Is decision making one word or two

Table of Contents

  1. Is decision making hyphenated?
  2. Is decision maker one word or two?
  3. Is decision making a word?
  4. Is re energize hyphenated?
  5. Is it reenergize or re energize?
  6. What does re educate mean?
  7. What is re energize mean?
  8. How do you’re energize yourself?
  9. Is imprudently a word?
  10. What is the definition of a volunteer?
  11. What word can replace arduous?
  12. Is Discourteously a word?
  13. Is ungracious a word?
  14. What is a word for not graceful?

Reader’s question: Should decision making be hyphenated in noun form i.e. decision-making? Answer: You can use either decision making or decision-making in noun form, but decision-making is becoming more common. Just be consistent. Everyone in the team had a role in the decision making.

Is decision making hyphenated?

decision making, decision taking, decision-making, decision-taking. Do not hyphenate decision making or decision taking when these expressions are used as nouns.

Is decision maker one word or two?

Is “decision making” one word or two? “Decision-making” is a compound noun and hyphenated, comprising a standard noun and a gerund (a noun derived from a verb form). Dictionaries show it spelled with a hyphen, as well “decision-maker,” but you will often see it in use on the Internet as two words without the hyphen.

Is decision making a word?

However spelt, “decision making” consists of a noun (“decision”) and a gerund (“making”). A gerund is the form that a verb takes when used as a noun.

Is re energize hyphenated?

Case in point: For years, the Associated Press Stylebook advised followers: “except for ‘cooperate’ and ‘coordinate,’ use a hyphen if a prefix ends in a vowel and the word that follows begins with the same vowel.” This came up most with the prefixes “pre” and “re”: pre-election, re-energize, and the like. No more.

Is it reenergize or re energize?

transitive verb. Give fresh vitality, enthusiasm, or impetus to. ‘The bailout money was supposed to be used to jump-start the economy by re-energizing lending.

What does re educate mean?

transitive verb. : to train again especially : to rehabilitate through education.

What is re energize mean?

transitive verb. : to energize (someone or something) again Memphis has worked as hard as any U.S. city to reenergize its downtown …— Mel White The whole family was immobilized when Teddie was, and reenergized when he gained health.—

How do you’re energize yourself?

Recharge yourself physically

  1. Take a warm bath. A warm bath can be relaxing.
  2. Use an exfoliating scrub. Exfoliating scrubs can help recharge your body by improving blood circulation.
  3. Change your diet. Your energy levels are greatly impacted by your diet.
  4. Stretch.
  5. Exercise.
  6. Aromatherapy.
  7. Get more sleep.
  8. Get regular rest.

Is imprudently a word?

adj. Unwise or indiscreet; not prudent.

What is the definition of a volunteer?

(Entry 1 of 3) 1 : a person who voluntarily undertakes or expresses a willingness to undertake a service: such as. a : one who enters into military service voluntarily. b(1) : one who renders a service or takes part in a transaction while having no legal concern or interest.

What word can replace arduous?

Synonyms & Antonyms of arduous

  • Augean,
  • backbreaking,
  • challenging,
  • demanding,
  • difficult,
  • effortful,
  • exacting,
  • formidable,

Is Discourteously a word?

adj. Exhibiting no courtesy; rude. dis·cour′te·ous·ly adv. dis·cour′te·ous·ness n.

Is ungracious a word?

adjective. discourteous; ill-mannered: ungracious behavior. unpleasant; disagreeable; unrewarding: an ungracious task. ungraceful; unpleasing.

What is a word for not graceful?

unglue, unglued, ungodly, ungotten, ungovernable, ungraceful, ungracious, ungrammatical, ungrateful, ungrudging, ungt.

Hyphen rules in the English language can be difficult, and there are plenty of words out there that sometimes need hyphens and sometimes don’t. The decision-making or decision making hyphen rule is no stranger to this, so we’ll discuss it today and how you can learn when to hyphenate it and when to not!

Both forms of the phrase are correct, though “decision making” without a hyphen is used mostly as a noun. “Decision-making” with a hyphen is used more commonly as an adjective. However, “decision-making” with the hyphen has slowly started to take over as the noun form, too. Though it’s not technically correct, it’s just more common to see in writing.

Is Decision Making Hyphenated AP Style?

So, now we get to the question is decision making hyphenated AP Style? AP style tells us that hyphens are considered “joiners” between two closely linked words. You only ever use a hyphen between two (or more) words when you want the reader to understand that they are closely related and therefore modifying a noun together rather than as two individual units. It’s one of those rules you have to get used to using, but it’s straightforward once you get the hang of it.

Should I Capitalize “Making” In The Word “Decision-Making”?

Now we get to the slightly more tricky question that is often asked alongside hyphenation questions. What happens when you’re trying to put the hyphenated word in a title? Do you capitalize both words, just one word, or neither of the words? Well, the answer is a little more complicated than a simple “yes” or “no.” We’re going to say it depends and explain what we mean.

Basically, there are three styles of the title you can use in writing. The first is more old-fashioned but features only capitalizing the first word and any proper nouns. In this case, neither “decision” nor “making” are capitalized (unless “decision” is the first word, then it’ll be the only one capitalized). The second style capitalizes all words except for articles and short prepositions. In this case, “decision” is capitalized, but “making” isn’t.

The final form is becoming more popular over time and involves capitalizing every word, whether proper nouns, articles, adjectives, or something else. In this case, both “decision” and “making” are capitalized together.

Examples Of When To Use “Decision Making”

Now let’s look at some examples of when to use decision making vs decision-making. We want to make sure we’re using it without the hyphen when we’re using it as a noun. It’s fairly easy to figure out when you’re doing that. The easiest way to tell is if the words come at the end of a clause or sentence. Though this isn’t the only way they can become a noun, it helps not having anything after them that might be modified.

  • We helped them in the decision making.
  • Everyone had a role in the decision making.
  • I spend more time decision making than anything else.
  • Would you please help me with the decision making?

In each of these cases, “decision making” is left unhyphenated and put at the end of a clause. No nouns are being modified, so “decision making” becomes the noun itself.

Examples Of When To Use “Decision-Making”

Let’s see how the hyphenation can change the word to “decision-making” then. In this case, we’ll see nouns come after each of the forms that are modified as we go.

  • We helped with the decision-making process.
  • The decision-making group is still going.
  • I’m in more of a decision-making role.
  • I’m not good at the decision-making part.

Alternatives To “Decision Making”

If you’re still struggling to understand the hyphenation rule, there is one final thing you can do. Look for alternatives for the word so that you don’t have to worry about the hyphens at all! We thought we’d recommend you some to use to help you out.

  • analyzing
  • administrative
  • supervisory
  • policymaking
  • managerial

Quiz – Decision Making Or Decision-Making?

And to finish up, let’s see what we’ve learned from the article, shall we? A quick quiz will do the trick!

  1. We weren’t there for the (A. decision making / B. decision-making) process.
  2. Why do we always miss the (A. decision making / B. decision-making)?
  3. Where is the (A. decision making / B. decision-making) group going to be?
  4. Everyone took part in the (A. decision making / B. decision-making) for this product.
  5. I’m no good at (A. decision making / B. decision-making).

Quiz Answers

  1. B
  2. A
  3. B
  4. A
  5. A

martin lassen dam grammarhow

Martin holds a Master’s degree in Finance and International Business. He has six years of experience in professional communication with clients, executives, and colleagues. Furthermore, he has teaching experience from Aarhus University. Martin has been featured as an expert in communication and teaching on Forbes and Shopify. Read more about Martin here.

  • #1

Hello,

I recently read the word «decisionmaking» in this report from the Cato Institute. They are professional organization composed of very bright individuals with a strong political bent. Regardless of their politics, I feel confident that they choose their words carefully so that others will regard them seriously. So I doubt this is a mistake.

However, my spellcheck does not like the word «decisionmaking». Nor, for that matter, does it like the word «spellcheck». For both, it suggests either hyphenation or separation. Interestingly, «decisionmaking» is decidedly less popular in google than is «decision making,» but «spellcheck» is actually more popular than «spell check».

So is «decisionmaking» completely unjustafiable? If so, does decision making make more sense than decision-making?

Is there some recognized process by which phrases like this are coalesced into a single word?

  • Matching Mole


    • #2

    I think the adoption and acceptability of fused phrases is fairly arbitrary, but there is a much stronger tendency in US English to do this than in British. I’d say that it is much more likely to happen with short words as long compounds may be difficult to read, and also look out of proportion in a text.

    It’s also possible that long compounds are more likely to appear in business, political or academic writing, where density, conciseness (and the creation of «buzz terms», which are perhaps more likely to be fused) is more the norm.

    Dictionaries tend to be conservative, however, and although newer fused terms will appear as alternatives, the hyphenated version will appear as the headword. As far as I can see, even «decision-maker» doesn’t seem to warrant its own entry in the main dictionaries (I can’t see it in either M-W Collegiate or the OED), so I don’t know their views on the compound.

    I’d tend to be conservative (and I tend to be British), but I think ultimately it’s a style choice as it is more a matter of punctuation than spelling.

    cuchuflete


    • #3

    » So is «decisionmaking» completely unjustafiable? If so, does decision making make more sense than decision-making?»

    Unjustafiable? I won’t go off-topic or ballistic about that.

    I can’t think of a good reason to fuse decision + making as a noun, but I won’t suffer too much if some people see a point in eliminating the space between the words. Where it may make sense to hyphenate is when the words form an adjectival phrase before a noun. This may avoid ambiguity.

    • #4

    The original sentence it was used in is:

    “In fact all of the rapid reformers developed into liberal democracies, whereas in many of the gradual reformers, such as Russia, small groups of super-wealthy oligarchs captured the state and dominated its economic decisionmaking.»

    • #5

    » So is «decisionmaking» completely unjustafiable? If so, does decision making make more sense than decision-making?»

    Unjustafiable? I won’t go off-topic or ballistic about that.

    I can’t think of a good reason to fuse decision + making as a noun, but I won’t suffer too much if some people see a point in eliminating the space between the words. Where it may make sense to hyphenate is when the words form an adjectival phrase before a noun. This may avoid ambiguity.

    As a professional lifetime decision maker, I have acquired a strong preference for decisionmaking. At first, it was mostly instinctive. On further reflection, it seemed there was a subconscious rationale. (1) It is that the mental process involved in making a decision feels very unified and singular.

    I can cite only a few respectable instances where I have seen it compounded. Still, I use it regularly.

    (2) The evolution to the compound version also feels justifiable, as a practical matter, because of the ambiguity and confusion experienced by those forced to choose between the hyphenated and 2-word versions.

    • #6

    English tends to be slower than some other languages to adopt compound words. As the author of two reasonably popular university textbooks on decision support systems, in which this phrase (in some form) is of necessity used often, I would not combine them. I find the compound word more awkward than two simple, direct ones that everyone understands. It smacks of business jargon to me. Despite having spent a few decades in high-tech, I hate business jargon.

    Added later: Both these books were edited by professionals at large, well-known publishing houses. No copy editor ever suggested making a single word out of «decision making» or «decision-making.»

    As for whether to hyphenate or not: I generally do, but I feel strongly about it only when it’s an adjective phrase as in «decision-making process.»

    Last edited: Sep 6, 2019

    • #7

    English tends to be slower than some other languages to adopt compound words. As the author of two reasonably popular university textbooks on decision support systems, in which this phrase (in some form) is of necessity used often, I would not combine them. I find the compound word more awkward than two simple, direct ones that everyone understands. It smacks of business jargon to me. Despite having spent a few decades in high-tech, I hate business jargon.

    As for whether to hyphenate or not: I generally do, but I feel strongly about it only when it’s an adjective phrase as in «decision-making process.»

    I appreciate the thoughtful reply. I don’t fully grasp the business jargon objection. In any event, to me, decisionmaking has a synergistic power that is blunted by the word-separation version whether it be by space or by hyphen. The heightened interest in political decisionmaking leads me to predict we’ll be seeing more of the compound version. English may be, as you say, relatively resistant to compounding words, but it is scarcely resistant as a general matter to accommodating change.

    sdgraham


    • #8

    I appreciate the thoughtful reply. I don’t fully grasp the business jargon objection. In any event, to me, decisionmaking has a synergistic power that is blunted by the word-separation version whether it be by space or by hyphen. The heightened interest in political decisionmaking leads me to predict we’ll be seeing more of the compound version. English may be, as you say, relatively resistant to compounding words, but it is scarcely resistant as a general matter to accommodating change.

    You have the right to attempt to create hyphen-less compound nouns and to predict future use, based upon your clairvoyance.
    The rest of us have an equal right to reject your efforts here. :)

    • #9

    You have the right to attempt to create hyphen-less compound nouns and to predict future use, based upon your clairvoyance.
    The rest of us have an equal right to reject your efforts here. :)

    Your use of “clairvoyance” cheapens what I thought was a useful discussion. What I said was based on my sense of observable circumstances—the heightened public interest in political decisionmaking.

