History of the word psychology

«Psychological» redirects here. For the Pet Shop Boys song, see Psychological (song).

Psychology is the scientific study of mind and behavior in humans and non-humans. Psychology includes the study of conscious and unconscious phenomena, including feelings and thoughts. It is an academic discipline of immense scope, crossing the boundaries between the natural and social sciences. Psychologists seek an understanding of the emergent properties of brains, linking the discipline to neuroscience. As social scientists, psychologists aim to understand the behavior of individuals and groups.[1][2] Ψ (psi), the first letter of the Greek word psyche from which the term psychology is derived (see below), is commonly associated with the science.

A professional practitioner or researcher involved in the discipline is called a psychologist. Some psychologists can also be classified as behavioral or cognitive scientists. Some psychologists attempt to understand the role of mental functions in individual and social behavior. Others explore the physiological and neurobiological processes that underlie cognitive functions and behaviors.

Psychologists are involved in research on perception, cognition, attention, emotion, intelligence, subjective experiences, motivation, brain functioning, and personality. Psychologists’ interests extend to interpersonal relationships, psychological resilience, family resilience, and other areas within social psychology. They also consider the unconscious mind.[3] Research psychologists employ empirical methods to infer causal and correlational relationships between psychosocial variables. Some, but not all, clinical and counseling psychologists rely on symbolic interpretation.

While psychological knowledge is often applied to the assessment and treatment of mental health problems, it is also directed towards understanding and solving problems in several spheres of human activity. By many accounts, psychology ultimately aims to benefit society.[4][5][6] Many psychologists are involved in some kind of therapeutic role, practicing psychotherapy in clinical, counseling, or school settings. Other psychologists conduct scientific research on a wide range of topics related to mental processes and behavior. Typically the latter group of psychologists work in academic settings (e.g., universities, medical schools, or hospitals). Another group of psychologists is employed in industrial and organizational settings.[7] Yet others are involved in work on human development, aging, sports, health, forensic science, education, and the media.

Etymology and definitions

The word psychology derives from the Greek word psyche, for spirit or soul. The latter part of the word «psychology» derives from -λογία -logia, which refers to «study» or «research».[8] The Latin word psychologia was first used by the Croatian humanist and Latinist Marko Marulić in his book, Psichiologia de ratione animae humanae (Psychology, on the Nature of the Human Soul) in the late 15th century or early 16th century.[9] The earliest known reference to the word psychology in English was by Steven Blankaart in 1694 in The Physical Dictionary. The dictionary refers to «Anatomy, which treats the Body, and Psychology, which treats of the Soul.»[10]

In 1890, William James defined psychology as «the science of mental life, both of its phenomena and their conditions.»[11] This definition enjoyed widespread currency for decades. However, this meaning was contested, notably by radical behaviorists such as John B. Watson, who in 1913 asserted that the discipline is a «natural science», the theoretical goal of which «is the prediction and control of behavior.»[12] Since James defined «psychology», the term more strongly implicates scientific experimentation.[13][12] Folk psychology refers to ordinary people’s, as contrasted with psychology professionals’, understanding of the mental states and behaviors of people.[14]

History

The ancient civilizations of Egypt, Greece, China, India, and Persia all engaged in the philosophical study of psychology. In Ancient Egypt the Ebers Papyrus mentioned depression and thought disorders.[15] Historians note that Greek philosophers, including Thales, Plato, and Aristotle (especially in his De Anima treatise),[16] addressed the workings of the mind.[17] As early as the 4th century BC, the Greek physician Hippocrates theorized that mental disorders had physical rather than supernatural causes.[18] In 387 BCE, Plato suggested that the brain is where mental processes take place, and in 335 BCE Aristotle suggested that it was the heart.[19]

In China, psychological understanding grew from the philosophical works of Laozi and Confucius, and later from the doctrines of Buddhism. This body of knowledge involves insights drawn from introspection and observation, as well as techniques for focused thinking and acting. It frames the universe in term of a division of physical reality and mental reality as well as the interaction between the physical and the mental. Chinese philosophy also emphasized purifying the mind in order to increase virtue and power. An ancient text known as The Yellow Emperor’s Classic of Internal Medicine identifies the brain as the nexus of wisdom and sensation, includes theories of personality based on yin–yang balance, and analyzes mental disorder in terms of physiological and social disequilibria. Chinese scholarship that focused on the brain advanced during the Qing Dynasty with the work of Western-educated Fang Yizhi (1611–1671), Liu Zhi (1660–1730), and Wang Qingren (1768–1831). Wang Qingren emphasized the importance of the brain as the center of the nervous system, linked mental disorder with brain diseases, investigated the causes of dreams and insomnia, and advanced a theory of hemispheric lateralization in brain function.[20]

