of influence on how sensory stimuli are processed and perceived. On a basic level, the information our senses receive is often ambiguous and incomplete. However, they are grouped together in order for us to be able to understand the physical world around us. But it is these various forms of stimulation, combined with our previous knowledge and experience that allows us to create our overall perception. For example, when engaging in conversation, we attempt to understand their message and words by not only paying attention to what we hear through our ears but also from the previous shapes we have seen our mouths make. Another example would be if we had a similar topic come up in another conversation, we would use our previous knowledge to guess the direction the conversation is headed in.[92]

Effect of motivation and expectation[edit]

A perceptual set (also called perceptual expectancy or simply set) is a predisposition to perceive things in a certain way.[93] It is an example of how perception can be shaped by «top-down» processes such as drives and expectations.[94] Perceptual sets occur in all the different senses.[59] They can be long term, such as a special sensitivity to hearing one’s own name in a crowded room, or short-term, as in the ease with which hungry people notice the smell of food.[95] A simple demonstration of the effect involved very brief presentations of non-words such as «sael». Subjects who were told to expect words about animals read it as «seal», but others who were expecting boat-related words read it as «sail».[95]

Sets can be created by motivation and so can result in people interpreting ambiguous figures so that they see what they want to see.[94] For instance, how someone perceives what unfolds during a sports game can be biased if they strongly support one of the teams.[96] In one experiment, students were allocated to pleasant or unpleasant tasks by a computer. They were told that either a number or a letter would flash on the screen to say whether they were going to taste an orange juice drink or an unpleasant-tasting health drink. In fact, an ambiguous figure was flashed on screen, which could either be read as the letter B or the number 13. When the letters were associated with the pleasant task, subjects were more likely to perceive a letter B, and when letters were associated with the unpleasant task they tended to perceive a number 13.[93]

Perceptual set has been demonstrated in many social contexts. When someone has a reputation for being funny, an audience is more likely to find them amusing.[95] Individual’s perceptual sets reflect their own personality traits. For example, people with an aggressive personality are quicker to correctly identify aggressive words or situations.[95]

One classic psychological experiment showed slower reaction times and less accurate answers when a deck of playing cards reversed the color of the suit symbol for some cards (e.g. red spades and black hearts).[97]

Philosopher Andy Clark explains that perception, although it occurs quickly, is not simply a bottom-up process (where minute details are put together to form larger wholes). Instead, our brains use what he calls predictive coding. It starts with very broad constraints and expectations for the state of the world, and as expectations are met, it makes more detailed predictions (errors lead to new predictions, or learning processes). Clark says this research has various implications; not only can there be no completely «unbiased, unfiltered» perception, but this means that there is a great deal of feedback between perception and expectation (perceptual experiences often shape our beliefs, but those perceptions were based on existing beliefs).[98] Indeed, predictive coding provides an account where this type of feedback assists in stabilizing our inference-making process about the physical world, such as with perceptual constancy examples.

Embodied cognition challenges the idea of perception as internal representations resulting from a passive reception of (incomplete) sensory inputs coming from the outside world. According to O’Regan (1992), the major issue with this perspective is that it leaves the subjective character of perception unexplained.[99] Thus, perception is understood as an active process conducted by perceiving and engaged agents (perceivers). Furthermore, perception is influenced by agents’ motives and expectations, their bodily states, and the interaction between the agent’s body and the environment around it.[100]

See also[edit]

  • Action-specific perception
  • Alice in Wonderland syndrome
  • Apophenia
  • Binding Problem
  • Embodied cognition
  • Change blindness
  • Experience model
  • Feeling
  • Generic views
  • Ideasthesia
  • Introspection
  • Model-dependent realism
  • Multisensory integration
  • Near sets
  • Neural correlates of consciousness
  • Pareidolia
  • Perceptual paradox
  • Philosophy of perception
  • Proprioception
  • Qualia
  • Recept
  • Samjñā, the Buddhist concept of perception
  • Simulated reality
  • Simulation
  • Transsaccadic memory
  • Visual routine

References[edit]

Citations[edit]