    Skip to content

    Decision-Making: Levels and Types | Management

    After reading this article you will learn about:- 1. Introduction to Decision Making in Management 2. The Nature of Decision Making 3. Decision Making Defined 4. The Decision-Making Context 5. Decision-Making Conditions 6. Types of Decisions 7. Decision-Making at Different Levels in the Organisation 8. Programmed and Non-Programmed Decisions 9. How Good should the Decisions Be?  and Other Details.

    Contents:

    1. Introduction to Decision Making in Management
    2. The Nature of Decision Making
    3. Definition of Decision Making
    4. The Context Decision-Making 
    5. Conditions for Decision-Making 
    6. Types of Decisions
    7. Decision-Making at Different Levels in the Organisation
    8. Programmed and Non-Programmed Decisions
    9. How Good should the Decisions Be? 
    10. Steps in Decision Making
    11. Group Decision Making — Use of Committees


    1. Introduction to Decision Making in Management:

    In today’s dynamic world business firms have to take a number of decisions every now and then. Managers know how important decision-making is from the organisational point of view. For example, in research and development management has to decide whether to pursue one or multiple design strategies. That is, should the company introduce one new high-priced stereo system or four complementary systems for each market segment?

    Likewise, the production department has to decide whether to manufacture all of the electrical components or to subcontract to other firms. Similarly, the financial manager has to decide whether to invest in a new plant or to lease. Again, marketing managers have to determine the appropriate production mix with regard to price and promotion: if multiple products are produced, what should be the price range among different products? Finally, in personnel decisions have to be made about new and different pay scales and the likely impact on current wage rates.

    None of the decisions is simple and it is virtually impossible for decision makers to account fully for all of the factors that will influence the outcome of the decision. As a result, the future is surrounded by uncertainty and risks have to be assumed.

    Decision making is an integral part of all marginal activities including organising, leading and controlling. However, decision-making is usually most closely associated with the planning function, inasmuch as it is an important tool for most planning activities. Everyday we have to make one decision or the other. Managers are faced with a wide range of decisions on any given day. For a manager the ability to make the best professional decision is the key to success. In fact, management is basically a study of the decision-making process within an organisation.


    2. The Nature of Decision Making:

    The ability to make good decisions is the key to successful managerial performance. Managers of most profit-seeking firms are always faced with a wide range of important decisions in the areas of pricing, product choice, cost control, advertising, capital investments, dividend policy and so on. Managers in the not-for-profit and public enterprises are faced with a similarly wide range of decisions. For example, the Dean of the Faculty of Indian Institute of Management, Calcutta, must decide how to allocate funds among such competing needs as travel, phone services, secretarial support, and so on.

    Longer-range decisions must be made concerning new facilities, new pro­grammes, the purchase or lease of a new computer and the decision to establish an executive development centre. Public sector managers or government agencies face such decisions as the construction of a new bridge over river Hooghly, the location of the bridge, the need to support public transit systems, the enforcement of anti-monopoly laws (such as the M.R.T.P. Act) and the economic viability of setting up a Second Mumbai Airport.

    A manager has always to take decisions of one sort or another. The decisions may be such as where to invest money, where to set up a new plant or warehouse, how to deal with to invest money, where to set up a new plant or warehouse, how to deal with an employee who is invariably late, or what subject should be brought into focus in the next departmental meeting. Whatever may be the nature and dimension of the problem at hand, the manager has to decide what actions need to be taken or has to arrange for others to decide.

    Decision making is perhaps the most important component of a manager’s activities. It plays the most important role in the planning process.

    As Stoner puts it:

    “Planning involves the most significant and far-reaching decisions a manager can make. When managers plan, they decide such matters as what goals or opportunities their organisation will pursue, what resources they will use, and who will perform each required task. When plans go wrong or out of track, managers have to decide what to do to correct the deviation. In fact, the whole planning process involves managers constantly in a series of decision-making situations. How good their decisions are will largely determine how effective their plan will be.”


    3. Definition of Decision Making:

    Most writers on management feel that management is basically decision-making. They argue that it is only through making decisions (about planning, organizing, directing and controlling) that an organisation can be en­abled to accomplish its short term and long term goals. In this article we shall discuss how managers can best go about reaching good (rational) decisions.

    Decision making can be defined as making a choice among alternative courses of action or as the process of choosing one alternative from among a set of rational alternatives. This definition has three different but interrelated implications. See Fig.8.1.

    Decision-Making

    Effective decision-making requires a clear understanding of the situation. Most people think that an effective decision is one that optimism some factor such as profits, sales employee welfare, or market share. In some situations, however, the effective decision may be one that minimises loss, expenses, or employee turnover. It may even mean selecting the best method for going out of business or terminating a contract.

    1. When managers make decisions they exercise choice — they decide what to do on the basis of some conscious and deliberate logic or judgement they have made in the past.

    2. When making a decision managers are faced with alternatives. As Stoner puts it: “It does not take a wise manager to reach a decision when there are no other possible choices. It does require wisdom and experience to evaluate several alternatives and select the best one.”

    3. When making a decision managers have a purpose. As R. W. Morell has put it, there is hardly any reason for carefully making a choice among alternatives unless the decision has to bring them closer to same goal.

    Therefore in this article the stress will be on the formal decision-making process, i.e., how managers proceed systematically to reach logical decisions that can help them in the best possible way to reach their goals.

    The implication is simple enough: Managers are almost always faced with a problem or oppor­tunity. So they propose and analyse alternative courses of action and finally make a choice that is likely to move the organisation in the direction of its goals.

    We noted that effective decision requires an under­standing of the situation. In a like manner, the effectiveness of any decision has to be assessed in terms of the decision­-maker’s underlying goal.


    4. The Context of Decision-Making:

    Here, we treat decision-making as essentially an individ­ual process, but a process that occurs in an organisa­tional context. Fig. 8.2 illustrates this point. The individual decision-maker lies at the centre of the proc­ess, but any given decision is likely to be influenced by a number of other people, departments and organisa­tions. Fig. 8.2 shows such important influences as super­visors, peers and colleagues, subordinates, other organisational components (such as other departments and their managers), and the environment (including elements of the task environment, such as competitors and suppliers, as well as general environmental factors such as technology and the economy).

    Decision-Making Context


    5. Conditions for Decision-Making:

    Although decision-making is essentially an individual process, the surrounding conditions can vary widely. Organisa­tional decisions are made under three con­ditions, viz., certainly, risk and uncertainty. These conditions are repre­sented in Fig. 8.3.

    Decision-Making Conditions

    Decision Making Under Certainty:

    When managers know with certainty what their possible alternatives are and what conditions are associated with each alternative, a state of certainty ex­ists.

    Decision Making under Risk:

    A more realistic decision-making situ­ation is a state of risk. Under a state of risk, the availability of each alternative and its potential pay-offs (rewards) and costs are all associated with profitability estimates. It is, therefore, quite obvious that the key element in decision-mak­ing under a state of risk is accurately determining the probabilities associated with each alternative.

    Decision Making under Uncertainty:

    However, most important and strategic decisions in modern organisations are taken under conditions of uncertainty. A state of uncertainty refers to a situation in which the decision maker does not know what all the alternatives are, and the risks associated with each, or what consequences each is likely to have.

    This complexity arises from the complexity and dynamism of today’s organisations and their environments. All successful organi­sations have made various effective decisions under uncertainty. The key to effective decision-mak­ing under uncertainty is to acquire as much relevant information as possible and to approach the situation from a logical and rational perspective. Intuition, judgement and experience always play a very important role in decision-making under uncertain conditions.


    6. Types of Decisions:

    As managers “we will make different types of decisions under different circumstances. When deciding whether or not to add a new wing to the administration building, or where to build a new plant, we will have to consider our choice carefully and extensively. When deciding what salary to pay a new employee, we will usually be able to be less cautious. Similarly, the amount of information we will have available to us when making a decision will vary. When choosing a supplier, we will usually dose on the basis of price and past performance. We will be reasonably confident that the supplier chosen will meet our expectations. When deciding to enter a new market, we will be much less certain about the success of our decision. For this reason, we will have to be particularly careful making decisions when we have little past experience or information to guide us.”

    In short, the nature and circumstances of a decision can vary enormously. Managers have to vary their approach to decision-making, depending on the particular situation involved. For our purposes, it will be useful to distinguish between situations that call for programmed decisions and those that call for non-programmed decisions. Business managers have to make various types of decisions. Such decisions can be placed into three broad categories: technical decisions, managerial decisions and institutional decisions.

    These three types of decisions may now be briefly illustrated:

    (i) Technical Decisions:

    In every organisation there is need to make decisions about core activities. These are basic activities relating directly to the ‘work of the organisation’. The core activities of Oil India Ltd. would be exploration, drilling, refining and distribution. Decisions concerning such activities are basically technical in nature. In general, the infor­mation required to solve problems related to these activities is generally concerned with the operational aspects of the technology involved.

    In short, technical decisions are concerned with the process through which inputs such as people, information or products are converted into outputs by the organisation.

    (ii) Managerial Decisions:

    Such decisions are related to the co-ordination and support of the core activities of the organisation.

    Managerial decision-making is also concerned with regulating and altering the relationship between the organisation and its external (immediate) environment. In order to maximize the efficiency of its core activities it becomes absolutely essential for management to ensure that these actions are not unduly disturbed by short-term changes in the environment.

    This explains why various organisations often build up inventories and forecasting of short-term changes in demand and supply conditions are integral parts of managerial decision-making.

    (iii) Institutional Decisions:

    Institutional decisions concern such diverse issues as diversification of activities, large-scale capital expansion, acquisition and mergers, shifts in R & D activities and various other organisational choices. Such decisions obviously involve long-term planning and policy formulation.

    In the words of Boone and Koontz: “Institutional decisions involve long-term planning and policy formulation with the aim of assuring the organisa­tion’s survival as a productive part of the economy and society.” The implication is clear: if an organisation is to thrive in the long run as a viable organisation, it must occupy a useful, productive place in the economy and society as a whole.

    With changes in society and in its economic framework, an organisation must adapt itself to such changes. Otherwise it may cease to exist. Due to shortage of traditional sources of energy the passenger car industry of the U.S. was reeling under recession from 1973 onwards. Some automobile companies faced with falling demand for petrol-operated cars have produced battery-operated motor cars.

    Managers in every organisation are faced with these three types of decisions, viz., technical, managerial and institutional. Table 8.1 illustrates each type of decision for two different organisa­tions: one profit-seeking firm (an oil company) and non-profit seeking firm (an oil company) and one non-profit organisation (a hospital).

    Two Organisations and Three Kinds of Decisions


    7. Decision-Making at Different Levels in the Organisation:

    A study of the decision-making in different organisations reveals that the three types of decisions listed above are not evenly spread throughout the organisa­tion. In general most institutional decisions are mostly made at the supervisory level. This point is il­lustrated in Fig.8.4.

    Types of Decisions and the Management Hierarchy

    Fig.8.4 gives an indication of the relative number of each type of decision made at each level in the organisations. However, the categories should not be treated as exclusive. For example, the production manager of a machinery manufacturing firm like the Texmaco might primarily be en­gaged in technical decisions, while the legal adviser of the company might be involved in institu­tional matters.


    8. Programmed and Non-Programmed Decisions:

    Nobel Laureate H. A. Simon has distinguished between two types of decisions, viz., programmed and non-programmed moved decisions. He has made the point that decisions differ not only in their content but also in terms of their relative uniqueness. By the term ‘relative uniqueness’ he means the degree to which a problem or decision (1) has been seen before; (2) occurs frequently and at regular intervals; and (3) has been solved or resolved in a satisfactory manner.

    According to Simon, programmed decisions are those which involve simple, common, frequently occurring problems that have well-established and understood solutions. On the contrary, non- programmed decisions are those involving new, often unusual or novel problems.

    In the words of Stoner:

    “Programmed decisions are those that are made in accordance with some habit, rule or procedure. Every organisation has written or unwritten policies that simplify decision-­making in a particular situation by limiting or excluding alternatives.” 

    A few examples of such decisions may now be given. In most situations managers will not have to worry about what to pay a new employee because most organisations have an established salary structure (or pay policy) for any position. (Of course, salary of highly skilled or top management is often negotiable. But these are exceptions rather than the rule).

    In a like manner managers will not generally have to think about the routine problems they face every day. Their habits, or those of their peers, will help them decide quickly what to do about them.

    There is no denying the fact that programmed decisions limit the freedom of managers to a considerable extent. In other words, managers hardly enjoy any discretion in matters involving programmed decisions set managers, decide what to do. This implies that programmed decisions set managers free on most occasions.

    The policies, rules or procedures by which managers make decisions free them of the need to find out new solutions to every problem they face. For instance, it would really be time-consuming to decide how to handle customer complaints on an individual basis. Adoption of routine procedures such as permitting customers to exchange unsuitable merchandise would really help matters.

    Since managers regularly have a series of decisions to make, organisations have to develop varying decision rules, programmes, policies, and procedures to use. Existing pay scales are used as guideline to fix the starting salary of a new factory guard or a new security officer. Similarly, when inventory of raw materials occurs.

    What can be said in favour of programmed decisions is that such decisions can be made “quickly, consistently and inexpensively since the procedures, rules and regulations eliminate the time-consuming process of identifying and evaluating alternatives and making a new choice each time a decision is required. While programmed decisions limit the flexibility of managers, they take little time and free the decision maker to devote his or her efforts to unique, non-programmed decisions. It is perhaps easiest for managers to make programmed decisions.”