Influenced by Hinduism, Indian philosophy explored distinctions in types of awareness. A central idea of the Upanishads and other Vedic texts that formed the foundations of Hinduism was the distinction between a person’s transient mundane self and their eternal, unchanging soul. Divergent Hindu doctrines and Buddhism have challenged this hierarchy of selves, but have all emphasized the importance of reaching higher awareness. Yoga encompasses a range of techniques used in pursuit of this goal. Theosophy, a religion established by Russian-American philosopher Helena Blavatsky, drew inspiration from these doctrines during her time in British India.[21][22]

Psychology was of interest to Enlightenment thinkers in Europe. In Germany, Gottfried Wilhelm Leibniz (1646–1716) applied his principles of calculus to the mind, arguing that mental activity took place on an indivisible continuum. He suggested that the difference between conscious and unconscious awareness is only a matter of degree. Christian Wolff identified psychology as its own science, writing Psychologia Empirica in 1732 and Psychologia Rationalis in 1734. Immanuel Kant advanced the idea of anthropology as a discipline, with psychology an important subdivision. Kant, however, explicitly rejected the idea of an experimental psychology, writing that «the empirical doctrine of the soul can also never approach chemistry even as a systematic art of analysis or experimental doctrine, for in it the manifold of inner observation can be separated only by mere division in thought, and cannot then be held separate and recombined at will (but still less does another thinking subject suffer himself to be experimented upon to suit our purpose), and even observation by itself already changes and displaces the state of the observed object.» In 1783, Ferdinand Ueberwasser (1752–1812) designated himself Professor of Empirical Psychology and Logic and gave lectures on scientific psychology, though these developments were soon overshadowed by the Napoleonic Wars.[23] At the end of the Napoleonic era, Prussian authorities discontinued the Old University of Münster.[23] Having consulted philosophers Hegel and Herbart, however, in 1825 the Prussian state established psychology as a mandatory discipline in its rapidly expanding and highly influential educational system. However, this discipline did not yet embrace experimentation.[24] In England, early psychology involved phrenology and the response to social problems including alcoholism, violence, and the country’s crowded «lunatic» asylums.[25]

Beginning of experimental psychology

Wilhelm Wundt (seated) with colleagues in his psychological laboratory, the first of its kind

Philosopher John Stuart Mill believed that the human mind was open to scientific investigation, even if the science is in some ways inexact.[26] Mill proposed a «mental chemistry» in which elementary thoughts could combine into ideas of greater complexity.[26] Gustav Fechner began conducting psychophysics research in Leipzig in the 1830s. He articulated the principle that human perception of a stimulus varies logarithmically according to its intensity.[27]: 61  The principle became known as the Weber–Fechner law. Fechner’s 1860 Elements of Psychophysics challenged Kant’s negative view with regard to conducting quantitative research on the mind.[28][24] Fechner’s achievement was to show that «mental processes could not only be given numerical magnitudes, but also that these could be measured by experimental methods.»[24] In Heidelberg, Hermann von Helmholtz conducted parallel research on sensory perception, and trained physiologist Wilhelm Wundt. Wundt, in turn, came to Leipzig University, where he established the psychological laboratory that brought experimental psychology to the world. Wundt focused on breaking down mental processes into the most basic components, motivated in part by an analogy to recent advances in chemistry, and its successful investigation of the elements and structure of materials.[29] Paul Flechsig and Emil Kraepelin soon created another influential laboratory at Leipzig, a psychology-related lab, that focused more on experimental psychiatry.[24]

The German psychologist Hermann Ebbinghaus, a researcher at the University of Berlin, was another 19th-century contributor to the field. He pioneered the experimental study of memory and developed quantitative models of learning and forgetting.[30] In the early twentieth century, Wolfgang Kohler, Max Wertheimer, and Kurt Koffka co-founded the school of Gestalt psychology (not to be confused with the Gestalt therapy of Fritz Perls). The approach of Gestalt psychology is based upon the idea that individuals experience things as unified wholes. Rather than reducing thoughts and behavior into smaller component elements, as in structuralism, the Gestaltists maintained that whole of experience is important, and differs from the sum of its parts.