  1. ^ «Soltani, A. A., Huang, H., Wu, J., Kulkarni, T. D., & Tenenbaum, J. B. Synthesizing 3D Shapes via Modeling Multi-View Depth Maps and Silhouettes With Deep Generative Networks. In Proceedings of the IEEE Conference on Computer Vision and Pattern Recognition (pp. 1511-1519)». GitHub. 28 May 2019. Archived from the original on 9 May 2018.
  2. ^ Schacter, Daniel (2011). Psychology. Worth Publishers. ISBN 9781429237192.
  3. ^ a b c d e f Goldstein (2009) pp. 5–7
  4. ^ a b c d e Gregory, Richard. «Perception» in Gregory, Zangwill (1987) pp. 598–601.
  5. ^ a b c Bernstein, Douglas A. (5 March 2010). Essentials of Psychology. Cengage Learning. pp. 123–124. ISBN 978-0-495-90693-3. Archived from the original on 2 January 2017. Retrieved 25 March 2011.
  6. ^ Gustav Theodor Fechner. Elemente der Psychophysik. Leipzig 1860.
  7. ^ a b DeVere, Ronald; Calvert, Marjorie (31 August 2010). Navigating Smell and Taste Disorders. Demos Medical Publishing. pp. 33–37. ISBN 978-1-932603-96-5. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 26 March 2011.
  8. ^ a b Pomerantz, James R. (2003): «Perception: Overview». In: Lynn Nadel (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Cognitive Science, Vol. 3, London: Nature Publishing Group, pp. 527–537.
  9. ^ «Sensation and Perception». Archived from the original on 10 May 2011. Retrieved 24 March 2011.
  10. ^ Willis, William D.; Coggeshall, Richard E. (31 January 2004). Sensory Mechanisms of the Spinal Cord: Primary afferent neurons and the spinal dorsal horn. Springer. p. 1. ISBN 978-0-306-48033-1. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 25 March 2011.
  11. ^ «Perception, Attribution, and, Judgment of Others» (PDF). Pearson Education. Retrieved 8 March 2020.
  12. ^ Alan S. & Gary J. (2011). Perception, Attribution, and Judgment of Others. Organizational Behaviour: Understanding and Managing Life at Work, Vol. 7.
  13. ^ Sincero, Sarah Mae. 2013. «Perception.» Explorable. Retrieved 8 March 2020 (https://explorable.com/perception).
  14. ^ Gollisch, Tim; Meister, Markus (28 January 2010). «Eye Smarter than Scientists Believed: Neural Computations in Circuits of the Retina». Neuron. 65 (2): 150–164. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2009.12.009. PMC 3717333. PMID 20152123.
  15. ^ Kim US, Mahroo OA, Mollon JD, Yu-Wai-Man P (2021). «Retinal Ganglion Cells-Diversity of Cell Types and Clinical Relevance». Front Neurol. 12: 661938. doi:10.3389/fneur.2021.661938. PMC 8175861. PMID 34093409.
  16. ^ Berry MJ, Warland DK, Meister M (May 1997). «The structure and precision of retinal spike trains». Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 94 (10): 5411–6. Bibcode:1997PNAS…94.5411B. doi:10.1073/pnas.94.10.5411. PMC 24692. PMID 9144251.
  17. ^ Tengölics ÁJ, Szarka G, Ganczer A, Szabó-Meleg E, Nyitrai M, Kovács-Öller T, Völgyi B (October 2019). «Response Latency Tuning by Retinal Circuits Modulates Signal Efficiency». Sci Rep. 9 (1): 15110. Bibcode:2019NatSR…915110T. doi:10.1038/s41598-019-51756-y. PMC 6806000. PMID 31641196.
  18. ^ Kim AE, Gilley PM (2013). «Neural mechanisms of rapid sensitivity to syntactic anomaly». Front Psychol. 4: 45. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00045. PMC 3600774. PMID 23515395.
  19. ^ D’Ambrose, Christoper; Choudhary, Rizwan (2003). Elert, Glenn (ed.). «Frequency range of human hearing». The Physics Factbook. Retrieved 22 January 2022.
  20. ^ a b c Moore, Brian C. J. (15 October 2009). «Audition». In Goldstein, E. Bruce (ed.). Encyclopedia of Perception. Sage. pp. 136–137. ISBN 978-1-4129-4081-8. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 26 March 2011.
  21. ^ Klatzky, R. L.; Lederman, S. J.; Metzger, V. A. (1985). «Identifying objects by touch: An «expert system.»«. Perception & Psychophysics. 37 (4): 299–302. doi:10.3758/BF03211351. PMID 4034346.
  22. ^ Lederman, S. J.; Klatzky, R. L. (1987). «Hand movements: A window into haptic object recognition». Cognitive Psychology. 19 (3): 342–368. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(87)90008-9. PMID 3608405. S2CID 3157751.
  23. ^ Robles-de-la-torre, Gabriel; Hayward, Vincent (2001). «Force can overcome object geometry in the perception of shape through active touch». Nature. 412 (6845): 445–448. Bibcode:2001Natur.412..445R. doi:10.1038/35086588. PMID 11473320. S2CID 4413295.
  24. ^ Gibson, J.J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. ISBN 978-0-313-23961-8.
  25. ^ Human biology (Page 201/464) Archived 2 January 2017 at the Wayback Machine Daniel D. Chiras. Jones & Bartlett Learning, 2005.
  26. ^ a b DeVere, Ronald; Calvert, Marjorie (31 August 2010). Navigating Smell and Taste Disorders. Demos Medical Publishing. pp. 39–40. ISBN 978-1-932603-96-5. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 26 March 2011.
  27. ^ «Umami Dearest: The mysterious fifth taste has officially infiltrated the food scene». trendcentral.com. 23 February 2010. Archived from the original on 18 April 2011. Retrieved 26 March 2011.
  28. ^ «#8 Food Trend for 2010: I Want My Umami». foodchannel.com. 6 December 2009. Archived from the original on 11 July 2011.
  29. ^ a b c Siegel, George J.; Albers, R. Wayne (2006). Basic neurochemistry: molecular, cellular, and medical aspects. Academic Press. p. 825. ISBN 978-0-12-088397-4. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 26 March 2011.
  30. ^ Food texture: measurement and perception (page 3–4/311) Archived 2 January 2017 at the Wayback Machine Andrew J. Rosenthal. Springer, 1999.
  31. ^ Why do two great tastes sometimes not taste great together? Archived 28 November 2011 at the Wayback Machine scientificamerican.com. Dr. Tim Jacob, Cardiff University. 22 May 2009.
  32. ^ Brookes, Jennifer (13 August 2010). «Science is perception: what can our sense of smell tell us about ourselves and the world around us?». Philosophical Transactions. Series A, Mathematical, Physical, and Engineering Sciences. 368 (1924): 3491–3502. Bibcode:2010RSPTA.368.3491B. doi:10.1098/rsta.2010.0117. PMC 2944383. PMID 20603363.
  33. ^ Weir, Kirsten (February 2011). «Scents and sensibility». American Psychological Association. Retrieved 11 December 2018.
  34. ^ Bergland, Christopher (29 June 2015). «Psychology Today». How Does Scent Drive Human Behavior?.
  35. ^ E. R. Smith, D. M. Mackie (2000). Social Psychology. Psychology Press, 2nd ed., p. 20
  36. ^ Watkins, Anthony J.; Raimond, Andrew; Makin, Simon J. (23 March 2010). «Room reflection and constancy in speech-like sounds: Within-band effects». In Lopez-Poveda, Enrique A. (ed.). The Neurophysiological Bases of Auditory Perception. Springer. p. 440. Bibcode:2010nbap.book…..L. ISBN 978-1-4419-5685-9. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 26 March 2011.
  37. ^ Rosenblum, Lawrence D. (15 April 2008). «Primacy of Multimodal Speech Perception». In Pisoni, David; Remez, Robert (eds.). The Handbook of Speech Perception. p. 51. ISBN 9780470756775.
  38. ^ Davis, Matthew H.; Johnsrude, Ingrid S. (July 2007). «Hearing speech sounds: Top-down influences on the interface between audition and speech perception». Hearing Research. 229 (1–2): 132–147. doi:10.1016/j.heares.2007.01.014. PMID 17317056. S2CID 12111361.
  39. ^ Warren, R. M. (1970). «Restoration of missing speech sounds». Science. 167 (3917): 392–393. Bibcode:1970Sci…167..392W. doi:10.1126/science.167.3917.392. PMID 5409744. S2CID 30356740.
  40. ^ «Somatosensory Cortex». The Human Memory. 31 October 2019. Retrieved 8 March 2020.
  41. ^ Case, LK; Laubacher, CM; Olausson, H; Wang, B; Spagnolo, PA; Bushnell, MC (2016). «Encoding of Touch Intensity But Not Pleasantness in Human Primary Somatosensory Cortex». J Neurosci. 36 (21): 5850–60. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1130-15.2016. PMC 4879201. PMID 27225773.
  42. ^ «Multi-Modal Perception». Lumen Waymaker. p. Introduction to Psychology. Retrieved 8 March 2020.
  43. ^ Rao SM, Mayer AR, Harrington DL (March 2001). «The evolution of brain activation during temporal processing». Nature Neuroscience. 4 (3): 317–23. doi:10.1038/85191. PMID 11224550. S2CID 3570715.
  44. ^ «Brain Areas Critical To Human Time Sense Identified». UniSci – Daily University Science News. 27 February 2001.
  45. ^ Parker KL, Lamichhane D, Caetano MS, Narayanan NS (October 2013). «Executive dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease and timing deficits». Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience. 7: 75. doi:10.3389/fnint.2013.00075. PMC 3813949. PMID 24198770. Manipulations of dopaminergic signaling profoundly influence interval timing, leading to the hypothesis that dopamine influences internal pacemaker, or «clock», activity. For instance, amphetamine, which increases concentrations of dopamine at the synaptic cleft advances the start of responding during interval timing, whereas antagonists of D2 type dopamine receptors typically slow timing;… Depletion of dopamine in healthy volunteers impairs timing, while amphetamine releases synaptic dopamine and speeds up timing.
  46. ^ Metzinger, Thomas (2009). The Ego Tunnel. Basic Books. pp. 117–118. ISBN 978-0-465-04567-9.
  47. ^ Wegner DM, Wheatley T (July 1999). «Apparent mental causation. Sources of the experience of will». The American Psychologist. 54 (7): 480–92. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.188.8271. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.54.7.480. PMID 10424155.
  48. ^ Metzinger, Thomas (2003). Being No One. p. 508.
  49. ^ Mandler (1980). «Recognizing: the judgement of prior occurrence». Psychological Review. 87 (3): 252–271. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.87.3.252. S2CID 2166238.
  50. ^ Ho JW, Poeta DL, Jacobson TK, Zolnik TA, Neske GT, Connors BW, Burwell RD (September 2015). «Bidirectional Modulation of Recognition Memory». The Journal of Neuroscience. 35 (39): 13323–35. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2278-15.2015. PMC 4588607. PMID 26424881.