    It is perhaps easiest for managers to refer to a policy rather than think of some problem and suggest solution. Effective managers usually rely on policy as a time saver. But they must remain alert for any exceptional case(s). For example, in case of a multi-product firm like the Godrej, the company policy may put a ceiling on the advertising budget for each product.

    However, a particular product, say Cinthol, may demand an expensive advertising campaign to counter a competitor’s aggressive marketing strategy. In such a situation a programmed decision — that is a decision to advertise the product in accordance with budget guidelines — may prove to be wrong. Thus when a situation calls for a programmed decision managers must ultimately make use of their own judgement.

    Non-programmed decisions, as Stoner has put it, are “those that are out of the ordinary or unique. If a problem is complex or exceptional, or, if it has not come up often enough to be covered by a policy, it must be handled by a non-programmed decision.”

    Such decisions are needed to solve problems like how to allocate an organisation’s resources, what to do about a failing product line, how community relations should be improved, and almost all significant problems a manager faces. In Table 8.2, we prepare a list of the traditional and modern techniques of decision-making.

    Techniques of Decision-Making

    In those organisations and decision situations where non-programmed decisions are the rule, the creation of alternatives and the selection and implementation of the most appropriate one becomes the distinction between effective and ineffective managers is drawn on the basis of their ability to make good non- programmed decisions.

    However, managers are often evaluated on the basis of their ability to solve problems, to apply creativity and judgement to the solution of problems and to make decisions in a logical, step-by-step manner. Since established procedures are of little use for making such decisions, new solutions are to be found out.

    This explains why most management training programmes are directed towards improving a manager’s ability to make non-programmed decisions by teaching them how to take such decisions.


    9. How Good should the Decisions Be?

    In traditional economic theory it is argued that the objective of the business manager is to maximize something. This is, of course, a realistic assumption provided the decision maker is able to obtain complete information concerning all possible alternatives and thus choose the best solution designed to achieve a particular goal. The manager will choose to maximize profit or some other value.

    However, 1978 Nobel Laureate H. A. Simon has made extensive study of managerial behaviour and on the basis of his investigation arrived at the conclusion that modern managers do not always attempt to maximize profits. Since managers are often forced to make decisions in the absence of complete information there is departure from the goal of profit maximization. Managers rarely consider all possible alternatives to the solution of a problem. Rather they examine a few alternatives that appear to be likely solutions.

    Most non-programmed decisions involve innumerable variables and it is neither possible nor feasible, with limited knowledge and resources, to examine them all. Therefore, Simon argues that instead of attempting to maximise, the modern manager satisfies.

    The manager, in fact, examines four to five alternative possibilities and chooses the best possible option from among them, rather than investing the time necessary to examine thoroughly all possible alternatives.

    Satisficing:

    An important concept developed by Simon is satisfying, which suggests that, rather than conducting an exhaustive search for the best possible alternative, decision makers tend to search only until they identify an alternative that meets some minimum standard of sufficiency.

    In the case of the manager who must choose a site for a new plant, some of the minimum requirements for the site may be that it must be within 500 meters of a railroad spur and within 2 kilometers of a major highway, be located in a community of at least 40,000 people, and cost less than Rs. 1,000,000.

    After a period of searching, the manager may locate a site 490 meters from a railroad spur, 1.8 kilometers from a highway, in a community of 41,000 people, and with a price tag of Rs. 950,000. The satisfying concept suggests that she or he will select this site even though further searching might reveal a better one.

    People tend to satisfice for a variety of reasons. Managers may simply be unwilling to ignore their own motives and therefore not be able to continue searching after a minimally acceptable alternative is identified. The decision maker may be unable to weigh and evaluate large numbers of alternatives and criteria. Subjective and personal considerations often intervene in decision situations. For example, the final criterion used to select a plant site might be its proximity to the manager’s home town. For all these reasons, the satisfying process plays a major role in decision-making.

    H. A. Simon makes the following assumptions about the decision-making process:

    1. Decision makers have incomplete information regarding the decision situation.

    2. Decision makers have incomplete information regarding all possible alternatives.

    3. Decision makers are unable or unwilling, or both, to fully anticipate the consequences of each available alternative.

    One important concept that Simon derived from these ideas is the notion of bounded rationality. He specifically notes that decision makers are limited by their values and unconscious reflexes, skills and habits. They are also limited by less-than-complete information and knowledge.

    Further, he argues that “the individual can be rational in terms of the organisation’s goals only to the extent that he is able to pursue a particular course of action, he has a correct conception of the goal of the action, and he is correctly informed about the conditions surrounding his choice. Within the boundaries laid down by these factors his choices are rational-goal-oriented.”

    Essentially, Simon suggests that people may try to be rational decision makers but that their rationality has limits.

    Consider the case of a manager attempting to decide where to locate a new manufacturing facility. To be rational, he or she must have the power and ability to make the correct decision, must clearly understand what the new facility is to do, and must have complete information about all alternatives. It is very unlikely that all of these conditions will be met, so the decision maker’s rationality is bounded by situational factors.

    According to Simon modern managers act within bounded rationality. The central feature of the principle of bounded rationality is Simon’s contention that the so-called ‘administrative man’ does not follow an exhaustive process of evaluation of the options open to find a course of action that is satisfactory or good enough. This Simon calls ‘satisfying’ and he describes it in contrast to the actions of ‘economic man’, who selects the best possible option from among those that are available.

    Simon states in Administrative Behaviour that managers ‘satisfies’, that is, look for a course of action that is satisfactory or good enough. The inference is that rather than optimizing in the strict sense of proceeding to a maximum they consider all the constraints bearing on the decision situation and choose a course of action that is satisfactory to them (i.e., good enough under the present circumstances).

    In short, the concept of bounded rationality refers to “boundaries or limits that exist in any problem situation that necessarily restrict the manager’s picture of the world. Such boundaries include limits to any manager’s knowledge of all alternatives as well as such elements as prices, costs and technology that cannot be changed by the decision maker.”

    Consequently the manager hardly strives to reach the optimum solution but realistically attempts to reach a satisfactory solution to the problem at hand.

    In fact, Simon’s view of the modern manager is different from the views of other writers on management. He attempts to present a realistic picture of a decision maker who is faced with two sets of constraints — internal and external. The former include such things as the individual’s intellectual ability (or-inability), training and experience, personality, attitudes and motivation.

    The latter refer to all external influences — influences exerted by workers of the organisation and groups outside it. Simon does not attempt to prove that managers do not attempt to make effective decisions. He only recognizes the very important fact that more often than not, decisions are balanced with the cost (measured in terms of time and money) of making it.


    10. Steps in Decision Making:

    There are various types of decisions such as setting up a new area or adding or dropping a new product on the product line, or hiring additional sales persons to increase the market share for a particular product, or even dismissing a worker. Whatever may be type of decision the decision maker has to proceed through a number of well-defined and interrelated steps. Some decisions can be made in a minute’s time.

    On the contrary, others may take months or years. Some decisions may be made hur­riedly and thus prove to be ineffective. On the con­trary, some decisions may be taken after much deliberation and careful consideration of alternatives. But all decisions have to proceed through these steps. Fig. 8.5 illustrates the steps in the decision-making process. However, the actual process of decision-making may not be as rational as Fig. 8.5 implies.

    1. Recognising and Defining the Decision Situation,

    2. Perception of the Environment,

    3. Establishing Objectives,

    4. Problem Formulation,

    5. Identification of Resources and Constraints,

    6. Development of Alternatives,

    7. Evaluation of Alternatives and Selection of a Course of Action,

    8. Implementation,

    9. Feedback, and

    10. Follow-up and Evaluation.

    Steps in Decision-Making

    1. Recognising and Defining the Decision Situation:

    The first step in making a decision is recognising that a decision is necessary — there must be some stimulus to initiate the process. For example, when an impor­tant equipment breaks down, the manager has to de­cide whether to repair or replace it.

    Moreover, the manager must also be able to define the situation. This is partly a matter of determining how the problem that is being addressed came about. This is an important step because situation definition plays a major role in subsequent steps.

    2. Perception of the Environment:

    The manager does not operate in a certain environ­ment. Assessing the effect of possible future changes in the environment is an essential step in decision­-making. The term ‘environment’ here covers all fac­tors external to the firm. This explains why the decision maker must become aware of and be sensi­tive to the decision environment before any decision is possible.

    This sensitivity results from two inputs:

    1. Specific information which is of relevance to the decision maker (such as cost control re­ports, quality control reports, periodical sales reports, data on raw materials prices, etc.).

    2. General information which are impressionistic in nature about conditions and operations (such as the manager’s ‘feel’ for the situation).

    It is necessary to distinguish, at the outset, between the environment as an objective entity and the manager’s perception of the environment. Most often than not decision makers filter the information they receive, i.e., they pay more attention to some information than to other information. This practice sometimes prove to be disastrous to both the decision maker and the organisation.

    The manager’s primary task is to monitor the environment for potential change. In fact, in every management information system there is an in-built early warning signal system of reporting various environmental developments such as new or adapted products by competing producers; changes in attitudes and sentiments of buyers; development of new processes or methods of production.

    If the organisation is to survive and grow in the long nm it must be ready to adapt and evolve in response to diverse environmental changes. In short, while strategy should not be conceived as exclusively concerned with the relation between the enterprise and its environment, assessing the effects of possible future changes in the environment is an essential task in strategy formulation.

    There are two steps to this process: the first is to consider how the relevant environmental factors may change; the second is to assess the strategic implications of such changes for the firm.

    3. Establishing Objectives:

    When a manager makes a decision, he (she) chooses from some set of alternatives as the one he (she) believes will best contribute to some particular end result. That is, decisions are made within the context of, and influenced by, the objective or set of objectives defined by the decision maker. Thus the second step in the decision process is to establish objectives or to take account of those that have been previously defined.

    Objectives have to be defined in a concrete, operational form, since if these are stated in a general or vague form, it becomes virtually impossible to establish whether or not a particular decision brings one closer to the stated goal.

    Consider, for example, the following two ways in which a firm might state one of its objectives:

    Objective A:

    To increase our share of the market.

    Objective B:

    To increase our market share by at least 3.5% in the next fiscal year.

    With ‘Objective A’, the firm has little way to evaluate the effectiveness of various decisions as they relate to their goal. A 0.001% increase in market share satisfies the objective, as does a 1% increase, or 10% increase. However, with an objective stated as in B, there would be less room for debate about success or failure.

    The firm either increases market share by the prescribed amount in B might be revised. If the firm consistently achieves a given objective, then the objective might be reviewed or changed to prevent under-achievement.

    4. Problem Formulation:

    With objectives firmly in hand, the next phase in the decision process is to define the particular problem that gives (give) rise to the need to make a decision. The fact that someone must make a decision implies that there is a problem to be solved. In defining or formulating a problem the decision maker should be as precise as possible and should state the problem explicitly. Problems act as barriers to the achievement of organisation goals.

    In other words, they act as obstacles to be overcome by the decision makers — when an organisation fails to achieve its goals, a performance gap is said to exist. This gap reveals the difference between the predicted or expected level of performance and the actual level.

    Problem formulation seems to be the most neglected aspect of the decision-making process. More often than not it is simply assessed that the nature of a managerial problem is obvious to all concerned. A major problem, however, is that managers often feel psychologically uncomfortable to think about problems.

    Moreover, since time management is a very real part of managerial work manages devote much of their time for problem solving and not for problem formulation. In general managers simply do not give themselves sufficient time to consider the situation and do an effective job of problem formulation.

    5. Identification of Resources and Constraints:

    Just as a business manager does not operate in isolation, problem solving does not occur in vacuum. In fact, problem solving lies embedded in the fabric of the organisations and its external environ­ment. The truth is that most organisations face a multiplicity of problems at the same time. These problems compete for the lim­ited amount of organisation’s resources and manager’s attention.

    Anything that contributes to problem solving is a resource which includes time, money, personnel, experience, equipment, raw materials and information. Managers use various types of resources and we often speak of five Ms in this context, viz., mate­rials, money, manpower, machinery and management. See Fig.8.7.

    Basic Resources of the Organisation

    Fig. 8.7: Basic resources of the organisation – the five

    However, managers are faced with vari­ous constraints in the decision-making process. A significant constraint is, of course, lack of adequate resources. Other constraints may be unfavourable government policy (such as the MRTP Act which acts as a constraint on the expansion of the so-called large houses in India), or adverse attitude of employees (due to lack of motivation and morale). In general constraints are factors that impede problem solution or limit managers in their efforts to solve a problem.

    The decision maker has to develop a brief explicit list of the major resources which enables the decision maker to make the best possible utilization of the organisation’s resources. In other words, such an exhaustive list permits the decision-maker to budget organisational assets in order to maximize their usefulness.

    In a like manner the listing of constraints “alerts the decision maker to the important stumbling blocks affecting a solution so that they can be avoided.” Furthermore, “organisations sometimes confront situations in which the absence of a specific resource or the existence of a particular constraint is a significant problem itself.”