Psychologists in Germany, Denmark, Austria, England, and the United States soon followed Wundt in setting up laboratories.[31] G. Stanley Hall, an American who studied with Wundt, founded a psychology lab that became internationally influential. The lab was located at Johns Hopkins University. Hall, in turn, trained Yujiro Motora, who brought experimental psychology, emphasizing psychophysics, to the Imperial University of Tokyo.[32] Wundt’s assistant, Hugo Münsterberg, taught psychology at Harvard to students such as Narendra Nath Sen Gupta—who, in 1905, founded a psychology department and laboratory at the University of Calcutta.[21] Wundt’s students Walter Dill Scott, Lightner Witmer, and James McKeen Cattell worked on developing tests of mental ability. Cattell, who also studied with eugenicist Francis Galton, went on to found the Psychological Corporation. Witmer focused on the mental testing of children; Scott, on employee selection.[27]: 60 

Another student of Wundt, the Englishman Edward Titchener, created the psychology program at Cornell University and advanced «structuralist» psychology. The idea behind structuralism was to analyze and classify different aspects of the mind, primarily through the method of introspection.[33] William James, John Dewey, and Harvey Carr advanced the idea of functionalism, an expansive approach to psychology that underlined the Darwinian idea of a behavior’s usefulness to the individual. In 1890, James wrote an influential book, The Principles of Psychology, which expanded on the structuralism. He memorably described «stream of consciousness.» James’s ideas interested many American students in the emerging discipline.[33][11][27]: 178–82  Dewey integrated psychology with societal concerns, most notably by promoting progressive education, inculcating moral values in children, and assimilating immigrants.[27]: 196–200 

A different strain of experimentalism, with a greater connection to physiology, emerged in South America, under the leadership of Horacio G. Piñero at the University of Buenos Aires.[34] In Russia, too, researchers placed greater emphasis on the biological basis for psychology, beginning with Ivan Sechenov’s 1873 essay, «Who Is to Develop Psychology and How?» Sechenov advanced the idea of brain reflexes and aggressively promoted a deterministic view of human behavior.[35] The Russian-Soviet physiologist Ivan Pavlov discovered in dogs a learning process that was later termed «classical conditioning» and applied the process to human beings.[36]

Consolidation and funding

One of the earliest psychology societies was La Société de Psychologie Physiologique in France, which lasted from 1885 to 1893. The first meeting of the International Congress of Psychology sponsored by the International Union of Psychological Science took place in Paris, in August 1889, amidst the World’s Fair celebrating the centennial of the French Revolution. William James was one of three Americans among the 400 attendees. The American Psychological Association (APA) was founded soon after, in 1892. The International Congress continued to be held at different locations in Europe and with wide international participation. The Sixth Congress, held in Geneva in 1909, included presentations in Russian, Chinese, and Japanese, as well as Esperanto. After a hiatus for World War I, the Seventh Congress met in Oxford, with substantially greater participation from the war-victorious Anglo-Americans. In 1929, the Congress took place at Yale University in New Haven, Connecticut, attended by hundreds of members of the APA.[31] Tokyo Imperial University led the way in bringing new psychology to the East. New ideas about psychology diffused from Japan into China.[20][32]

American psychology gained status upon the U.S.’s entry into World War I. A standing committee headed by Robert Yerkes administered mental tests («Army Alpha» and «Army Beta») to almost 1.8 million soldiers.[37] Subsequently, the Rockefeller family, via the Social Science Research Council, began to provide funding for behavioral research.[38][39] Rockefeller charities funded the National Committee on Mental Hygiene, which disseminated the concept of mental illness and lobbied for applying ideas from psychology to child rearing.[37][40] Through the Bureau of Social Hygiene and later funding of Alfred Kinsey, Rockefeller foundations helped establish research on sexuality in the U.S.[41] Under the influence of the Carnegie-funded Eugenics Record Office, the Draper-funded Pioneer Fund, and other institutions, the eugenics movement also influenced American psychology. In the 1910s and 1920s, eugenics became a standard topic in psychology classes.[42] In contrast to the US, in the UK psychology was met with antagonism by the scientific and medical establishments, and up until 1939, there were only six psychology chairs in universities in England.[43]