  51. ^ Kinnavane L, Amin E, Olarte-Sánchez CM, Aggleton JP (November 2016). «Detecting and discriminating novel objects: The impact of perirhinal cortex disconnection on hippocampal activity patterns». Hippocampus. 26 (11): 1393–1413. doi:10.1002/hipo.22615. PMC 5082501. PMID 27398938.
  52. ^ Themes UF (29 March 2017). «Sensory Corpuscles». Abdominal Key. Retrieved 13 July 2018.
  53. ^ Wettlaufer, Alexandra K. (2003). In the mind’s eye : the visual impulse in Diderot, Baudelaire and Ruskin, pg. 257. Amsterdam: Rodopi. ISBN 978-90-420-1035-2.
  54. ^ The Secret Advantage Of Being Short Archived 21 May 2009 at the Wayback Machine by Robert Krulwich. All Things Considered, NPR. 18 May 2009.
  55. ^ Bedford, F. L. (2011). «The missing sensory modality: the immune system». Perception. 40 (10): 1265–1267. doi:10.1068/p7119. PMID 22308900. S2CID 9546850.
  56. ^ Kolb & Whishaw: Fundamentals of Human Neuropsychology (2003)
  57. ^ Farb N.; Daubenmier J.; Price C. J.; Gard T.; Kerr C.; Dunn B. D.; Mehling W. E. (2015). «Interoception, contemplative practice, and health». Frontiers in Psychology. 6: 763. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00763. PMC 4460802. PMID 26106345.
  58. ^ Atkinson, Rita L.; Atkinson, Richard C.; Smith, Edward E. (March 1990). Introduction to psychology. Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. pp. 177–183. ISBN 978-0-15-543689-3. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 24 March 2011.
  59. ^ a b Sonderegger, Theo (16 October 1998). Psychology. John Wiley and Sons. pp. 43–46. ISBN 978-0-8220-5327-9. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 24 March 2011.
  60. ^ a b c d Goldstein, E. Bruce (15 October 2009). «Constancy». In Goldstein, E. Bruce (ed.). Encyclopedia of Perception. Sage. pp. 309–313. ISBN 978-1-4129-4081-8. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 26 March 2011.
  61. ^ Roeckelein, Jon E. (2006). Elsevier’s dictionary of psychological theories. Elsevier. p. 126. ISBN 978-0-444-51750-0. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 24 March 2011.
  62. ^ Yantis, Steven (2001). Visual perception: essential readings. Psychology Press. p. 7. ISBN 978-0-86377-598-7. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 24 March 2011.
  63. ^ Gray, Peter O. (2006): Psychology, 5th ed., New York: Worth, p. 281. ISBN 978-0-7167-0617-5
  64. ^ Wolfe, Jeremy M.; Kluender, Keith R.; Levi, Dennis M.; Bartoshuk, Linda M.; Herz, Rachel S.; Klatzky, Roberta L.; Lederman, Susan J. (2008). «Gestalt Grouping Principles». Sensation and Perception (2nd ed.). Sinauer Associates. pp. 78, 80. ISBN 978-0-87893-938-1. Archived from the original on 23 July 2011.
  65. ^ Goldstein (2009). pp. 105–107
  66. ^ Banerjee, J. C. (1994). «Gestalt Theory of Perception». Encyclopaedic Dictionary of Psychological Terms. M.D. Publications Pvt. Ltd. pp. 107–108. ISBN 978-81-85880-28-0.
  67. ^ Weiten, Wayne (1998). Psychology: themes and variations (4th ed.). Brooks/Cole Pub. Co. p. 144. ISBN 978-0-534-34014-8.
  68. ^ Corsini, Raymond J. (2002). The dictionary of psychology. Psychology Press. p. 219. ISBN 978-1-58391-328-4. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 24 March 2011.
  69. ^ a b Kushner, Laura H. (2008). Contrast in judgments of mental health. p. 1. ISBN 978-0-549-91314-6. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 24 March 2011.
  70. ^ a b Plous, Scott (1993). The psychology of judgment and decision making. McGraw-Hill. pp. 38–41. ISBN 978-0-07-050477-6. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 24 March 2011.
  71. ^ Moskowitz, Gordon B. (2005). Social cognition: understanding self and others. Guilford Press. p. 421. ISBN 978-1-59385-085-2. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 24 March 2011.
  72. ^ Popper, Arthur N. (30 November 2010). Music Perception. Springer. p. 150. ISBN 978-1-4419-6113-6. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 24 March 2011.
  73. ^ Biederlack, J.; Castelo-Branco, M.; Neuenschwander, S.; Wheeler, D.W.; Singer, W.; Nikolić, D. (2006). «Brightness induction: Rate enhancement and neuronal synchronization as complementary codes». Neuron. 52 (6): 1073–1083. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.11.012. PMID 17178409. S2CID 16732916.
  74. ^ Stone, James V. (2012): «Vision and Brain: How we perceive the world», Cambridge, MIT Press, pp. 155-178.
  75. ^ Gibson, James J. (2002): «A Theory of Direct Visual Perception». In: Alva Noë/Evan Thompson (Eds.), Vision and Mind. Selected Readings in the Philosophy of Perception, Cambridge, MIT Press, pp. 77–89.
  76. ^ Sokolowski, Robert (2008). Phenomenology of the Human Person. New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 199–200. ISBN 978-0521717663. Archived from the original on 25 September 2015.
  77. ^ Richards, Robert J. (December 1976). «James Gibson’s Passive Theory of Perception: A Rejection of the Doctrine of Specific Nerve Energies» (PDF). Philosophy and Phenomenological Research. 37 (2): 218–233. doi:10.2307/2107193. JSTOR 2107193. Archived (PDF) from the original on 13 June 2013.
  78. ^ Consciousness in Action, S. L. Hurley, illustrated, Harvard University Press, 2002, 0674007964, pp. 430–432.
  79. ^ Glasersfeld, Ernst von (1995), Radical Constructivism: A Way of Knowing and Learning, London: RoutledgeFalmer; Poerksen, Bernhard (ed.) (2004), The Certainty of Uncertainty: Dialogues Introducing Constructivism, Exeter: Imprint Academic; Wright. Edmond (2005). Narrative, Perception, Language, and Faith, Basingstoke: Palgrave Macmillan.
  80. ^ a b c d e f g h i j k l m n o p q Gaulin, Steven J. C. and Donald H. McBurney. Evolutionary Psychology. Prentice Hall. 2003. ISBN 978-0-13-111529-3, Chapter 4, pp. 81–101.
  81. ^ Dewey J (1896). «The reflex arc concept in psychology» (PDF). Psychological Review. 3 (4): 359–370. doi:10.1037/h0070405. S2CID 14028152. Archived from the original (PDF) on 6 November 2018.
  82. ^ Friston, K. (2010) The free-energy principle: a unified brain theory? nature reviews neuroscience 11:127-38
  83. ^ Tishby, N. and D. Polani, Information theory of decisions and actions, in Perception-Action Cycle. 2011, Springer. p. 601-636.
  84. ^ Ahissar E., Assa E. (2016). «Perception as a closed-loop convergence process». eLife. 5: e12830. doi:10.7554/eLife.12830. PMC 4913359. PMID 27159238.
  85. ^ a b c d e Goldstein, E. Bruce (2015). Cognitive Psychology: Connecting Mind, Research, and Everyday Experience, 4th Edition. Stamford, CT: Cengage Learning. pp. 109–112. ISBN 978-1-285-76388-0.
  86. ^ Treisman, Anne; Gelade, Garry (1980). «A Feature-Integration Theory of Attention» (PDF). Cognitive Psychology. 12 (1): 97–136. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5. PMID 7351125. S2CID 353246. Archived from the original (PDF) on 5 September 2008 – via Science Direct.
  87. ^ Goldstein, E. Bruce (2010). Sensation and Perception (8th ed.). Belmont, CA: Cengage Learning. pp. 144–146. ISBN 978-0-495-60149-4.
  88. ^ a b Treisman, Anne; Schmidt, Hilary (1982). «Illusory Conjunctions in the Perception of Objects». Cognitive Psychology. 14 (1): 107–141. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(82)90006-8. PMID 7053925. S2CID 11201516 – via Science Direct.
  89. ^ a b Treisman, Anne (1977). «Focused Attention in The Perception and Retrieval of Multidimensional Stimuli». Cognitive Psychology. 14 (1): 107–141. doi:10.1016/0010-0285(82)90006-8. PMID 7053925. S2CID 11201516 – via Science Direct.
  90. ^ Sumner, Meghan. The Effect of Experience on the Perception and Representation of Dialect Variants (PDF). Journal of Memory and Language. Elsevier Inc., 2009. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2 February 2016. Retrieved 3 June 2015.
  91. ^ Fiori, Francesca; David, Nicole; Aglioti, Salvatore Maria (2014). «Processing of proprioceptive and vestibular body signals and self-transcendence in Ashtanga yoga practitioners». Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 8: 734. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2014.00734. PMC 4166896. PMID 25278866.
  92. ^ Snyder, Joel (31 October 2015). «How previous experience shapes perception in different sensory modalities». Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 9: 594. doi:10.3389/fnhum.2015.00594. PMC 4628108. PMID 26582982.
  93. ^ a b Weiten, Wayne (17 December 2008). Psychology: Themes and Variations. Cengage Learning. p. 193. ISBN 978-0-495-60197-5. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 24 March 2011.
  94. ^ a b Coon, Dennis; Mitterer, John O. (29 December 2008). Introduction to Psychology: Gateways to Mind and Behavior. Cengage Learning. pp. 171–172. ISBN 978-0-495-59911-1. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 24 March 2011.
  95. ^ a b c d Hardy, Malcolm; Heyes, Steve (2 December 1999). Beginning Psychology. Oxford University Press. pp. 24–27. ISBN 978-0-19-832821-6. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 24 March 2011.
  96. ^ Block, J. R.; Yuker, Harold E. (1 October 2002). Can You Believe Your Eyes?: Over 250 Illusions and Other Visual Oddities. Robson. pp. 173–174. ISBN 978-1-86105-586-6. Archived from the original on 9 November 2011. Retrieved 24 March 2011.
  97. ^ «On the Perception of Incongruity: A Paradigm» by Jerome S. Bruner and Leo Postman. Journal of Personality, 18, pp. 206-223. 1949. Yorku.ca Archived 15 February 2006 at the Wayback Machine
  98. ^ «Predictive Coding». Archived from the original on 5 December 2013. Retrieved 24 February 2011.
  99. ^ O’Regan, J. Kevin (1992). «Solving the «real» mysteries of visual perception: The world as an outside memory». Canadian Journal of Psychology/Revue Canadienne de Psychologie. 46 (3): 461–488. doi:10.1037/h0084327. ISSN 0008-4255. PMID 1486554.
  100. ^ O’Regan, J. Kevin; Noë, Alva (2001). «A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness». Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 24 (5): 939–973. doi:10.1017/S0140525X01000115. ISSN 0140-525X. PMID 12239892.