    6. Development of Alternatives:

    The generation of various possible alternatives is essential to the process of decision-making. Perhaps the most important step in decision-making process is to develop alternative courses of action to deal with the problem situation. It is generally useful to design the process in such a way that both obvious, standard solutions and creative, informative solutions or alternatives are generated.

    In general, the more important the decision, the more attention is directed to developing alternatives. If the decision involves where to build a multi-crore rupee office building, a great deal of time and expertise will be devoted to identifying the best locations.

    Although managers should encourage creative solutions, they should also recognise that various constraints often limit their alternatives. Common constraints include legal restrictions, moral and ethical norms, authority constraints, or constraints imposed by the power and authority of the manager, available technology, economic considerations and unofficial social norms.

    In most real-life situations managers adopt a shortcut approach and thus fail to arrive at the best solution. In fact, managers often identify one or two alternatives very fairly and choose from among them. So more effective alternatives are not considered. Writers on organisations have suggested that creativity is needed at this stage in developing various possible alternatives for consideration.

    How much time and money should be developing alternatives:

    Time and money are the important resources at the disposal of the decision-maker. However, time seems to be the ultimate scarce resource of the manager. Moreover, since there are always additional alternatives waiting to be discovered, the process of generating alternatives could conceivably go on forever.

    Managers should consider three proximate factors in determining the appropriate amount to spend in generating alternatives. Firstly, managers should assess how important is this problem or opportunity. The more important the decision the greater the value of marginal improvements in the solution.

    The second factor is the ability of the decision-maker to differentiate accurately among alterna­tives determining the amount of time that he should devote in developing alternatives and cannot, in advance, tell the difference between two alternatives and cannot rank them accurately according to this likely effectiveness.

    Thirdly, the larger the number of people concerned with a problem, the greater the number of likely alternatives to be sought.

    In this context Boone and Koontz have opined that: “when dealing with complex problems effecting numerous people, it is often necessary to compromise on some points. But compromises by their very nature require participants to sacrifice some of their interests. This can lead to considerable dissatisfaction or frustration. These ‘human costs’ are often considerable” even though these cannot be measured in terms of money.

    7. Evaluation of Alternatives and Selection of a Course of Action:

    The next step in the decision-making process is evaluating each of the alternatives generated in the previous step. Usually each alternative has to be assessed to determine its feasibility, its satisfactoriness, and its consequences. This step lies at the heart of the decision-making process. All the previous steps have been of a preparatory nature and it is in this step that the manager finally decides what to do.

    This crucial stage has the following three distinct but closely interrelated phases:

    (i) Determining feasible alternatives:

    In case where a large number of alternatives have been generated, it is quite likely that many of them will not appear to be feasible. There are two reasons for this. Either the resources necessary to implement the alternatives are not available. Alternatively there may be prohibitive constraints.

    As Boone and Kurtz have argued: “if judgement was suspended during the creative generation of alternatives in the previous step, most of the alternatives generated would fall into the infusible category. Separating the feasible alternatives from the infeasible ones saves time, since the decision maker can then evaluate only those alternatives that are likely to be chosen.”

    (ii) Evaluation of alternatives: 

    The evaluation of alternatives is no doubt a complex exercise. In fact many of the operations research techniques developed during the last few decades are methods of determining the relative efficiency of various alternatives.

    Alvar Elbing has proposed the following five rules for evaluating alternatives:

    1. A solution should have substantial quality so that it can meet organisational goals.

    2. A solution has to be acceptable to those affected by it and to those who must implement it.

    3. A solution has to be evaluated in terms of the anticipated responses to it.

    4. The risks of each alternative must be considered.

    5. The choice of solution should focus on present alternatives, not past possibilities.

    Is quality consistent with goals?

    In order to assess the quality of a solution we have to reintroduce the concepts of efficiency and effectiveness. In fact, the quality of a solution has these two dimen­sions. Efficiency may be reinterpreted as the ratio of output to inputs. In other words, it is a measure of organisational productivity. It therefore lies at the heart of business cost-benefit analysis.

    On the contrary, effectiveness is a measure of the extent to which an alternative meets the stated objective (regardless of the costs involved). Before attempting to evaluate the quality of any alternative, it is absolutely essential for the decision-maker to first establish the extent to which each of these criteria will be used.

    (iii) Choosing the Most Appropriate Alternative:

    After evaluating the alternatives properly it is necessary to choose the alternative which is accept­able to those who must implement it and those who have to bear the consequences of the decision. Failure to meet this condition often results in the failure of the whole decision-making process to solve problems. In fact, choosing the best alternative in terms of facilities, satisfactoriness and affordable consequences is the real crux (or the essence) of the decision-making process.

    A related point may be noted in the context. It is possible to assess the acceptability and efficacy (efficiency) of a proposed solution by considering the anticipated responses to it. Such a response refers to the reaction of the organisation and its individual members to an alternative that has been chosen. This should be of critical concern to the manager or decision maker.

    The solution is simple to find: even a technically mediocre solution may prove to be ‘effective’ (in the sense defined above) if it is implemented with enthusiasm and dedication. On the contrary, the technically correct alternative may fail to evolve sufficient response or succeed if it is implemented in half-hearted and haphazard fashion.

    This explains why most writers on management stress the importance of including as many members of the organisation as feasible in the decision-making process. True, “participation in problem solving by organisational members should increase their receptiveness to the chosen alternative.”

    It is also necessary to consider the various types of risks associated with each alternative. In fact, different risks are involved for different individuals and groups in the organisation. It may be stressed at this stage that the differences among those who make decision, those who implement them and those who must live on them should not be minimised.

    8. Implementation:

    After one or more alternatives have been selected, the manager must put the alternative or alternatives into effect. In some situations, implementation may be fairly easy; in other situations it may be quite difficult. However, since most managerial problems are intimately concerned with the human element in the organisation, implementation of solution is no doubt a complex exercise.

    It is to be noted that so far no generalised rules have been developed that deal with managing the implementation phase.

    However, three questions must be answered at the phase:

    Firstly, what should the internal structure of implementation be? In other words, what should be done? When? By whom? Since the solution of most managerial problems requires the combined effort of various members of the organisation, each must understand what role he (she) has to play during each phase of the implementation process.

    Secondly, how can the manager reward organisation members for participating in the implemen­tation of the proposed solution? Decisions are no doubt made by managers but these are carried out by other members of the organisation. So managers must ensure that those who are responsible for implementation have some stake — financial or otherwise — in the success of the solution.

    Thirdly, how provisions for evaluation and modification of the chosen solution during the implementation process be made? As implementation of solution proceeds, organisation members should be able to modify the solution based on what they learn during implementation.

    But unless some specific provision is made for modification of the chosen solution, the chosen alternative may be left untouched and implemented without any thought of possible modification — even in those situations where minor adjustments would produce better solutions.

    The key to effective implementation is action planning, a well thought out, step-by-step descrip­tion of the programme. Some appropriate techniques for solving organisational problems arising from decision situations are tactical plans, operational plans and programmes, and standing plans. A programme, for example, might be developed for the sole purpose of implementing a course of action for solving an organisational problem.

    Another problem to consider when implementing decisions is people’s resistance to change. There are various reasons for such resistance such as insecurity, inconvenience and fear of the unknown. Hence, it will be judicious on the part of managers to anticipate potential resistance at various stages of the implementation process.

    Managers should also recognise that “even when all alternatives have been evaluated as precisely as possible and consequences of each alternative weighed, it is likely that unanticipated conse­quences will also arise. Unexpected cost increases a less-than-perfect ‘fit’ with existing organisa­tional subsystems, unpredicted effects on cash-flow or operating expenses, or any number of other situations could develop after the implementation process has begun”.

    9. Feedback:

    Feedback is “a necessary component of the decision process, providing the decision maker with a means of determining the effectiveness of the chosen alternatives in solving the problem or taking advantage of the opportunity and moving the organisation closer to the attainment of its goals.”

    In order to make such an evaluation of the effectiveness of a possible decision, the following three conditions must be fulfilled:

    Firstly, there must exist a set of standards which act as yardstick against which to compare performance. For example, if the sales goal of a company in the next quarter is Rs. 2 lakhs more than the current quarter, the relevant standard is present sales turnover plus Rs. 2 lakhs. Likewise if a company adopts a zero defect programme, a zero rejection rate for output becomes the relevant standard.

    Secondly, performance data must be readily available so that the comparison to standards may be made. The chief approach to formulating the data collection process is the design of management information systems.

    Finally, it is absolutely essential to develop a data analysis strategy. Such a strategy includes a formal plan which outlines how the data will be used. However, one unfortunate characteristic of most data are never used for decision-making purposes. The problem is not insoluble.

    In fact, “managers who know exactly how the data are to be analysed will be able to specify the types of the data they need, the most preferred format, and the time sequence in which they are needed.” Such advance specifications are likely to act as aids in reducing the mass of useless data that are often collected.

    10. Follow-up and Evaluation:

    As the final step in the decision-making process, managers should be very sure to evaluate the effectiveness of their decision. That is, they should make sure that the alternatives chosen in step 5 and implemented in step 6 have accomplished the desired result.

    When an implemented alternative fails to work, the manager has to respond quickly. There are several ways of doing it. One of the alternatives that was identified previously (the second or third choice) could be adopted.

    Alternatively, the manager might recognise that the situation was not correctly defined to start with and begin the decision-making process all over again. Finally, the manager might decide that the alternative originally chosen is in fact appropriate, but that it simply has not yet had time to work or should be implemented in a different way.

    Fig. 8.8 shows an effective process for evaluating alternatives. Prior to the actual decision, exist­ing conditions relevant to the deci­sion itself are observed, assessed and measured. Finally, a post deci­sion observation should be made to determine how successful the decision was in solving the original problem.

    Evaluating the Effectiveness of Decisions


    11. Group Decision Making—Use of Committees:

    The steps in the decision-making process descried so far focused primarily on the individual decision maker. However, in practice, most of the decision in large, complex organisations are made by groups. This often creates additional problems (which are often of a complex nature) because of shared power, bargaining activities and need for compromise present in most group decisions.

    Group decision-making is the accepted norm in Japanese organisations. Use is made of commit­tees in the decision-making process. The process starts with supervisory managers meeting as a group to analyse a problem or opportunity and develop alternative solutions.

    Two or three of the most likely alternatives are then presented to top management which makes the final decision. Lower level managers are used in the preliminary stages of the decision process. They are entrusted with responsibilities in decision-making. At the same time the amount of time top management must devote to the process is considerably reduced.

    The practice in America is just the opposite. American managers often criticise the group (or committee approach) on two major grounds. Firstly, it is thought to be a waste of time. Secondly, this is treated as a method of obtaining only compromise solutions.

    However, the fact remains that today’s complex world in which most organisations operate makes it increasingly difficult for a single manager to make complex decisions independently. Even in America task forces, conferences, committees and staff meetings are widely made use of in arriving at important (and often strategic) decisions.

    Strengths and Weaknesses:

    Strengths:

    Group decision-making has its merit and drawbacks. Empirical evidence available so far suggests that decision made by groups are more accurate than those made by individuals. This is more so in those situations involving complex problems where no one member is a specialist in the problem area.

    The saying ‘two brains are better than one’, like many others, contains an elephant of truth. The implication of this statement in the present context is clear: more information can be processed by the various group members.

    A second advantage of this method is that “the presence of several group members also means that more alternative solutions may be proposed and a great number of proposed solution can be analysed.”

    Thirdly, managers’ acceptance of solution is increased through their participation.

    Fourthly, managers can communicate decisions and their rationale to their own work groups.

    Finally, a major strength of group decision-making is the relative ease of implementing decisions that have been made.

    Weaknesses:

    However, there are certain weaknesses of the group decision-making process.

    These are the following:

    Firstly, group decisions are slower than individual decisions and are more costly in terms of time and money due to the number of personnel involved.

    Secondly, more often than not group decisions are comprehensive decisions resulting from differing points of view of individual members, rather than the selection of the most appropriate (or the best possible) choice for solving the problem.

    In the opinion of Boone and Koontz: “There is often pressure to accept the decision favoured by most group members. Group-think — a phenomenon in which the time for group cohesiveness and consequence becomes stronger than the desire for the best possible decision — may occur. Some groups experience more indecisiveness than individual decision makers since the pressure to reach a decision is diffused among the group members.”

    Thirdly, group decision-making is characterised by indecisiveness and buck passing — blaming one another for a poorly made decision or the lack of decision. This occurs in situations where clear lines of authority and responsibility for making a decision have not been drawn. This phenomenon can, of course, be prevented if the leader accepts ultimate responsibility for decision-making.

    Finally, Normal R. F. Maier has pointed out that, in most instance, one person or a few individuals will dominate the group because of differences in status or rank from the other members or through force of personality. The table below summarizes the advantages and disadvantages of group decision-making.

    Strengths and Weeknesses of Group Decision-Making


    Page load link

    Go to Top

    This article is about decision-making as analyzed in psychology. For a broader discipline, see Decision theory. For decision-making in groups, see Group decision-making.