During World War II and the Cold War, the U.S. military and intelligence agencies established themselves as leading funders of psychology by way of the armed forces and in the new Office of Strategic Services intelligence agency. University of Michigan psychologist Dorwin Cartwright reported that university researchers began large-scale propaganda research in 1939–1941. He observed that «the last few months of the war saw a social psychologist become chiefly responsible for determining the week-by-week-propaganda policy for the United States Government.» Cartwright also wrote that psychologists had significant roles in managing the domestic economy.[44] The Army rolled out its new General Classification Test to assess the ability of millions of soldiers. The Army also engaged in large-scale psychological research of troop morale and mental health.[45] In the 1950s, the Rockefeller Foundation and Ford Foundation collaborated with the Central Intelligence Agency (CIA) to fund research on psychological warfare.[46] In 1965, public controversy called attention to the Army’s Project Camelot, the «Manhattan Project» of social science, an effort which enlisted psychologists and anthropologists to analyze the plans and policies of foreign countries for strategic purposes.[47][48]

In Germany after World War I, psychology held institutional power through the military, which was subsequently expanded along with the rest of the military during Nazi Germany.[24] Under the direction of Hermann Göring’s cousin Matthias Göring, the Berlin Psychoanalytic Institute was renamed the Göring Institute. Freudian psychoanalysts were expelled and persecuted under the anti-Jewish policies of the Nazi Party, and all psychologists had to distance themselves from Freud and Adler, founders of psychoanalysis who were also Jewish.[49] The Göring Institute was well-financed throughout the war with a mandate to create a «New German Psychotherapy.» This psychotherapy aimed to align suitable Germans with the overall goals of the Reich. As described by one physician, «Despite the importance of analysis, spiritual guidance and the active cooperation of the patient represent the best way to overcome individual mental problems and to subordinate them to the requirements of the Volk and the Gemeinschaft.» Psychologists were to provide Seelenführung [lit., soul guidance], the leadership of the mind, to integrate people into the new vision of a German community.[50] Harald Schultz-Hencke melded psychology with the Nazi theory of biology and racial origins, criticizing psychoanalysis as a study of the weak and deformed.[51] Johannes Heinrich Schultz, a German psychologist recognized for developing the technique of autogenic training, prominently advocated sterilization and euthanasia of men considered genetically undesirable, and devised techniques for facilitating this process.[52]

After the war, new institutions were created although some psychologists, because of their Nazi affiliation, were discredited. Alexander Mitscherlich founded a prominent applied psychoanalysis journal called Psyche. With funding from the Rockefeller Foundation, Mitscherlich established the first clinical psychosomatic medicine division at Heidelberg University. In 1970, psychology was integrated into the required studies of medical students.[53]

After the Russian Revolution, the Bolsheviks promoted psychology as a way to engineer the «New Man» of socialism. Consequently, university psychology departments trained large numbers of students in psychology. At the completion of training, positions were made available for those students at schools, workplaces, cultural institutions, and in the military. The Russian state emphasized pedology and the study of child development. Lev Vygotsky became prominent in the field of child development.[35] The Bolsheviks also promoted free love and embraced the doctrine of psychoanalysis as an antidote to sexual repression.[54]: 84–6[55] Although pedology and intelligence testing fell out of favor in 1936, psychology maintained its privileged position as an instrument of the Soviet Union.[35] Stalinist purges took a heavy toll and instilled a climate of fear in the profession, as elsewhere in Soviet society.[54]: 22 Following World War II, Jewish psychologists past and present, including Lev Vygotsky, A.R. Luria, and Aron Zalkind, were denounced; Ivan Pavlov (posthumously) and Stalin himself were celebrated as heroes of Soviet psychology.[54]: 25–6, 48–9  Soviet academics experienced a degree of liberalization during the Khrushchev Thaw. The topics of cybernetics, linguistics, and genetics became acceptable again. The new field of engineering psychology emerged. The field involved the study of the mental aspects of complex jobs (such as pilot and cosmonaut). Interdisciplinary studies became popular and scholars such as Georgy Shchedrovitsky developed systems theory approaches to human behavior.[54]: 27–33