Sources[edit]

Bibliography[edit]

  • Arnheim, R. (1969). Visual Thinking. Berkeley: University of California Press. ISBN 978-0-520-24226-5.
  • Flanagan, J. R., & Lederman, S. J. (2001). «‘Neurobiology: Feeling bumps and holes. News and Views», Nature, 412(6845):389–91. (PDF)
  • Gibson, J. J. (1966). The Senses Considered as Perceptual Systems, Houghton Mifflin.
  • Gibson, J. J. (1987). The Ecological Approach to Visual Perception. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 0-89859-959-8
  • Robles-De-La-Torre, G. (2006). «The Importance of the Sense of Touch in Virtual and Real Environments». IEEE MultiMedia,13(3), Special issue on Haptic User Interfaces for Multimedia Systems, pp. 24–30. (PDF)

External links[edit]

  • Theories of Perception Several different aspects on perception
  • Richard L Gregory Theories of Richard. L. Gregory.
  • Comprehensive set of optical illusions, presented by Michael Bach.
  • Optical Illusions Examples of well-known optical illusions.
  • The Epistemology of Perception Article in the Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy
  • Cognitive Penetrability of Perception and Epistemic Justification Article in the Internet Encyclopedia of Philosophy

3

a

: awareness of the elements of environment through physical sensation

b

: physical sensation interpreted in the light of experience

4

b

: a capacity for comprehension

Synonyms

Choose the Right Synonym for perception



the discernment to know true friends

discrimination stresses the power to distinguish and select what is true or appropriate or excellent.



the discrimination that develops through listening to a lot of great music

perception implies quick and often sympathetic discernment (as of shades of feeling).



a novelist of keen perception into human motives

penetration implies a searching mind that goes beyond what is obvious or superficial.



lacks the penetration to see the scorn beneath their friendly smiles

insight suggests depth of discernment coupled with understanding sympathy.



a documentary providing insight into the plight of the homeless

acumen implies characteristic penetration combined with keen practical judgment.



a director of reliable box-office acumen

Example Sentences

It is ironic that the impact of smoking on nonsmokers, rather than on smokers themselves, is what finally transformed the regulation and cultural perception of the cigarette.