    In psychology, decision-making (also spelled decision making and decisionmaking) is regarded as the cognitive process resulting in the selection of a belief or a course of action among several possible alternative options. It could be either rational or irrational. The decision-making process is a reasoning process based on assumptions of values, preferences and beliefs of the decision-maker.[1] Every decision-making process produces a final choice, which may or may not prompt action.

    Sample flowchart representing a decision process when confronted with a lamp that fails to light.

    Research about decision-making is also published under the label problem solving, particularly in European psychological research.[2]

    OverviewEdit

    Decision-making can be regarded as a problem-solving activity yielding a solution deemed to be optimal, or at least satisfactory. It is therefore a process which can be more or less rational or irrational and can be based on explicit or tacit knowledge and beliefs. Tacit knowledge is often used to fill the gaps in complex decision-making processes.[3] Usually, both of these types of knowledge, tacit and explicit, are used together in the decision-making process.

    Human performance has been the subject of active research from several perspectives:

    • Psychological: examining individual decisions in the context of a set of needs, preferences and values the individual has or seeks.
    • Cognitive: the decision-making process is regarded as a continuous process integrated in the interaction with the environment.
    • Normative: the analysis of individual decisions concerned with the logic of decision-making, or communicative rationality, and the invariant choice it leads to.[4]

    A major part of decision-making involves the analysis of a finite set of alternatives described in terms of evaluative criteria. Then the task might be to rank these alternatives in terms of how attractive they are to the decision-maker(s) when all the criteria are considered simultaneously. Another task might be to find the best alternative or to determine the relative total priority of each alternative (for instance, if alternatives represent projects competing for funds) when all the criteria are considered simultaneously. Solving such problems is the focus of multiple-criteria decision analysis (MCDA). This area of decision-making, although very old, has attracted the interest of many researchers and practitioners and is still highly debated as there are many MCDA methods which may yield very different results when they are applied to exactly the same data.[5] This leads to the formulation of a decision-making paradox. Logical decision-making is an important part of all science-based professions, where specialists apply their knowledge in a given area to make informed decisions. For example, medical decision-making often involves a diagnosis and the selection of appropriate treatment. But naturalistic decision-making research shows that in situations with higher time pressure, higher stakes, or increased ambiguities, experts may use intuitive decision-making rather than structured approaches. They may follow a recognition primed decision that fits their experience, and arrive at a course of action without weighing alternatives.[6]

    The decision-maker’s environment can play a part in the decision-making process. For example, environmental complexity is a factor that influences cognitive function.[7] A complex environment is an environment with a large number of different possible states which come and go over time.[8] Studies done at the University of Colorado have shown that more complex environments correlate with higher cognitive function, which means that a decision can be influenced by the location. One experiment measured complexity in a room by the number of small objects and appliances present; a simple room had less of those things. Cognitive function was greatly affected by the higher measure of environmental complexity, making it easier to think about the situation and make a better decision.[7]

    Problem solving vs. decision makingEdit

    It is important to differentiate between problem solving, or problem analysis, and decision-making. Problem solving is the process of investigating the given information and finding all possible solutions through invention or discovery. Traditionally, it is argued that problem solving is a step towards decision making, so that the information gathered in that process may be used towards decision-making.[9][page needed]

    Characteristics of problem solving
    • Problems are merely deviations from performance standards.
    • Problems must be precisely identified and described
    • Problems are caused by a change from a distinctive feature
    • Something can always be used to distinguish between what has and hasn’t been affected by a cause
    • Causes of problems can be deduced from relevant changes found in analyzing the problem
    • Most likely cause of a problem is the one that exactly explains all the facts, while having the fewest (or weakest) assumptions (Occam’s razor).
    Characteristics of decision-making
    • Objectives must first be established
    • Objectives must be classified and placed in order of importance
    • Alternative actions must be developed
    • The alternatives must be evaluated against all the objectives
    • The alternative that is able to achieve all the objectives is the tentative decision
    • The tentative decision is evaluated for more possible consequences
    • The decisive actions are taken, and additional actions are taken to prevent any adverse consequences from becoming problems and starting both systems (problem analysis and decision-making) all over again
    • There are steps that are generally followed that result in a decision model that can be used to determine an optimal production plan[10]
    • In a situation featuring conflict, role-playing may be helpful for predicting decisions to be made by involved parties[11]

    Analysis paralysisEdit

    When a group or individual is unable to make it through the problem-solving step on the way to making a decision, they could be experiencing analysis paralysis. Analysis paralysis is the state that a person enters where they are unable to make a decision, in effect paralyzing the outcome.[12][13] Some of the main causes for analysis paralysis is the overwhelming flood of incoming data or the tendency to overanalyze the situation at hand.[14] There are said to be three different types of analysis paralysis.[15]

    • The first is analysis process paralysis. This type of paralysis is often spoken of as a cyclical process. One is unable to make a decision because they get stuck going over the information again and again for fear of making the wrong decision.
    • The second is decision precision paralysis. This paralysis is cyclical, just like the first one, but instead of going over the same information, the decision-maker will find new questions and information from their analysis and that will lead them to explore into further possibilities rather than making a decision.
    • The third is risk uncertainty paralysis. This paralysis occurs when the decision-maker wants to eliminate any uncertainty but the examination of provided information is unable to get rid of all uncertainty.

    Extinction by instinctEdit

    On the opposite side of analysis paralysis is the phenomenon called extinction by instinct. Extinction by instinct is the state that a person is in when they make careless decisions without detailed planning or thorough systematic processes.[16] Extinction by instinct can possibly be fixed by implementing a structural system, like checks and balances into a group or one’s life. Analysis paralysis is the exact opposite where a group’s schedule could be saturated by too much of a structural checks and balance system.[16]

    Extinction by instinct in a group setting

    Groupthink is another occurrence that falls under the idea of extinction by instinct. Groupthink is when members in a group become more involved in the “value of the group (and their being part of it) higher than anything else”; thus, creating a habit of making decisions quickly and unanimously. In other words, a group stuck in groupthink is participating in the phenomenon of extinction by instinct.[17]

    Information overloadEdit

    Information overload is «a gap between the volume of information and the tools we have to assimilate» it.[18] Information used in decision-making is to reduce or eliminate the uncertainty.[19] Excessive information affects problem processing and tasking, which affects decision-making.[20] Psychologist George Armitage Miller suggests that humans’ decision making becomes inhibited because human brains can only hold a limited amount of information.[21] Crystal C. Hall and colleagues described an «illusion of knowledge», which means that as individuals encounter too much knowledge, it can interfere with their ability to make rational decisions.[22] Other names for information overload are information anxiety, information explosion, infobesity, and infoxication.[23][24][25][26]

    Decision fatigueEdit

    Decision fatigue is when a sizable amount of decision-making leads to a decline in decision-making skills. People who make decisions in an extended period of time begin to lose mental energy needed to analyze all possible solutions. Impulsive decision-making and decision avoidance are two possible paths that extend from decision fatigue. Impulse decisions are made more often when a person is tired of analysis situations or solutions; the solution they make is to act and not think.[27] Decision avoidance is when a person evades the situation entirely by not ever making a decision. Decision avoidance is different from analysis paralysis because this sensation is about avoiding the situation entirely, while analysis paralysis is continually looking at the decisions to be made but still unable to make a choice.[28][self-published source]

    Post-decision analysisEdit

    Evaluation and analysis of past decisions is complementary to decision-making. See also mental accounting and Postmortem documentation.

    NeuroscienceEdit

    Decision-making is a region of intense study in the fields of systems neuroscience, and cognitive neuroscience. Several brain structures, including the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), orbitofrontal cortex, and the overlapping ventromedial prefrontal cortex are believed to be involved in decision-making processes. A neuroimaging study[29] found distinctive patterns of neural activation in these regions depending on whether decisions were made on the basis of perceived personal volition or following directions from someone else. Patients with damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex have difficulty making advantageous decisions.[30][page needed]

    A common laboratory paradigm for studying neural decision-making is the two-alternative forced choice task (2AFC), in which a subject has to choose between two alternatives within a certain time. A study of a two-alternative forced choice task involving rhesus monkeys found that neurons in the parietal cortex not only represent the formation of a decision[31] but also signal the degree of certainty (or «confidence») associated with the decision.[32] A 2012 study found that rats and humans can optimally accumulate incoming sensory evidence, to make statistically optimal decisions.[33] Another study found that lesions to the ACC in the macaque resulted in impaired decision-making in the long run of reinforcement guided tasks suggesting that the ACC may be involved in evaluating past reinforcement information and guiding future action.[34] It has recently been argued that the development of formal frameworks will allow neuroscientists to study richer and more naturalistic paradigms than simple 2AFC decision tasks; in particular, such decisions may involve planning and information search across temporally extended environments.[35]

    EmotionsEdit

    Emotion appears able to aid the decision-making process. Decision-making often occurs in the face of uncertainty about whether one’s choices will lead to benefit or harm (see also Risk). The somatic marker hypothesis is a neurobiological theory of how decisions are made in the face of uncertain outcomes.[36] This theory holds that such decisions are aided by emotions, in the form of bodily states, that are elicited during the deliberation of future consequences and that mark different options for behavior as being advantageous or disadvantageous. This process involves an interplay between neural systems that elicit emotional/bodily states and neural systems that map these emotional/bodily states.[37] A recent lesion mapping study of 152 patients with focal brain lesions conducted by Aron K. Barbey and colleagues provided evidence to help discover the neural mechanisms of emotional intelligence.[38][39][40]

    Decision-making techniquesEdit

    Decision-making techniques can be separated into two broad categories: group decision-making techniques and individual decision-making techniques. Individual decision-making techniques can also often be applied by a group.

    GroupEdit

    • Consensus decision-making tries to avoid «winners» and «losers». Consensus requires that a majority approve a given course of action, but that the minority agree to go along with the course of action. In other words, if the minority opposes the course of action, consensus requires that the course of action be modified to remove objectionable features.
    • Voting-based methods:
      • Majority requires support from more than 50% of the members of the group. Thus, the bar for action is lower than with consensus. See also Condorcet method.
      • Plurality, where the largest faction in a group decides, even if it falls short of a majority.
      • Score voting (or range voting) lets each member score one or more of the available options, specifying both preference and intensity of preference information. The option with the highest total or average is chosen. This method has experimentally been shown to produce the lowest Bayesian regret among common voting methods, even when voters are strategic.[41] It addresses issues of voting paradox and majority rule. See also approval voting.
      • Quadratic voting allows participants to cast their preference and intensity of preference for each decision (as opposed to a simple for or against decision). As in score voting, it addresses issues of voting paradox and majority rule.
    • Delphi method is a structured communication technique for groups, originally developed for collaborative forecasting but has also been used for policy making.[42]
    • Dotmocracy is a facilitation method that relies on the use of special forms called Dotmocracy. They are sheets that allows large groups to collectively brainstorm and recognize agreements on an unlimited number of ideas they have each wrote.[43]
    • Participative decision-making occurs when an authority opens up the decision-making process to a group of people for a collaborative effort.
    • Decision engineering uses a visual map of the decision-making process based on system dynamics and can be automated through a decision modeling tool, integrating big data, machine learning, and expert knowledge as appropriate.

    IndividualEdit

    • Decisional balance sheet: listing the advantages and disadvantages (benefits and costs, pros and cons) of each option, as suggested by Plato’s Protagoras and by Benjamin Franklin.[44]
    • Expected-value optimization: choosing the alternative with the highest probability-weighted utility, possibly with some consideration for risk aversion. This may involve considering the opportunity cost of different alternatives. See also Decision analysis and Decision theory.
    • Satisficing: examining alternatives only until the first acceptable one is found. The opposite is maximizing or optimizing, in which many or all alternatives are examined in order to find the best option.
    • Acquiesce to a person in authority or an «expert»; «just following orders».
    • Anti-authoritarianism: taking the most opposite action compared to the advice of mistrusted authorities.
    • Flipism e.g. flipping a coin, cutting a deck of playing cards, and other random or coincidence methods – or prayer, tarot cards, astrology, augurs, revelation, or other forms of divination, superstition or pseudoscience.
    • Automated decision support: setting up criteria for automated decisions.
    • Decision support systems: using decision-making software when faced with highly complex decisions or when considering many stakeholders, categories, or other factors that affect decisions.

    StepsEdit

    A variety of researchers have formulated similar prescriptive steps aimed at improving decision-making.

    GOFEREdit

    In the 1980s, psychologist Leon Mann and colleagues developed a decision-making process called GOFER, which they taught to adolescents, as summarized in the book Teaching Decision Making To Adolescents.[45] The process was based on extensive earlier research conducted with psychologist Irving Janis.[46] GOFER is an acronym for five decision-making steps:[47]

    1. Goals clarification: Survey values and objectives.
    2. Options generation: Consider a wide range of alternative actions.
    3. Facts-finding: Search for information.
    4. Consideration of Effects: Weigh the positive and negative consequences of the options.
    5. Review and implementation: Plan how to review the options and implement them.

    OtherEdit

    In 2007, Pam Brown of Singleton Hospital in Swansea, Wales, divided the decision-making process into seven steps:[48]

    1. Outline the goal and outcome.
    2. Gather data.
    3. Develop alternatives (i.e., brainstorming).
    4. List pros and cons of each alternative.
    5. Make the decision.
    6. Immediately take action to implement it.
    7. Learn from and reflect on the decision.