Twentieth-century Chinese psychology originally modeled itself on U.S. psychology, with translations from American authors like William James, the establishment of university psychology departments and journals, and the establishment of groups including the Chinese Association of Psychological Testing (1930) and the Chinese Psychological Society (1937). Chinese psychologists were encouraged to focus on education and language learning. Chinese psychologists were drawn to the idea that education would enable modernization. John Dewey, who lectured to Chinese audiences between 1919 and 1921, had a significant influence on psychology in China. Chancellor T’sai Yuan-p’ei introduced him at Peking University as a greater thinker than Confucius. Kuo Zing-yang who received a PhD at the University of California, Berkeley, became President of Zhejiang University and popularized behaviorism.[56]: 5–9  After the Chinese Communist Party gained control of the country, the Stalinist Soviet Union became the major influence, with Marxism–Leninism the leading social doctrine and Pavlovian conditioning the approved means of behavior change. Chinese psychologists elaborated on Lenin’s model of a «reflective» consciousness, envisioning an «active consciousness» (pinyin: tzu-chueh neng-tung-li) able to transcend material conditions through hard work and ideological struggle. They developed a concept of «recognition» (pinyin: jen-shih) which referred to the interface between individual perceptions and the socially accepted worldview; failure to correspond with party doctrine was «incorrect recognition.»[56]: 9–17 Psychology education was centralized under the Chinese Academy of Sciences, supervised by the State Council. In 1951, the academy created a Psychology Research Office, which in 1956 became the Institute of Psychology. Because most leading psychologists were educated in the United States, the first concern of the academy was the re-education of these psychologists in the Soviet doctrines. Child psychology and pedagogy for the purpose of a nationally cohesive education remained a central goal of the discipline.[56]: 18–24 

Disciplinary organization

Institutions

In 1920, Édouard Claparède and Pierre Bovet created a new applied psychology organization called the International Congress of Psychotechnics Applied to Vocational Guidance, later called the International Congress of Psychotechnics and then the International Association of Applied Psychology.[31] The IAAP is considered the oldest international psychology association.[57] Today, at least 65 international groups deal with specialized aspects of psychology.[57] In response to male predominance in the field, female psychologists in the U.S. formed the National Council of Women Psychologists in 1941. This organization became the International Council of Women Psychologists after World War II and the International Council of Psychologists in 1959. Several associations including the Association of Black Psychologists and the Asian American Psychological Association have arisen to promote the inclusion of non-European racial groups in the profession.[57]

The International Union of Psychological Science (IUPsyS) is the world federation of national psychological societies. The IUPsyS was founded in 1951 under the auspices of the United Nations Educational, Cultural and Scientific Organization (UNESCO).[31][58] Psychology departments have since proliferated around the world, based primarily on the Euro-American model.[21][58] Since 1966, the Union has published the International Journal of Psychology.[31] IAAP and IUPsyS agreed in 1976 each to hold a congress every four years, on a staggered basis.[57]

IUPsyS recognizes 66 national psychology associations and at least 15 others exist.[57] The American Psychological Association is the oldest and largest.[57] Its membership has increased from 5,000 in 1945 to 100,000 in the present day.[33] The APA includes 54 divisions, which since 1960 have steadily proliferated to include more specialties. Some of these divisions, such as the Society for the Psychological Study of Social Issues and the American Psychology–Law Society, began as autonomous groups.[57]

The Interamerican Psychological Society, founded in 1951, aspires to promote psychology across the Western Hemisphere. It holds the Interamerican Congress of Psychology and ha had 1,000 members in year 2000. The European Federation of Professional Psychology Associations, founded in 1981, represents 30 national associations with a total of 100,000 individual members. At least 30 other international organizations represent psychologists in different regions.[57]

In some places, governments legally regulate who can provide psychological services or represent themselves as a «psychologist.»[59] The APA defines a psychologist as someone with a doctoral degree in psychology.[60]

Boundaries

Early practitioners of experimental psychology distinguished themselves from parapsychology, which in the late nineteenth century enjoyed popularity (including the interest of scholars such as William James). Some people considered parapsychology to be part of «psychology.» Parapsychology, hypnotism, and psychism were major topics at the early International Congresses. But students of these fields were eventually ostracized, and more or less banished from the Congress in 1900–1905.[31] Parapsychology persisted for a time at Imperial University in Japan, with publications such as Clairvoyance and Thoughtography by Tomokichi Fukurai, but it was mostly shunned by 1913.[32]