Allan M. Brandt, The Cigarette Century, 2007


Some drugs cause blurred vision and changes in color perception, or increased tears.


Sallie Tisdale, Harper’s, June 2007


The urge of these acolytes is not dramatic but mercantile—to traduce all personal history, to subvert all perception or insight, into gain, or the hope of gain.


David Mamet, Jafsie and John Henry Essays, 1999


Everything is research for the sake of erudition. No one is taught to value himself for nice perception and cultivated taste.


Robert Frost, letter, 2 Jan. 1915



a writer of considerable perception, she remembers how it feels to be confused and insecure



a growing perception of the enormity of the problem

See More

Recent Examples on the Web

And how will perceptions and personal experiences affect public support for strategies to address the changing climate and ensure future Californians have water to drink?


Ryan Fonseca, Los Angeles Times, 10 Apr. 2023





One dramatic and tragic exception drives our perception — mass shootings.


Ron Avi Astor, CNN, 3 Apr. 2023





Why are people cutting back? Reasons for the conservative mindset are economic uncertainty and also value for money, with the perception being that resorts and airlines are continuing to inflate prices without any reflection in service.


Eleanor Pringle, Fortune, 3 Apr. 2023





The perception of a doctor-law enforcement alliance is unsettling for communities that have negative experiences with police.


Sally Mahmoud-werthmann, STAT, 3 Apr. 2023





Its feelings will be even more powerful, its perceptions greater, and its happiness increased.


Ingrid Vasquez, Peoplemag, 31 Mar. 2023





Also known as Ramses the Great and Ozymandias, this New Kingdom pharaoh embodies many modern perceptions of ancient Egypt: militarism, diplomacy, advanced infrastructure, vast wealth.


Christopher Parker, Smithsonian Magazine, 31 Mar. 2023





Its feelings will be even more powerful, its perceptions greater, and its happiness increased.


Glenn Rowley, Billboard, 31 Mar. 2023





As a photographer and storyteller, Myesha Evon Gardner examines truth by documenting and redefining themes of legacy, labor, the perception of beauty, and familial love in underrepresented communities.


Condé Nast, Vogue, 29 Mar. 2023



See More

These examples are programmatically compiled from various online sources to illustrate current usage of the word ‘perception.’ Any opinions expressed in the examples do not represent those of Merriam-Webster or its editors. Send us feedback about these examples.

Word History

Etymology

Latin perception-, perceptio act of perceiving, from percipere — see perceive

First Known Use

14th century, in the meaning defined at sense 2

Time Traveler

The first known use of perception was
in the 14th century

Dictionary Entries Near perception

Cite this Entry

“Perception.” Merriam-Webster.com Dictionary, Merriam-Webster, https://www.merriam-webster.com/dictionary/perception. Accessed 14 Apr. 2023.

Share

More from Merriam-Webster on perception

Last Updated:
12 Apr 2023
— Updated example sentences

Subscribe to America’s largest dictionary and get thousands more definitions and advanced search—ad free!

Merriam-Webster unabridged

восприятие, ощущение, понимание, осознание, перцепция, сбор, завладение, получение

существительное

- восприятие; ощущение

organs of perception — органы восприятия
stereo perception — стереоскопическое восприятие
keen perception — острое восприятие
colour perception — спец. восприятие /ощущение/ цвета

- способность восприятия
- осознание, понимание

clear perception — ясное понимание
perception of necessity — осознание необходимости
perception of beauty — понимание красоты

- представление (о чём-л.)

lyric perceptions of friendship and love — сентиментальные представления о дружбе и любви

- проницательность, способность постижения (чего-л.)

perception into the heart of human activity — ясное представление о сути человеческой деятельности
a film bristling with sharp perceptions — фильм, полный тонких наблюдений
well-intentioned people lacking in keen perception — люди с благими намерениями, но лишённые проницательности

- филос. перцепция
- юр. сбор

the perception of profits — сбор доходов

Мои примеры

Словосочетания

children’s perceptions of the world — детское восприятие мира  
a man admired for the depth of his perception — мужчина, вызывавший восхищение глубиной своего восприятия  
a person’s subjective perception of the world — субъективное восприятие мира человеком  
depth perception — ощущение глубины  
human perception — человеческое восприятие  
speech perception — восприятие речи  
aural perception — слуховое восприятие  
signal perception — восприятие сигнала  
visual perception threshold — порог видимости  
kinaesthetic perception — кинестетическое восприятие  
binocular perception — бинокулярное восприятие  
blaze perception — восприятие блеска  

Примеры с переводом

Events confirmed our perception that she had been treated unfairly.

События подтвердили наше ощущение, что с ней обращались несправедливо.

Luther had a new perception of the Bible.

Лютер воспринял библию по-новому.

Ross shows unusual perception for a boy of his age.

Росс демонстрирует редкую способность к восприятию для мальчика своего возраста.

Some drugs cause blurred vision and changes in color perception, or increased tears.

Некоторые препараты вызывают ухудшение зрения, изменения в восприятии цвета, или повышенную слезливость.

A writer of considerable perception, she remembers how it feels to be confused and insecure.

Будучи очень восприимчивой, эта писательница помнит, каково это — чувствовать себя растерянным и неуверенным в себе.

Возможные однокоренные слова

percept  — объект или результат перцепции
perceptional  — перцепционный
misperception  — неправильное восприятие
imperception  — отсутствие понимания, нарушение или отсутствие восприятия, нарушение восприятия

Формы слова

noun
ед. ч.(singular): perception
мн. ч.(plural): perceptions

Educalingo cookies are used to personalize ads and get web traffic statistics. We also share information about the use of the site with our social media, advertising and analytics partners.

Download the app
educalingo

If the doors of perception were cleansed everything would appear to man as it is, infinite.

William Blake

section

ETYMOLOGY OF THE WORD PERCEPTION

From Latin perceptiō comprehension.

info

Etymology is the study of the origin of words and their changes in structure and significance.

facebooktwitterpinterestwhatsapp

section

PRONUNCIATION OF PERCEPTION

facebooktwitterpinterestwhatsapp

GRAMMATICAL CATEGORY OF PERCEPTION

Perception is a noun.

A noun is a type of word the meaning of which determines reality. Nouns provide the names for all things: people, objects, sensations, feelings, etc.

WHAT DOES PERCEPTION MEAN IN ENGLISH?