    In 2008, Kristina Guo published the DECIDE model of decision-making, which has six parts:[49]

    1. Define the problem
    2. Establish or Enumerate all the criteria (constraints)
    3. Consider or Collect all the alternatives
    4. Identify the best alternative
    5. Develop and implement a plan of action
    6. Evaluate and monitor the solution and examine feedback when necessary

    In 2009, professor John Pijanowski described how the Arkansas Program, an ethics curriculum at the University of Arkansas, used eight stages of moral decision-making based on the work of James Rest:[50]: 6 

    1. Establishing community: Create and nurture the relationships, norms, and procedures that will influence how problems are understood and communicated. This stage takes place prior to and during a moral dilemma.
    2. Perception: Recognize that a problem exists.
    3. Interpretation: Identify competing explanations for the problem, and evaluate the drivers behind those interpretations.
    4. Judgment: Sift through various possible actions or responses and determine which is more justifiable.
    5. Motivation: Examine the competing commitments which may distract from a more moral course of action and then prioritize and commit to moral values over other personal, institutional or social values.
    6. Action: Follow through with action that supports the more justified decision.
    7. Reflection in action.
    8. Reflection on action.

    Group stagesEdit

    There are four stages or phases that should be involved in all group decision-making:[51]

    • Orientation. Members meet for the first time and start to get to know each other.
    • Conflict. Once group members become familiar with each other, disputes, little fights and arguments occur. Group members eventually work it out.
    • Emergence. The group begins to clear up vague opinions by talking about them.
    • Reinforcement. Members finally make a decision and provide justification for it.

    It is said that establishing critical norms in a group improves the quality of decisions, while the majority of opinions (called consensus norms) do not.[52]

    Conflicts in socialization are divided in to functional and dysfunctional types. Functional conflicts are mostly the questioning the managers assumptions in their decision making and dysfunctional conflicts are like personal attacks and every action which decrease team effectiveness. Functional conflicts are the better ones to gain higher quality decision making caused by the increased team knowledge and shared understanding.[53]

    Rational and irrationalEdit

    In economics, it is thought that if humans are rational and free to make their own decisions, then they would behave according to rational choice theory.[54]: 368–370  Rational choice theory says that a person consistently makes choices that lead to the best situation for themselves, taking into account all available considerations including costs and benefits; the rationality of these considerations is from the point of view of the person themselves, so a decision is not irrational just because someone else finds it questionable.

    In reality, however, there are some factors that affect decision-making abilities and cause people to make irrational decisions – for example, to make contradictory choices when faced with the same problem framed in two different ways (see also Allais paradox).

    Rational decision making is a multi-step process for making choices between alternatives. The process of rational decision making favors logic, objectivity, and analysis over subjectivity and insight. Irrational decision is more counter to logic. The decisions are made in haste and outcomes are not considered.[55]

    One of the most prominent theories of decision making is subjective expected utility (SEU) theory, which describes the rational behavior of the decision maker.[56] The decision maker assesses different alternatives by their utilities and the subjective probability of occurrence.[56]

    Rational decision-making is often grounded on experience and theories that are able to put this approach on solid mathematical grounds so that subjectivity is reduced to a minimum, see e.g. scenario optimization.

    Rational decision is generally seen as the best or most
    likely decision to achieve the set goals or outcome.[57]

    Children, adolescents, and adultsEdit

    ChildrenEdit

    It has been found that, unlike adults, children are less likely to have research strategy behaviors. One such behavior is adaptive decision-making, which is described as funneling and then analyzing the more promising information provided if the number of options to choose from increases. Adaptive decision-making behavior is somewhat present for children, ages 11–12 and older, but decreases in presence the younger they are.[58] The reason children aren’t as fluid in their decision making is because they lack the ability to weigh the cost and effort needed to gather information in the decision-making process. Some possibilities that explain this inability are knowledge deficits and lack of utilization skills. Children lack the metacognitive knowledge necessary to know when to use any strategies they do possess to change their approach to decision-making.[58]

    When it comes to the idea of fairness in decision making, children and adults differ much less. Children are able to understand the concept of fairness in decision making from an early age. Toddlers and infants, ranging from 9–21 months, understand basic principles of equality. The main difference found is that more complex principles of fairness in decision making such as contextual and intentional information don’t come until children get older.[59]

    AdolescentsEdit

    During their adolescent years, teens are known for their high-risk behaviors and rash decisions. Research[60] has shown that there are differences in cognitive processes between adolescents and adults during decision-making. Researchers have concluded that differences in decision-making are not due to a lack of logic or reasoning, but more due to the immaturity of psychosocial capacities that influence decision-making. Examples of their undeveloped capacities which influence decision-making would be impulse control, emotion regulation, delayed gratification and resistance to peer pressure. In the past, researchers have thought that adolescent behavior was simply due to incompetency regarding decision-making. Currently, researchers have concluded that adults and adolescents are both competent decision-makers, not just adults. However, adolescents’ competent decision-making skills decrease when psychosocial capacities become present.

    Research[61] has shown that risk-taking behaviors in adolescents may be the product of interactions between the socioemotional brain network and its cognitive-control network. The socioemotional part of the brain processes social and emotional stimuli and has been shown to be important in reward processing. The cognitive-control network assists in planning and self-regulation. Both of these sections of the brain change over the course of puberty. However, the socioemotional network changes quickly and abruptly, while the cognitive-control network changes more gradually. Because of this difference in change, the cognitive-control network, which usually regulates the socioemotional network, struggles to control the socioemotional network when psychosocial capacities are present.[clarification needed]

    When adolescents are exposed to social and emotional stimuli, their socioemotional network is activated as well as areas of the brain involved in reward processing. Because teens often gain a sense of reward from risk-taking behaviors, their repetition becomes ever more probable due to the reward experienced. In this, the process mirrors addiction. Teens can become addicted to risky behavior because they are in a high state of arousal and are rewarded for it not only by their own internal functions but also by their peers around them. A recent study suggests that adolescents have difficulties adequately adjusting beliefs in response to bad news (such as reading that smoking poses a greater risk to health than they thought), but do not differ from adults in their ability to alter beliefs in response to good news.[62] This creates biased beliefs, which may lead to greater risk taking.[63]

    AdultsEdit

    Adults are generally better able to control their risk-taking because their cognitive-control system has matured enough to the point where it can control the socioemotional network, even in the context of high arousal or when psychosocial capacities are present. Also, adults are less likely to find themselves in situations that push them to do risky things. For example, teens are more likely to be around peers who peer pressure them into doing things, while adults are not as exposed to this sort of social setting.[64][65]

    Cognitive and personal biasesEdit

    Biases usually affect decision-making processes. They appear more when decision task has time pressure, is done under high stress and/or task is highly complex.[66]

    Here is a list of commonly debated biases in judgment and decision-making:

    • Selective search for evidence (also known as confirmation bias): People tend to be willing to gather facts that support certain conclusions but disregard other facts that support different conclusions. Individuals who are highly defensive in this manner show significantly greater left prefrontal cortex activity as measured by EEG than do less defensive individuals.[67]
    • Premature termination of search for evidence: People tend to accept the first alternative that looks like it might work.
    • Cognitive inertia is the unwillingness to change existing thought patterns in the face of new circumstances.
    • Selective perception: People actively screen out information that they do not think is important (see also Prejudice). In one demonstration of this effect, discounting of arguments with which one disagrees (by judging them as untrue or irrelevant) was decreased by selective activation of right prefrontal cortex.[68]
    • Wishful thinking is a tendency to want to see things in a certain – usually positive – light, which can distort perception and thinking.[69]
    • Choice-supportive bias occurs when people distort their memories of chosen and rejected options to make the chosen options seem more attractive.
    • Recency: People tend to place more attention on more recent information and either ignore or forget more distant information (see Semantic priming). The opposite effect in the first set of data or other information is termed primacy effect.[70][page needed]
    • Repetition bias is a willingness to believe what one has been told most often and by the greatest number of different sources.
    • Anchoring and adjustment: Decisions are unduly influenced by initial information that shapes our view of subsequent information.
    • Groupthink is peer pressure to conform to the opinions held by the group.
    • Source credibility bias is a tendency to reject a person’s statement on the basis of a bias against the person, organization, or group to which the person belongs. People preferentially accept statements by others that they like (see also Prejudice).
    • Incremental decision-making and escalating commitment: People look at a decision as a small step in a process, and this tends to perpetuate a series of similar decisions. This can be contrasted with zero-based decision-making (see Slippery slope).
    • Attribution asymmetry: People tend to attribute their own success to internal factors, including abilities and talents, but explain their failures in terms of external factors such as bad luck. The reverse bias is shown when people explain others’ success or failure.
    • Role fulfillment is a tendency to conform to others’ decision-making expectations.
    • Underestimating uncertainty and the illusion of control: People tend to underestimate future uncertainty because of a tendency to believe they have more control over events than they really do.
    • Framing bias: This is best avoided by increasing numeracy and presenting data in several formats (for example, using both absolute and relative scales).[71]
      • Sunk-cost fallacy is a specific type of framing effect that affects decision-making. It involves an individual making a decision about a current situation based on what they have previously invested in the situation.[54]: 372  An example of this would be an individual that is refraining from dropping a class that they are most likely to fail, due to the fact that they feel as though they have done so much work in the course thus far.
    • Prospect theory involves the idea that when faced with a decision-making event, an individual is more likely to take on a risk when evaluating potential losses, and are more likely to avoid risks when evaluating potential gains. This can influence one’s decision-making depending if the situation entails a threat, or opportunity.[54]: 373 
    • Optimism bias is a tendency to overestimate the likelihood of positive events occurring in the future and underestimate the likelihood of negative life events.[72] Such biased expectations are generated and maintained in the face of counter-evidence through a tendency to discount undesirable information.[73] An optimism bias can alter risk perception and decision-making in many domains, ranging from finance to health.
    • Reference class forecasting was developed to eliminate or reduce cognitive biases in decision-making.

    Cognitive limitations in groupsEdit

    In groups, people generate decisions through active and complex processes. One method consists of three steps: initial preferences are expressed by members; the members of the group then gather and share information concerning those preferences; finally, the members combine their views and make a single choice about how to face the problem. Although these steps are relatively ordinary, judgements are often distorted by cognitive and motivational biases, include «sins of commission», «sins of omission», and «sins of imprecision».[74][page needed]

    Cognitive stylesEdit

    Optimizing vs. satisficingEdit

    Herbert A. Simon coined the phrase «bounded rationality» to express the idea that human decision-making is limited by available information, available time and the mind’s information-processing ability. Further psychological research has identified individual differences between two cognitive styles: maximizers try to make an optimal decision, whereas satisficers simply try to find a solution that is «good enough». Maximizers tend to take longer making decisions due to the need to maximize performance across all variables and make tradeoffs carefully; they also tend to more often regret their decisions (perhaps because they are more able than satisficers to recognize that a decision turned out to be sub-optimal).[75]

    Intuitive vs. rationalEdit

    The psychologist Daniel Kahneman, adopting terms originally proposed by the psychologists Keith Stanovich and Richard West, has theorized that a person’s decision-making is the result of an interplay between two kinds of cognitive processes: an automatic intuitive system (called «System 1») and an effortful rational system (called «System 2»). System 1 is a bottom-up, fast, and implicit system of decision-making, while system 2 is a top-down, slow, and explicit system of decision-making.[76] System 1 includes simple heuristics in judgment and decision-making such as the affect heuristic, the availability heuristic, the familiarity heuristic, and the representativeness heuristic.

    Combinatorial vs. positionalEdit

    Styles and methods of decision-making were elaborated by Aron Katsenelinboigen, the founder of predispositioning theory. In his analysis on styles and methods, Katsenelinboigen referred to the game of chess, saying that «chess does disclose various methods of operation, notably the creation of predisposition-methods which may be applicable to other, more complex systems.»[77]: 5 

    Katsenelinboigen states that apart from the methods (reactive and selective) and sub-methods (randomization, predispositioning, programming), there are two major styles: positional and combinational. Both styles are utilized in the game of chess. The two styles reflect two basic approaches to uncertainty: deterministic (combinational style) and indeterministic (positional style). Katsenelinboigen’s definition of the two styles are the following.

    The combinational style is characterized by:

    • a very narrow, clearly defined, primarily material goal; and
    • a program that links the initial position with the outcome.

    In defining the combinational style in chess, Katsenelinboigen wrote: «The combinational style features a clearly formulated limited objective, namely the capture of material (the main constituent element of a chess position). The objective is implemented via a well-defined, and in some cases, unique sequence of moves aimed at reaching the set goal. As a rule, this sequence leaves no options for the opponent. Finding a combinational objective allows the player to focus all his energies on efficient execution, that is, the player’s analysis may be limited to the pieces directly partaking in the combination. This approach is the crux of the combination and the combinational style of play.[77]: 57 

    The positional style is distinguished by:

    • a positional goal; and
    • a formation of semi-complete linkages between the initial step and final outcome.