As a discipline, psychology has long sought to fend off accusations that it is a «soft» science. Philosopher of science Thomas Kuhn’s 1962 critique implied psychology overall was in a pre-paradigm state, lacking agreement on the type of overarching theory found in mature sciences such as chemistry and physics.[61] Because some areas of psychology rely on research methods such as surveys and questionnaires, critics asserted that psychology is not an objective science. Skeptics have suggested that personality, thinking, and emotion cannot be directly measured and are often inferred from subjective self-reports, which may be problematic. Experimental psychologists have devised a variety of ways to indirectly measure these elusive phenomenological entities.[62][63][64]

Divisions still exist within the field, with some psychologists more oriented towards the unique experiences of individual humans, which cannot be understood only as data points within a larger population. Critics inside and outside the field have argued that mainstream psychology has become increasingly dominated by a «cult of empiricism,» which limits the scope of research because investigators restrict themselves to methods derived from the physical sciences.[65]: 36–7 Feminist critiques have argued that claims to scientific objectivity obscure the values and agenda of (historically) mostly male researchers.[37] Jean Grimshaw, for example, argues that mainstream psychological research has advanced a patriarchal agenda through its efforts to control behavior.[65]: 120

Major schools of thought

Biological

False-color representations of cerebral fiber pathways affected, per Van Horn et al.[V]: 3 

Psychologists generally consider biology the substrate of thought and feeling, and therefore an important area of study. Behaviorial neuroscience, also known as biological psychology, involves the application of biological principles to the study of physiological and genetic mechanisms underlying behavior in humans and other animals. The allied field of comparative psychology is the scientific study of the behavior and mental processes of non-human animals.[66] A leading question in behavioral neuroscience has been whether and how mental functions are localized in the brain. From Phineas Gage to H.M. and Clive Wearing, individual people with mental deficits traceable to physical brain damage have inspired new discoveries in this area.[67] Modern behavioral neuroscience could be said to originate in the 1870s, when in France Paul Broca traced production of speech to the left frontal gyrus, thereby also demonstrating hemispheric lateralization of brain function. Soon after, Carl Wernicke identified a related area necessary for the understanding of speech.[68]: 20–2 

The contemporary field of behavioral neuroscience focuses on the physical basis of behavior. Behaviorial neuroscientists use animal models, often relying on rats, to study the neural, genetic, and cellular mechanisms that underlie behaviors involved in learning, memory, and fear responses.[69] Cognitive neuroscientists, by using neural imaging tools, investigate the neural correlates of psychological processes in humans. Neuropsychologists conduct psychological assessments to determine how an individual’s behavior and cognition are related to the brain. The biopsychosocial model is a cross-disciplinary, holistic model that concerns the ways in which interrelationships of biological, psychological, and socio-environmental factors affect health and behavior.[70]

Evolutionary psychology approaches thought and behavior from a modern evolutionary perspective. This perspective suggests that psychological adaptations evolved to solve recurrent problems in human ancestral environments. Evolutionary psychologists attempt to find out how human psychological traits are evolved adaptations, the results of natural selection or sexual selection over the course of human evolution.[71]

The history of the biological foundations of psychology includes evidence of racism. The idea of white supremacy and indeed the modern concept of race itself arose during the process of world conquest by Europeans.[72] Carl von Linnaeus’s four-fold classification of humans classifies Europeans as intelligent and severe, Americans as contented and free, Asians as ritualistic, and Africans as lazy and capricious. Race was also used to justify the construction of socially specific mental disorders such as drapetomania and dysaesthesia aethiopica—the behavior of uncooperative African slaves.[73] After the creation of experimental psychology, «ethnical psychology» emerged as a subdiscipline, based on the assumption that studying primitive races would provide an important link between animal behavior and the psychology of more evolved humans.[74]

Behaviorist

A tenet of behavioral research is that a large part of both human and lower-animal behavior is learned. A principle associated with behavioral research is that the mechanisms involved in learning apply to humans and non-human animals. Behavioral researchers have developed a treatment known as behavior modification, which is used to help individuals replace undesirable behaviors with desirable ones.