Perception

Perception is the organization, identification, and interpretation of sensory information in order to represent and understand the environment. All perception involves signals in the nervous system, which in turn result from physical or chemical stimulation of the sense organs. For example, vision involves light striking the retina of the eye, smell is mediated by odor molecules, and hearing involves pressure waves. Perception is not the passive receipt of these signals, but is shaped by learning, memory, expectation, and attention. Perception involves these «top-down» effects as well as the «bottom-up» process of processing sensory input. The «bottom-up» processing transforms low-level information to higher-level information. The «top-down» processing refers to a person’s concept and expectations, and selective mechanisms that influence perception. Perception depends on complex functions of the nervous system, but subjectively seems mostly effortless because this processing happens outside conscious awareness. Since the rise of experimental psychology in the 19th Century, psychology’s understanding of perception has progressed by combining a variety of techniques.


Definition of perception in the English dictionary

The first definition of perception in the dictionary is the act or the effect of perceiving. Other definition of perception is insight or intuition gained by perceiving. Perception is also the ability or capacity to perceive.

WORDS THAT RHYME WITH PERCEPTION

Synonyms and antonyms of perception in the English dictionary of synonyms

SYNONYMS OF «PERCEPTION»

The following words have a similar or identical meaning as «perception» and belong to the same grammatical category.

Translation of «perception» into 25 languages

online translator

TRANSLATION OF PERCEPTION

Find out the translation of perception to 25 languages with our English multilingual translator.

The translations of perception from English to other languages presented in this section have been obtained through automatic statistical translation; where the essential translation unit is the word «perception» in English.

Translator English — Chinese


理解

1,325 millions of speakers

Translator English — Spanish


percepción

570 millions of speakers

Translator English — Hindi


धारणा

380 millions of speakers

Translator English — Arabic


التصور

280 millions of speakers

Translator English — Russian


восприятие

278 millions of speakers

Translator English — Portuguese


percepção

270 millions of speakers

Translator English — Bengali


উপলব্ধি

260 millions of speakers

Translator English — French


perception

220 millions of speakers

Translator English — Malay


Persepsi

190 millions of speakers

Translator English — German


Wahrnehmung

180 millions of speakers

Translator English — Japanese


認識

130 millions of speakers

Translator English — Korean


인식

85 millions of speakers

Translator English — Javanese


Pamikir

85 millions of speakers

Translator English — Vietnamese


nhận thức

80 millions of speakers

Translator English — Tamil


கருத்து

75 millions of speakers

Translator English — Marathi


समज

75 millions of speakers

Translator English — Turkish


algı

70 millions of speakers

Translator English — Italian


percezione

65 millions of speakers

Translator English — Polish


percepcja

50 millions of speakers

Translator English — Ukrainian


сприйняття

40 millions of speakers

Translator English — Romanian


Percepția

30 millions of speakers

Translator English — Greek


αντίληψη

15 millions of speakers

Translator English — Afrikaans


persepsie

14 millions of speakers

Translator English — Swedish


uppfattning

10 millions of speakers

Translator English — Norwegian


persepsjon

5 millions of speakers

Trends of use of perception

TENDENCIES OF USE OF THE TERM «PERCEPTION»

The term «perception» is very widely used and occupies the 10.333 position in our list of most widely used terms in the English dictionary.

Trends

FREQUENCY

Very widely used

The map shown above gives the frequency of use of the term «perception» in the different countries.

Principal search tendencies and common uses of perception

List of principal searches undertaken by users to access our English online dictionary and most widely used expressions with the word «perception».

FREQUENCY OF USE OF THE TERM «PERCEPTION» OVER TIME

The graph expresses the annual evolution of the frequency of use of the word «perception» during the past 500 years. Its implementation is based on analysing how often the term «perception» appears in digitalised printed sources in English between the year 1500 and the present day.

Examples of use in the English literature, quotes and news about perception

10 QUOTES WITH «PERCEPTION»

Famous quotes and sentences with the word perception.

I’ve always believed that photography is a way to shape human perception.

I think that dwelling on other people’s perception of you is the road to complete madness, unfortunately. I try and resist that.

To perceive means to immobilize… we seize, in the act of perception, something which outruns perception itself.

Certainly almost everything we do and think is colored in some way by memes, but it is important to realize that not everything we experience is a meme. If I walk down the street and see a tree, the basic perception that’s going on is not memetic.

If the doors of perception were cleansed everything would appear to man as it is, infinite.

Studies have shown that 90% of error in thinking is due to error in perception. If you can change your perception, you can change your emotion and this can lead to new ideas.

There is still a perception that the way women are isn’t necessarily what you need for the powerhouse start-ups.

I can go into the wilderness and not see anyone for days and experience a kind of space that hasn’t changed for tens of thousands of years. Having that experience was necessary to my perception of how photography can look at the changes humanity has brought about in the landscape. My work does become a kind of lament.

Intuition comes very close to clairvoyance; it appears to be the extrasensory perception of reality.

10 ENGLISH BOOKS RELATING TO «PERCEPTION»

Discover the use of perception in the following bibliographical selection. Books relating to perception and brief extracts from same to provide context of its use in English literature.

1

Perception: A Representative Theory

What is the nature of, and what is the relationship between, external objects and our visual perceptual experience of them? In this book, Frank Jackson defends the answers provided by the traditional Representative theory of perception.

2

Perception: Theory, Development and Organisation

Introduces students to the basic biological and psychological processes and their development. It discusses pattern recognition, culture and attention and includes a brief discussion of artificial intelligence.

Paul Rookes, Jane Willson, 2005

3

Art and Visual Perception: A Psychology of the Creative Eye

Gestalt theory and the psychology of visual perception form the basis for an analysis of art and its basic elements

4

The Primacy of Perception: And Other Essays on …

Taken together, the studies in this volume provide a systematic introduction to the major themes of Merleau-Ponty’s philosophy. This book consists of Northwestern University Studies in Phenomenology and Existential Philosophy.

Maurice Merleau-Ponty, James M. Edie, 1964

5

Sensation and Perception

A key strength of this book has always been the ability to illustrate concepts through examples and visuals.

An argument that perception is something we do, not something that happens to us: not a process in the brain, but a skillful bodily activity.

7

Phenomenology of Perception

This new translation, the first for over fifty years, makes this classic work of philosophy available to a new generation of readers.

Maurice Merleau-Ponty, 2013

8

Suspensions of Perception: Attention, Spectacle, and Modern …

Suspensions of Perception is a major historical study of humanattention and its volatile role in modern Western culture.

This collection will interest anyone who seeks a deeper understanding of the complex problems of perception. It will also appeal to scholars in search of a compact collection of some of the best expository writing in this area.

10

Skepticism and the Veil of Perception

This book develops and defends a version of direct realism: the thesis that perception gives us direct awareness, and non-inferential knowledge, of the external world.

10 NEWS ITEMS WHICH INCLUDE THE TERM «PERCEPTION»

Find out what the national and international press are talking about and how the term perception is used in the context of the following news items.