    «Unlike the combinational player, the positional player is occupied, first and foremost, with the elaboration of the position that will allow him to develop in the unknown future. In playing the positional style, the player must evaluate relational and material parameters as independent variables. … The positional style gives the player the opportunity to develop a position until it becomes pregnant with a combination. However, the combination is not the final goal of the positional player – it helps him to achieve the desirable, keeping in mind a predisposition for the future development. The pyrrhic victory is the best example of one’s inability to think positionally.»[78]

    The positional style serves to:

    • create a predisposition to the future development of the position;
    • induce the environment in a certain way;
    • absorb an unexpected outcome in one’s favor; and
    • avoid the negative aspects of unexpected outcomes.

    Influence of Myers–Briggs typeEdit

    According to Isabel Briggs Myers, a person’s decision-making process depends to a significant degree on their cognitive style.[79][page needed] Myers developed a set of four bi-polar dimensions, called the Myers–Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI). The terminal points on these dimensions are: thinking and feeling; extroversion and introversion; judgment and perception; and sensing and intuition. She claimed that a person’s decision-making style correlates well with how they score on these four dimensions. For example, someone who scored near the thinking, extroversion, sensing, and judgment ends of the dimensions would tend to have a logical, analytical, objective, critical, and empirical decision-making style. However, some psychologists say that the MBTI lacks reliability and validity and is poorly constructed.[80][81]

    Other studies suggest that these national or cross-cultural differences in decision-making exist across entire societies. For example, Maris Martinsons has found that American, Japanese and Chinese business leaders each exhibit a distinctive national style of decision-making.[82]

    The Myers–Briggs typology has been the subject of criticism regarding its poor psychometric properties.[83][84][85]

    General decision-making style (GDMS)Edit

    In the general decision-making style (GDMS) test developed by Suzanne Scott and Reginald Bruce, there are five decision-making styles: rational, intuitive, dependent, avoidant, and spontaneous.[86][87] These five different decision-making styles change depending on the context and situation, and one style is not necessarily better than any other. In the examples below, the individual is working for a company and is offered a job from a different company.

    • The rational style is an in-depth search for, and a strong consideration of, other options and/or information prior to making a decision. In this style, the individual would research the new job being offered, review their current job, and look at the pros and cons of taking the new job versus staying with their current company.
    • The intuitive style is confidence in one’s initial feelings and gut reactions. In this style, if the individual initially prefers the new job because they have a feeling that the work environment is better suited for them, then they would decide to take the new job. The individual might not make this decision as soon as the job is offered.
    • The dependent style is asking for other people’s input and instructions on what decision should be made. In this style, the individual could ask friends, family, coworkers, etc., but the individual might not ask all of these people.
    • The avoidant style is averting the responsibility of making a decision. In this style, the individual would not make a decision. Therefore, the individual would stick with their current job.
    • The spontaneous style is a need to make a decision as soon as possible rather than waiting to make a decision. In this style, the individual would either reject or accept the job as soon as it is offered.

    Organizational vs. individual levelEdit

    There are a few characteristics that differentiate organizational decision-making from individual decision-making as studied in lab experiments:[88]

    1. Unlike most lab studies of individual decision-making, ambiguity is pervasive in organizations. There is often only ambiguous information, and there is ambiguity about preferences as well as about interpreting the history of decisions.
    2. Decision-making in and by organizations is embedded in a longitudinal context, meaning that participants in organizational decision-making are a part of ongoing processes. Even if they don’t take on active roles in all phases of decision-making, they are part of the Decision Process and its consequences. Decisions in organizations are made in a sequential manner, and commitment may be more important in such processes than judgmental accuracy. In contrast, most lab studies of individual decision-making are conducted in artificial settings (lab) that are not connected to the subjects’ ongoing activities.
    3. Incentives play an important role in organizational decision-making. Incentives, penalties, and their ramifications are real and may have long-lasting effects. These effects are intensified due to the longitudinal nature of decision-making in organizational settings. Incentives and penalties are very salient in organizations, and often they command managerial attention.
    4. Many executives, especially in middle management, may make repeated decisions on similar issues. Managers may develop a sense of using his/her skills (which may be faulty) and a sense of having control and using one’s skills are pervasive in managerial thinking about risk taking. Several repeated decisions are made by following rules rather than by using pure information processing modes.
    5. Conflict is pervasive in organizational decision-making. Many times power considerations and agenda setting determine decisions rather than calculations based on the decision’s parameters. The nature of authority relations may have a large impact on the way decisions are made in organizations, which are basically political systems.

    See alsoEdit

    • Aboulomania
    • Adaptive performance
    • Agent (economics)
    • Analytic hierarchy process
    • Argument map
    • Business decision mapping
    • Choice architecture
    • Choice modelling
    • Concept driven strategy
    • Ordinal Priority Approach
    • Decision downloading
    • Decision fatigue
    • Decision quality
    • Decision-making software
    • Decision-making unit
    • Emotional choice theory
    • Foresight (psychology)
    • Framing (social sciences)
    • Free will
    • Idea networking
    • Let Simon Decide
    • Rational choice theory
    • Robust decision