The film of the Little Albert experiment

Early behavioral researchers studied stimulus–response pairings, now known as classical conditioning. They demonstrated that when a biologically potent stimulus (e.g., food that elicits salivation) is paired with a previously neutral stimulus (e.g., a bell) over several learning trials, the neutral stimulus by itself can come to elicit the response the biologically potent stimulus elicits. Ivan Pavlov—known best for inducing dogs to salivate in the presence of a stimulus previously linked with food—became a leading figure in the Soviet Union and inspired followers to use his methods on humans.[35] In the United States, Edward Lee Thorndike initiated «connectionist» studies by trapping animals in «puzzle boxes» and rewarding them for escaping. Thorndike wrote in 1911, «There can be no moral warrant for studying man’s nature unless the study will enable us to control his acts.»[27]: 212–5  From 1910 to 1913 the American Psychological Association went through a sea change of opinion, away from mentalism and towards «behavioralism.» In 1913, John B. Watson coined the term behaviorism for this school of thought.[27]: 218–27  Watson’s famous Little Albert experiment in 1920 was at first thought to demonstrate that repeated use of upsetting loud noises could instill phobias (aversions to other stimuli) in an infant human,[12][75] although such a conclusion was likely an exaggeration.[76] Karl Lashley, a close collaborator with Watson, examined biological manifestations of learning in the brain.[67]

Clark L. Hull, Edwin Guthrie, and others did much to help behaviorism become a widely used paradigm.[33] A new method of «instrumental» or «operant» conditioning added the concepts of reinforcement and punishment to the model of behavior change. Radical behaviorists avoided discussing the inner workings of the mind, especially the unconscious mind, which they considered impossible to assess scientifically.[77] Operant conditioning was first described by Miller and Kanorski and popularized in the U.S. by B.F. Skinner, who emerged as a leading intellectual of the behaviorist movement.[78][79]

Noam Chomsky published an influential critique of radical behaviorism on the grounds that behaviorist principles could not adequately explain the complex mental process of language acquisition and language use.[80][81] The review, which was scathing, did much to reduce the status of behaviorism within psychology.[27]: 282–5  Martin Seligman and his colleagues discovered that they could condition in dogs a state of «learned helplessness», which was not predicted by the behaviorist approach to psychology.[82][83] Edward C. Tolman advanced a hybrid «cognitive behavioral» model, most notably with his 1948 publication discussing the cognitive maps used by rats to guess at the location of food at the end of a maze.[84] Skinner’s behaviorism did not die, in part because it generated successful practical applications.[81]

The Association for Behavior Analysis International was founded in 1974 and by 2003 had members from 42 countries. The field has gained a foothold in Latin America and Japan.[85] Applied behavior analysis is the term used for the application of the principles of operant conditioning to change socially significant behavior (it supersedes the term, «behavior modification»).[86]

Cognitive

Green Red Blue
Purple Blue Purple


Blue Purple Red
Green Purple Green


The Stroop effect is the fact that naming the color of the first set of words is easier and quicker than the second.

Cognitive psychology involves the study of mental processes, including perception, attention, language comprehension and production, memory, and problem solving.[87] Researchers in the field of cognitive psychology are sometimes called cognitivists. They rely on an information processing model of mental functioning. Cognitivist research is informed by functionalism and experimental psychology.

Starting in the 1950s, the experimental techniques developed by Wundt, James, Ebbinghaus, and others re-emerged as experimental psychology became increasingly cognitivist and, eventually, constituted a part of the wider, interdisciplinary cognitive science.[88][89] Some called this development the cognitive revolution because it rejected the anti-mentalist dogma of behaviorism as well as the strictures of psychoanalysis.[89]

Albert Bandura helped along the transition in psychology from behaviorism to cognitive psychology. Bandura and other social learning theorists advanced the idea of vicarious learning. In other words, they advanced the view that a child can learn by observing the immediate social environment and not necessarily from having been reinforced for enacting a behavior, although they did not rule out the influence of reinforcement on learning a behavior.[90]

The Müller–Lyer illusion. Psychologists make inferences about mental processes from shared phenomena such as optical illusions.

Technological advances also renewed interest in mental states and mental representations. English neuroscientist Charles Sherrington and Canadian psychologist Donald O. Hebb used experimental methods to link psychological phenomena to the structure and function of the brain. The rise of computer science, cybernetics, and artificial intelligence underlined the value of comparing information processing in humans and machines.

A popular and representative topic in this area is cognitive bias, or irrational thought. Psychologists (and economists) have classified and described a sizeable catalogue of biases which recur frequently in human thought. The availability heuristic, for example, is the tendency to overestimate the importance of something which happens to come readily to mind.[91]

Elements of behaviorism and cognitive psychology were synthesized to form cognitive behavioral therapy, a form of psychotherapy modified from techniques developed by American psychologist Albert Ellis and American psychiatrist Aaron T. Beck.