SNAMM ’15 — Fret-King Jerry Donahue Signature, Esprit III …

SNAMM ’15 — Fret-King Jerry Donahue Signature, Esprit III, & Perception 5-String Bass Demos. Shawn Hammond. July 26, 2015. A A. Fret King-FEAT … «Premier Guitar, Jul 15»

Perception of security eroded by mass shootings

And there’s really nowhere we’re safe out in the public – or at least that is the perception,” said Adam Lankford, a criminal justice professor at … «Press Herald, Jul 15»

Biogen Changed the Perception of This Market

Something changed in the market on Friday. It began on Thursday, but Friday solidified it. For months, we’ve had this divergence between the … «TheStreet.com, Jul 15»

SunEdison’s head rubbishes payment risk perception in TN

Pashupathy Gopalan, who heads the Asia-Pacific and Sub-Sahara African operations of the US renewable energy major, SunEdison, says he … «Hindu Business Line, Jul 15»

Michael Stone – CIF’s new President highlights Apprentices and …

“In order to improve the perception of the construction industry, standards within the sector must be improved. Positive initiatives that go some … «Irish Building Magazine, Jul 15»

Chinese companies surge in Futurebrand perception study of …

Chinese companies surge in Futurebrand perception study of world’s biggest brands. Google has emerged on top of a study by Futurebrand of … «Mumbrella Asia, Jul 15»

Perception of scams

What made me very proud was that all countries our PM visited are way below us in the “corruption perception index.” This index is prepared … «The Statesman, Jul 15»

DVD review: Eskil Vogt’s Blind a study of isolation and perception

Norwegian writer-director Eskil Vogt’s stunning debut feature will do little to cast off the cliche that Scandinavian films are all to do with icy cold … «South China Morning Post, Jul 15»

Top 100 Brands Based On Consumer Perception: Google No. 1 Two …

For the second year in a row, Google tops FutureBrand’s list of top 100 companies ranked by consumer brand perception. Apple took the No. «Marketing Land, Jul 15»

Perception Finally Meets Reality for Robert Griffin III and Washington …

After three years of hyping the former Offensive Rookie of the Year to the hilt, the NFL fraternity’s perception of Griffin now matches the reality. «Bleacher Report, Jul 15»

REFERENCE

« EDUCALINGO. Perception [online]. Available <https://educalingo.com/en/dic-en/perception>. Apr 2023 ».

Download the educalingo app


Discover all that is hidden in the words on educalingo

Other forms: perceptions

Each generation has a different perception — view, idea or understanding — of what is cool. You wouldn’t want to walk around in the paisley patterns of the 1960s or the big hair of the 1980s today!

Based on the Latin root cipere, «to grasp,» perception refers to the way you take in the world through your senses. Have you ever thought a stair was bigger than it really was so your step was too heavy? Your depth perception was off. The noun also means the opinions and beliefs you’ve formed about something. Your perception of your room after you’ve cleaned it is different than your parents’ — you see clean and they probably don’t.

Definitions of perception

  1. noun

    the process of perceiving

    see moresee less

    types:

    show 32 types…
    hide 32 types…
    constancy, perceptual constancy

    (psychology) the tendency for perceived objects to give rise to very similar perceptual experiences in spite of wide variations in the conditions of observation

    detection, sensing

    the perception that something has occurred or some state exists

    beholding, seeing, visual perception

    perception by means of the eyes

    auditory perception, sound perception

    the perception of sound as a meaningful phenomenon

    aesthesis, esthesis, sensation, sense datum, sense experience, sense impression

    an unelaborated elementary awareness of stimulation

    somaesthesia, somatesthesia, somatic sensation, somesthesia

    the perception of tactual or proprioceptive or gut sensations

    feeling, tactile sensation, tactual sensation, touch, touch sensation

    the sensation produced by pressure receptors in the skin

    brightness constancy

    the tendency for a visual object to be perceived as having the same brightness under widely different conditions of illumination

    color constancy, colour constancy

    the tendency for a color to look the same under widely different viewing conditions

    shape constancy

    the tendency to perceive the shape of a rigid object as constant despite differences in the viewing angle (and consequent differences in the shape of the pattern projected on the retina of the eye)

    size constancy

    the tendency to perceive the veridical size of a familiar object despite differences in their distance (and consequent differences in the size of the pattern projected on the retina of the eye)

    contrast

    the perceptual effect of the juxtaposition of very different colors

    face recognition

    the visual perception of familiar faces

    object recognition

    the visual perception of familiar objects

    visual space

    the visual perception of space

    speech perception

    the auditory perception (and comprehension) of speech

    musical perception

    the auditory perception of musical sounds

    limen, threshold

    the smallest detectable sensation

    masking

    the blocking of one sensation resulting from the presence of another sensation

    vision, visual sensation

    the perceptual experience of seeing

    odor, odour, olfactory perception, olfactory sensation, smell

    the sensation that results when olfactory receptors in the nose are stimulated by particular chemicals in gaseous form

    gustatory perception, gustatory sensation, taste, taste perception, taste sensation

    the sensation that results when taste buds in the tongue and throat convey information about the chemical composition of a soluble stimulus

    auditory sensation, sound

    the subjective sensation of hearing something

    synaesthesia, synesthesia

    a sensation that normally occurs in one sense modality occurs when another modality is stimulated

    feeling

    a physical sensation that you experience

    prickling, tingle, tingling

    a somatic sensation as from many tiny prickles

    creepiness

    an uneasy sensation as of insects creeping on your skin

    cutaneous sensation, haptic sensation, skin sensation

    a sensation localized on the skin

    pressure, pressure sensation

    the somatic sensation that results from applying force to an area of skin

    pain, pain sensation, painful sensation

    a somatic sensation of acute discomfort

    temperature

    the somatic sensation of cold or heat

    fusion, optical fusion

    the combining of images from the two eyes to form a single visual percept

    type of:

    basic cognitive process

    cognitive processes involved in obtaining and storing knowledge

  2. noun

    becoming aware of something via the senses

    synonyms:

    sensing

    see moresee less

    types:

    show 20 types…
    hide 20 types…
    look, looking, looking at

    the act of directing the eyes toward something and perceiving it visually

    hearing, listening

    the act of hearing attentively

    lipreading

    perceiving what a person is saying by observing the movements of the lips

    taste, tasting

    a kind of sensing; distinguishing substances by means of the taste buds

    smell, smelling

    the act of perceiving the odor of something

    auscultation

    listening to sounds within the body (usually with a stethoscope)

    coup d’oeil, gander, glance, glimpse

    a quick look

    scrutiny

    a prolonged intense look

    peek, peep

    a secret look

    squint

    the act of squinting; looking with the eyes partly closed

    stare

    a fixed look with eyes open wide

    evil eye

    a look that is believed to have the power of inflicting harm

    rubber-necking, sightseeing

    going about to look at places of interest

    observance, observation, watching

    the act of observing; taking a patient look

    lookout, outlook

    the act of looking out

    sight, survey, view

    the act of looking or seeing or observing

    dekko

    British slang for a look

    rehearing, relistening

    the act of hearing again

    sniff, snuff

    sensing an odor by inhaling through the nose

    palate

    the ability to taste, judge, and appreciate food

    type of:

    sensory activity

    activity intended to achieve a particular sensory result

  3. noun

    a way of conceiving something

    “Luther had a new
    perception of the Bible”