    ReferencesEdit

    1. ^ Herbert Alexander Simon (1977). The New Science of Management Decision. Prentice-Hall. ISBN 978-0136161448.
    2. ^ Frensch, Peter A.; Funke, Joachim, eds. (1995). Complex problem solving: the European perspective. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. ISBN 978-0805813364. OCLC 32131412.
    3. ^ Brockmann, Erich N.; Anthony, William P. (December 2016). «Tacit knowledge and strategic decision making». Group & Organization Management. 27 (4): 436–455. doi:10.1177/1059601102238356. S2CID 145110719.
    4. ^ Kahneman, Daniel; Tversky, Amos, eds. (2000). Choices, values, and frames. New York; Cambridge, UK: Russell Sage Foundation; Cambridge University Press. p. 211. ISBN 978-0521621724. OCLC 42934579.
    5. ^ Triantaphyllou, Evangelos (2000). Multi-criteria decision making methods: a comparative study. Applied optimization. Vol. 44. Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic Publishers. p. 320. doi:10.1007/978-1-4757-3157-6. ISBN 978-0792366072.
    6. ^ Klein, Gary (2008). «Naturalistic Decision Making». Human Factors: The Journal of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society. 50 (3): 456–460. doi:10.1518/001872008×288385. ISSN 0018-7208. PMID 18689053. S2CID 11251289.
    7. ^ a b Davidson, Alice Ware; Bar-Yam, Yaneer (2006) [2000]. «Environmental complexity: information for human–environment well-being» (PDF). In Bar-Yam, Yaneer; Minai, Ali (eds.). Unifying themes in complex systems. Berlin; New York: Springer. pp. 157–168. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.33.7118. doi:10.1007/978-3-540-35866-4_16. ISBN 978-3540358640.
    8. ^ Godfrey-Smith, Peter (2001). «Environmental complexity and the evolution of cognition» (PDF). In Sternberg, Robert J.; Kaufman, James C. (eds.). The evolution of intelligence. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 223–250. ISBN 978-0805832679. OCLC 44775038.
    9. ^ Kepner, Charles Higgins; Tregoe, Benjamin B. (1997) [1965]. The new rational manager: an updated edition for a new world (Updated ed.). Princeton, NJ: Princeton Research Press. OCLC 37666447.
    10. ^ Monahan, George E. (2000). Management decision making: spreadsheet modeling, analysis, and application. Cambridge, UK; New York: Cambridge University Press. pp. 33–40. ISBN 978-0521781183. OCLC 42921287.
    11. ^ Armstrong, Jon Scott (2001). «Role playing: a method to forecast decisions». In Armstrong, Jon Scott (ed.). Principles of forecasting: a handbook for researchers and practitioners. International series in operations research & management science. Vol. 30. Boston, MA: Kluwer Academic Publishers. pp. 15–30. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.464.5677. doi:10.1007/978-0-306-47630-3_2. ISBN 978-0792379300.
    12. ^ «analysis paralysis | Definition of analysis paralysis in US English by Oxford Dictionaries». Oxford Dictionaries | English. Archived from the original on January 7, 2018. Retrieved 2018-11-10.
    13. ^ «Analysis Paralysis | Definition of Analysis Paralysis by Lexico». Lexico Dictionaries | English. Archived from the original on July 29, 2020. Retrieved 2020-04-09.
    14. ^ «Avoid Analysis Paralysis—Use Data to Enable Decision-Making and Growth». TechNative. 2019-03-06. Retrieved 2020-04-09.
    15. ^ Roberts, Lon (2010). Analysis paralysis: a case of terminological inexactitude. Defense AT&L. pp. 21–22.
    16. ^ a b «Between ‘Paralysis by analysis’ and ‘Extinction by instinct’«. Long Range Planning. 28 (4): 127. August 1995. doi:10.1016/0024-6301(95)94294-9. ISSN 0024-6301.
    17. ^ Hart, Paul’t (June 1991). «Irving L. Janis’ Victims of Groupthink». Political Psychology. 12 (2): 247–278. doi:10.2307/3791464. JSTOR 3791464.
    18. ^ Paul Saffo quoted in: Foley, John (30 October 1995). «Managing information: infoglut». InformationWeek. Archived from the original on 2001-02-22. Retrieved 2015-07-26.
    19. ^ Duncan (1972). «Characteristics of organizational environments and perceived environment uncertainty». Administrative Science Quarterly. 17 (3): 313–27. doi:10.2307/2392145. JSTOR 2392145.
    20. ^ Kutty, Ambalika D.; Kumar Shee, Himanshu; Pathak, R. D. (November 2007). «Decision-making: too much info!». Monash Business Review. 3 (3): 8–9. doi:10.2104/mbr07056.
    21. ^ Miller, George A. (1956). «The magical number seven, plus or minus two: some limits on our capacity for processing information». Psychological Review. 63 (2): 81–97. doi:10.1037/h0043158. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-002C-4646-B. ISSN 1939-1471. PMID 13310704. S2CID 15654531.
    22. ^ Hall, Crystal C.; Ariss, Lynn; Todorov, Alexander (July 2007). «The illusion of knowledge: when more information reduces accuracy and increases confidence» (PDF). Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 103 (2): 277–290. doi:10.1016/j.obhdp.2007.01.003.
    23. ^ «Enemy of the good». Nature. 503 (7477): 438. November 2013. doi:10.1038/503438a. ISSN 0028-0836. PMID 24298564.
    24. ^ Chamorro-Premuzic, Tomas; Furnham, Adrian (2014-04-08). Personality and Intellectual Competence. doi:10.4324/9781410612649. ISBN 978-1410612649.
    25. ^ «Richard Saul Wurman: Information, Mapping, and Understanding», Architectural Intelligence, The MIT Press, 2017, doi:10.7551/mitpress/10971.003.0004, ISBN 978-0262343428
    26. ^ Buckland, Michael. Information and society. Cambridge, Massachusetts. ISBN 978-0262339544. OCLC 978295031.
    27. ^ Szalavitz, Maia (2011-08-23). «Mind over Mind? Decision Fatigue Saps Willpower — if We Let It». Time. ISSN 0040-781X. Retrieved 2020-04-09.
    28. ^ McSweeney, Alan (2019-05-21), Stopping Analysis Paralysis And Decision Avoidance In Business Analysis And Solution Design, doi:10.13140/RG.2.2.21841.38243
    29. ^ Walton, Mark E.; Devlin, Joseph T.; Rushworth, Matthew F. S. (November 2004). «Interactions between decision making and performance monitoring within prefrontal cortex». Nature Neuroscience. 7 (11): 1259–1265. doi:10.1038/nn1339. PMID 15494729. S2CID 26711881.
    30. ^ Damasio, Antonio R. (1994). Descartes’ error: emotion, reason, and the human brain. New York: Putnam. ISBN 978-0399138942. OCLC 30780083.
    31. ^ Gold, Joshua I.; Shadlen, Michael N. (2007). «The neural basis of decision making». Annual Review of Neuroscience. 30: 535–574. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113038. PMID 17600525.
    32. ^ Kiani, Roozbeh; Shadlen, Michael N. (May 2009). «Representation of confidence associated with a decision by neurons in the parietal cortex». Science. 324 (5928): 759–764. Bibcode:2009Sci…324..759K. doi:10.1126/science.1169405. PMC 2738936. PMID 19423820.
    33. ^ Brunton, Bingni W.; Botvinick, Matthew M.; Brody, Carlos D. (April 2013). «Rats and humans can optimally accumulate evidence for decision-making» (PDF). Science. 340 (6128): 95–98. Bibcode:2013Sci…340…95B. doi:10.1126/science.1233912. PMID 23559254. S2CID 13098239. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2016-03-05.
    34. ^ Kennerley, Steven W.; Walton, Mark E.; Behrens, Timothy E. J.; Buckley, Mark J.; Rushworth, Matthew F. S. (July 2006). «Optimal decision making and the anterior cingulate cortex». Nature Neuroscience. 9 (7): 940–947. doi:10.1038/nn1724. PMID 16783368. S2CID 8868406.
    35. ^ Hunt, L. T.; Daw, N. D.; Kaanders, P.; MacIver, M. A.; Mugan, U.; Procyk, E.; Redish, A. D.; Russo, E.; Scholl, J.; Stachenfeld, K.; Wilson, C. R. E.; Kolling, N. (21 June 2021). «Formalizing planning and information search in naturalistic decision-making» (PDF). Nature Neuroscience. 24 (8): 1051–1064. doi:10.1038/s41593-021-00866-w. PMID 34155400. S2CID 235596957.
    36. ^ Reimann, Martin; Bechara, Antoine (October 2010). «The somatic marker framework as a neurological theory of decision-making: review, conceptual comparisons, and future neuroeconomics research». Journal of Economic Psychology. 31 (5): 767–776. doi:10.1016/j.joep.2010.03.002.
    37. ^ Naqvi, Nasir; Shiv, Baba; Bechara, Antoine (October 2006). «The role of emotion in decision making: a cognitive neuroscience perspective». Current Directions in Psychological Science. 15 (5): 260–264. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.137.4677. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2006.00448.x. S2CID 14789591.
    38. ^ Barbey, Aron K.; Colom, Roberto; Grafman, Jordan (March 2014). «Distributed neural system for emotional intelligence revealed by lesion mapping». Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 9 (3): 265–272. doi:10.1093/scan/nss124. PMC 3980800. PMID 23171618.
    39. ^ Yates, Diana. «Researchers map emotional intelligence in the brain». University of Illinois News Bureau. University of Illinois.
    40. ^ HealthDay (2013-01-28). «Scientists complete 1st map of ’emotional intelligence’ in the brain». U.S. News & World Report.
    41. ^ Verma, Dem (2009). DECISION MAKING STYLE: Social and Creative Dimensions. New Delhi: Global India Publications Pvt Ltd. p. 43. ISBN 978-9380228303.
    42. ^ Landeta, Jon (2006-06-01). «Current validity of the Delphi method in social sciences». Technological Forecasting and Social Change. 73 (5): 467–482. doi:10.1016/j.techfore.2005.09.002. ISSN 0040-1625.
    43. ^ Diceman, Jason (2010). Dotmocracy Handbook. Jason Diceman. pp. 1–2. ISBN 978-1451527087.
    44. ^ Franklin, Benjamin (1975) [1772]. «To Joseph Priestley». In Willcox, William Bradford (ed.). The papers of Benjamin Franklin: January 1 through December 31, 1772. Vol. 19. New Haven: Yale University Press. pp. 299–300. ISBN 978-0300018653. OCLC 310601.
    45. ^ Mann, Leon; Harmoni, Ros; Power, Colin (1991). «The GOFER course in decision making». In Baron, Jonathan; Brown, Rex V. (eds.). Teaching decision making to adolescents. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. pp. 61–78. ISBN 978-0805804973. OCLC 22507012. See also: Mann, Leon (July 1989). «Becoming a better decision maker». Australian Psychologist. 24 (2): 141–155. doi:10.1080/00050068908259558.
    46. ^ Janis, Irving L.; Mann, Leon (1977). Decision making: a psychological analysis of conflict, choice, and commitment. New York: Free Press. ISBN 978-0029161609. OCLC 2542340.
    47. ^ Mann, Leon; Harmoni, Ros; Power, Colin; Beswick, Gery; Ormond, Cheryl (July 1988). «Effectiveness of the GOFER course in decision making for high school students». Journal of Behavioral Decision Making. 1 (3): 159–168. doi:10.1002/bdm.3960010304.
    48. ^ Brown, Pam (November 29, 2007), Career coach: decision-making, Pulse, retrieved July 12, 2012 (subscription required)
    49. ^ Guo, Kristina L. (June 2008). «DECIDE: a decision-making model for more effective decision making by health care managers». The Health Care Manager. 27 (2): 118–127. doi:10.1097/01.HCM.0000285046.27290.90. PMID 18475113. S2CID 24492631.
    50. ^ Pijanowski, John (February 2009). «The role of learning theory in building effective college ethics curricula». Journal of College and Character. 10 (3): 1–13. doi:10.2202/1940-1639.1088.
    51. ^ Griffin, Emory A. (1991). «Interact system model of decision emergence of B. Aubrey Fisher» (PDF). A first look at communication theory (1st ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. pp. 253–262. ISBN 978-0070227781. OCLC 21973427.
    52. ^ Postmes, T; Spears, Russell; Cihangir, Sezgin (2001). «Quality of decision making and group norms». Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 80 (6): 918–930. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.80.6.918. PMID 11414374.
    53. ^ Brockmann, E.; Anthony, W. (2002). «Tacit knowledge and strategic decision making». Group & Organization Management. 27 (4): 436–455. doi:10.1177/1059601102238356. S2CID 145110719.
    54. ^ a b c Schacter, Daniel L.; Gilbert, Daniel Todd; Wegner, Daniel M. (2011) [2009]. Psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Worth Publishers. ISBN 978-1429237192. OCLC 755079969.
    55. ^ Boundless. (n.d.). Boundless Management. Retrieved December 11, 2020, from https://courses.lumenlearning.com/boundless-management/chapter/rational-and-nonrational-decision-making/
    56. ^ a b Crozier, W. Ray; Ranyard, Rob (1997). «Cognitive process models and explanations of decision making». In Ranyard, Rob; Crozier, W. Ray; Svenson, Ola (eds.). Decision making: cognitive models and explanations. Frontiers of cognitive science. London; New York: Routledge. pp. 5–20. ISBN 978-0415158183. OCLC 37043834.
    57. ^ Djulbegovic, B. (2017) Rational decision making in medicine: Implications for
      Overuse and Underuse
    58. ^ a b Gregan‐Paxton, Jennifer; John, Deborah Roedder (June 1997). «The Emergence of Adaptive Decision Making in Children». Journal of Consumer Research. 24 (1): 43–56. doi:10.1086/209492. ISSN 0093-5301.
    59. ^ Jaroslawska, Agnieszka J.; McCormack, Teresa; Burns, Patrick; Caruso, Eugene M. (January 2020). «Outcomes versus intentions in fairness-related decision making: School-aged children’s decisions are just like those of adults». Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 189: 104704. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2019.104704. ISSN 0022-0965. PMID 31634734.
    60. ^ Steinberg, Laurence (March 2008). «A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking». Developmental Review. 28 (1): 78–106. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002. PMC 2396566. PMID 18509515.
    61. ^ Steinberg, Laurence (March 2008). «A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking». Developmental Review. 28 (1): 78–106. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002. PMC 2396566. PMID 18509515.
    62. ^ Moutsiana, Christina; Garrett, Neil; Clarke, Richard C.; Lotto, R. Beau; Blakemore, Sarah-Jayne; Sharot, Tali (October 2013). «Human development of the ability to learn from bad news». Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 110 (41): 16396–16401. Bibcode:2013PNAS..11016396M. doi:10.1073/pnas.1305631110. PMC 3799330. PMID 24019466.
    63. ^ Reyna, Valerie F. (November 2013). «Psychology: Good and bad news on the adolescent brain». Nature. 503 (7474): 48–49. Bibcode:2013Natur.503…48R. doi:10.1038/nature12704. PMID 24172899. S2CID 205236138.
    64. ^ Gardner, Margo; Steinberg, Laurence (July 2005). «Peer influence on risk taking, risk preference, and risky decision making in adolescence and adulthood: an experimental study» (PDF). Developmental Psychology. 41 (4): 625–635. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.556.4973. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.41.4.625. PMID 16060809.
    65. ^ Steinberg, Laurence (April 2007). «Risk taking in adolescence: new perspectives from brain and behavioral science». Current Directions in Psychological Science. 16 (2): 55–59. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.519.7099. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2007.00475.x. S2CID 18601508.
    66. ^ T, Maqsood; A, Finegan; D, Walker (2004). «Biases and heuristics in judgment and decision making: The dark side of tacit knowledge». Issues in Informing Science and Information Technology. 1: 0295–0301. doi:10.28945/740. ISSN 1547-5840.
    67. ^ Blackhart, G. C.; Kline, J. P. (2005). «Individual differences in anterior EEG asymmetry between high and low defensive individuals during a rumination/distraction task». Personality and Individual Differences. 39 (2): 427–437. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2005.01.027.
    68. ^ Drake, R. A. (1993). «Processing persuasive arguments: 2. Discounting of truth and relevance as a function of agreement and manipulated activation asymmetry». Journal of Research in Personality. 27 (2): 184–196. doi:10.1006/jrpe.1993.1013.
    69. ^ Chua, E. F.; Rand-Giovannetti, E.; Schacter, D. L.; Albert, M.; Sperling, R. A. (2004). «Dissociating confidence and accuracy: Functional magnetic resonance imaging shows origins of the subjective memory experience» (PDF). Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 16 (7): 1131–1142. doi:10.1162/0898929041920568. PMID 15453969. S2CID 215728618.
    70. ^ Plous, Scott (1993). The psychology of judgment and decision making. Philadelphia: Temple University Press. ISBN 978-0877229131. OCLC 26548229.
    71. ^ Perneger, Thomas V.; Agoritsas, Thomas (December 2011). «Doctors and patients’ susceptibility to framing bias: a randomized trial». Journal of General Internal Medicine. 26 (12): 1411–1417. doi:10.1007/s11606-011-1810-x. PMC 3235613. PMID 21792695.
    72. ^ Sharot, Tali (2011). The optimism bias: a tour of the irrationally positive brain (1st ed.). New York: Pantheon Books. ISBN 978-0307378484. OCLC 667609433.
    73. ^ Sharot, Tali; Korn, Christoph W.; Dolan, Raymond J. (October 2011). «How unrealistic optimism is maintained in the face of reality». Nature Neuroscience. 14 (11): 1475–1479. doi:10.1038/nn.2949. PMC 3204264. PMID 21983684.
    74. ^ Forsyth, Donelson R. (2014) [1983]. Group dynamics (6th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth Cengage Learning. ISBN 978-1133956532. OCLC 826872491.
    75. ^ Sparks, Erin (2007). «Satisficing». In Baumeister, Roy F.; Vohs, Kathleen D. (eds.). Encyclopedia of social psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications. pp. 776–778. ISBN 978-1412916707. OCLC 123119782.
    76. ^ Kahneman, Daniel (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York: Farrar, Straus, and Giroux. ISBN 978-0374275631. OCLC 706020998.
    77. ^ a b Katsenelinboigen, Aron (1997). The concept of indeterminism and its applications: economics, social systems, ethics, artificial intelligence, and aesthetics (PDF). Westport, CT: Praeger. ISBN 978-0275957889. OCLC 36438766. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2011-07-23. Retrieved 2015-07-27.
    78. ^ Ulea, Vera (2002). A concept of dramatic genre and the comedy of a new type: chess, literature, and film. Carbondale: Southern Illinois University Press. pp. 17–18. ISBN 978-0809324521. OCLC 51301095.
    79. ^ Myers, Isabel Briggs; Kirby, Linda K.; Myers, Katharine D. (1998) [1976]. Introduction to type: a guide to understanding your results on the Myers–Briggs Type Indicator. Introduction to type series (6th ed.). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press. OCLC 40336039.
    80. ^ Pittenger, David J. (2005). «Cautionary comments regarding the Myers–Briggs Type Indicator». Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research. 57 (3): 210–221. doi:10.1037/1065-9293.57.3.210.
    81. ^ Hogan, Robert (2007). Personality and the fate of organizations. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. p. 28. ISBN 978-0805841428. OCLC 65400436. Most personality psychologists regard the MBTI as little more than an elaborate Chinese fortune cookie…
    82. ^ Martinsons, Maris G. (December 2006). «Comparing the decision styles of American, Chinese and Japanese business leaders». Best Paper Proceedings of Academy of Management Meetings, Washington, DC, August 2001. SSRN 952292.
    83. ^ Pittenger, David (1993). «Measuring the MBTI … And Coming Up Short» (PDF). Journal of Career Planning and Employment. 54 (1): 48–52.
    84. ^ Schuwirth, Lambert; Cantillon, Peter (2004-05-22). «What the educators are saying». BMJ. 328 (7450): 1244. doi:10.1136/bmj.328.7450.1244. ISSN 0959-8138.
    85. ^ Pittenger, David J. (2005). «Cautionary comments regarding the Myers–Briggs Type Indicator». Consulting Psychology Journal: Practice and Research. 57 (3): 210–221. doi:10.1037/1065-9293.57.3.210. ISSN 1939-0149.
    86. ^ Scott, Susanne G.; Bruce, Reginald A. (1995). «Decision-making style: the development and assessment of a new measure». Educational and Psychological Measurement. 55 (5): 818–831. doi:10.1177/0013164495055005017. S2CID 143479230.
    87. ^ Thunholm, Peter (March 2004). «Decision-making style: habit, style or both?». Personality and Individual Differences. 36 (4): 931–944. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(03)00162-4.
    88. ^ Shapira, Z. (2002). «Organizational Decision Making. Cambridge Series on Judgment and Decision Making», Cambridge University Press: pp. 4–6. ISBN 978-0521890502

    Понравилась статья? Поделить с друзьями:
  • Is debts a word
  • Is dears a real word
  • Is dustier a word
  • Is everyday one word or two words
  • Is dusked a word