  4. noun

    knowledge gained by perceiving

    “a man admired for the depth of his
    perception

  5. noun

    the representation of what is perceived; basic component in the formation of a concept

    synonyms:

    percept, perceptual experience

    see moresee less

    types:

    show 8 types…
    hide 8 types…
    figure

    a unitary percept having structure and coherence that is the object of attention and that stands out against a ground

    ground

    a relatively homogeneous percept extending back of the figure on which attention is focused

    visual image, visual percept

    a percept that arises from the eyes; an image in the visual system

    eye candy

    visual images that are pleasing to see but are intellectually undemanding

    field, field of view

    the area that is visible (as through an optical instrument)

    sight

    an instance of visual perception

    aspect, panorama, prospect, scene, view, vista

    the visual percept of a region

    field of regard, field of vision, visual field

    all of the points of the physical environment that can be perceived by a stable eye at a given moment

    type of:

    internal representation, mental representation, representation

    a presentation to the mind in the form of an idea or image

DISCLAIMER: These example sentences appear in various news sources and books to reflect the usage of the word ‘perception’.
Views expressed in the examples do not represent the opinion of Vocabulary.com or its editors.
Send us feedback

EDITOR’S CHOICE

Look up perception for the last time

Close your vocabulary gaps with personalized learning that focuses on teaching the
words you need to know.

VocabTrainer - Vocabulary.com's Vocabulary Trainer

Sign up now (it’s free!)

Whether you’re a teacher or a learner, Vocabulary.com can put you or your class on the path to systematic vocabulary improvement.

Get started

perception
pəˈsepʃən сущ.
1) восприятие, ощущение а) процесс получения сенсорных ощущений от чего-л. Events confirmed our perception that she had been treated unfairly. ≈ События подтвердили наше ощущение, что с ней обращались несправедливо. keen perception color perception depth perception extrasensory perception visual perception Syn : sensation, feeling, observation б) образ, полученный в результате этого процесса Syn : concept
2) а) осмысление, осознание, понимание clear perception ≈ ясное понимание Syn : acumen, appreciation б) проницательность, острое мышление Syn : discernment
3) филос. перцепция
4) юр. сбор
восприятие;
ощущение — organs of * органы восприятия — stereo * стереоскопическое восприятие — keen * острое восприятие — colour * (специальное) восприятие /ощущение/ цвета способность восприятия осознание, понимание — clear * ясное понимание — * of necessity осознание необходимости — * of beauty понимание красоты представление (о чем-л.) — lyric *s of friendship and love сентиментальные представления о дружбе и любви проницательность, способность постижения (чего-л.) — * into the heart of human activity ясное представление о сути человеческой деятельности — a film bristling with sharp *s фильм, полный тонких наблюдений — well-intentioned people lacking in keen * люди с благими намерениями, но лишенные проницательности (философское) перцепция (юридическое) сбор — the * of profits сбор доходов
artifical ~ вчт. распознавание образов
customer ~ понимание запросов клиента
perception восприятие, ощущение ~ восприятие ~ осознание, понимание ~ осознание ~ ощущение ~ филос. перцепция ~ понимание ~ проницательность ~ юр. сбор ~ сбор ~ способность постижения
tactile ~ восприятие тактильной информации
visual ~ восприятие зрительной информации

Большой англо-русский и русско-английский словарь.
2001.

Полезное

Смотреть что такое «perception» в других словарях:

  • PERCEPTION — Malgré l’intérêt incessant qu’a suscité l’étude de la perception tout au long de l’histoire de la philosophie occidentale et malgré l’énorme contribution, sur ce sujet, de la psychologie depuis l’époque où celle ci a tenté de se définir comme… …   Encyclopédie Universelle

  • Perception — Per*cep tion, n. [L. perceptio: cf. F. perception. See {Perceive}.] 1. The act of perceiving; cognizance by the senses or intellect; apperhension by the bodily organs, or by the mind, of what is presented to them; discernment; apperhension;… …   The Collaborative International Dictionary of English

  • perception — Perception. s. f. Recepte, recouvrement en matiere de deniers, fruits, revenus &c. Il est commis à la perception d un tel droit. il est obligé de rendre compte du revenu de cet heritage aprés la perception des fruits. Perception. En matiere de… …   Dictionnaire de l’Académie française

  • perception — per‧cep‧tion [pəˈsepʆn ǁ pər ] noun [countable] the way that people feel about a company, product, market etc and what they think it is like: • There is a perception among investors that an economic recovery is beginning. • National stereotypes… …   Financial and business terms

  • Perception — steht für: Perzeption, Gesamtheit der Vorgänge des Wahrnehmens bezeichnet. Perception (Film), US amerikanisches Filmdrama aus dem Jahr 2005 Perception Kayaks, einen Kanuhersteller, siehe Confluence Watersports #Perception …   Deutsch Wikipedia

  • perception — late 15c., receiving, collection, from L. perceptionem (nom. perceptio) perception, apprehension, a taking, from percipere perceive (see PERCEIVE (Cf. perceive)). First used in the more literal sense of the Latin word; in secondary sense, the… …   Etymology dictionary

  • perception — I noun ability to make distinctions, acuity, acumen, acuteness, apperception, appraisal, appreciation, apprehension, ascertainment, assessment, astuteness, attention, awareness, clear sight, cleverness, cognition, cognizance, comprehension,… …   Law dictionary

  • perception — penetration, insight, acumen, *discernment, discrimination Analogous words: appreciation, comprehension, understanding (see corresponding verbs at UNDERSTAND): sharpness, keenness, acuteness (see corresponding adjectives at SHARP) …   New Dictionary of Synonyms

  • perception — [n] understanding, idea acumen, apprehending, apprehension, approach, attention, attitude, awareness, big idea*, brainchild*, brain wave*, conceit, concept, conception, consciousness, discernment, feeling, flash, grasp, image, impression, insight …   New thesaurus

  • perception — ► NOUN 1) the ability to see, hear, or become aware of something through the senses. 2) the process of perceiving. 3) a way of understanding or interpreting something. 4) intuitive understanding and insight. ORIGIN Latin, from percipere seize,… …   English terms dictionary

  • perception — [pər sep′shən] n. [L perceptio < pp. of percipere: see PERCEIVE] 1. a) the act of perceiving or the ability to perceive; mental grasp of objects, qualities, etc. by means of the senses; awareness; comprehension b) insight or intuition, or the… …   English World dictionary

Like this post? Please share to your friends:
  • Is pre law a word
  • Is perceptible a word
  • Is pre existing a word
  • Is percent one word
  • Is pre canned a word