On a broader level, cognitive science is an interdisciplinary enterprise involving cognitive psychologists, cognitive neuroscientists, linguists, and researchers in artificial intelligence, human–computer interaction, and computational neuroscience. The discipline of cognitive science covers cognitive psychology as well as philosophy of mind, computer science, and neuroscience.[92] Computer simulations are sometimes used to model phenomena of interest.

Social psychology is concerned with how behaviors, thoughts, feelings, and the social environment influence human interactions.[93] Social psychologists study such topics as the influence of others on an individual’s behavior (e.g. conformity, persuasion) and the formation of beliefs, attitudes, and stereotypes about other people. Social cognition fuses elements of social and cognitive psychology for the purpose of understanding how people process, remember, or distort social information. The study of group dynamics involves research on the nature of leadership, organizational communication, and related phenomena. In recent years, social psychologists have become interested in implicit measures, mediational models, and the interaction of person and social factors in accounting for behavior. Some concepts that sociologists have applied to the study of psychiatric disorders, concepts such as the social role, sick role, social class, life events, culture, migration, and total institution, have influenced social psychologists.[94]

Psychoanalytic

Psychoanalysis refers to the theories and therapeutic techniques applied to the unconscious mind and its impact on everyday life. These theories and techniques inform treatments for mental disorders.[95][96][97] Psychoanalysis originated in the 1890s, most prominently with the work of Sigmund Freud. Freud’s psychoanalytic theory was largely based on interpretive methods, introspection, and clinical observation. It became very well known, largely because it tackled subjects such as sexuality, repression, and the unconscious.[54]: 84–6  Freud pioneered the methods of free association and dream interpretation.[98][99]

Psychoanalytic theory is not monolithic. Other well-known psychoanalytic thinkers who diverged from Freud include Alfred Adler, Carl Jung, Erik Erikson, Melanie Klein, D.W. Winnicott, Karen Horney, Erich Fromm, John Bowlby, Freud’s daughter Anna Freud, and Harry Stack Sullivan. These individuals ensured that psychoanalysis would evolve into diverse schools of thought. Among these schools are ego psychology, object relations, and interpersonal, Lacanian, and relational psychoanalysis.

Psychologists such as Hans Eysenck and philosophers including Karl Popper sharply criticized psychoanalysis. Popper argued that psychoanalysis had been misrepresented as a scientific discipline,[100] whereas Eysenck advanced the view that psychoanalytic tenets had been contradicted by experimental data. By the end of the 20th century, psychology departments in American universities mostly had marginalized Freudian theory, dismissing it as a «desiccated and dead» historical artifact.[101] Researchers such as António Damásio, Oliver Sacks, and Joseph LeDoux; and individuals in the emerging field of neuro-psychoanalysis have defended some of Freud’s ideas on scientific grounds.[102]

Existential-humanistic

Psychologist Abraham Maslow in 1943 posited that humans have a hierarchy of needs, and it makes sense to fulfill the basic needs first (food, water etc.) before higher-order needs can be met.[103]

Humanistic psychology, which has been influenced by existentialism and phenomenology,[104] stresses free will and self-actualization.[105] It emerged in the 1950s as a movement within academic psychology, in reaction to both behaviorism and psychoanalysis.[106] The humanistic approach seeks to view the whole person, not just fragmented parts of the personality or isolated cognitions.[107] Humanistic psychology also focuses on personal growth, self-identity, death, aloneness, and freedom. It emphasizes subjective meaning, the rejection of determinism, and concern for positive growth rather than pathology. Some founders of the humanistic school of thought were American psychologists Abraham Maslow, who formulated a hierarchy of human needs, and Carl Rogers, who created and developed client-centered therapy.

Later, positive psychology opened up humanistic themes to scientific study. Positive psychology is the study of factors which contribute to human happiness and well-being, focusing more on people who are currently healthy. In 2010, Clinical Psychological Review published a special issue devoted to positive psychological interventions, such as gratitude journaling and the physical expression of gratitude. It is, however, far from clear that positive psychology is effective in making people happier.[108][109] Positive psychological interventions have been limited in scope, but their effects are thought to be somewhat better than placebo effects.

The American Association for Humanistic Psychology, formed in 1963, declared:

Humanistic psychology is primarily an orientation toward the whole of psychology rather than a distinct area or school. It stands for respect for the worth of persons, respect for differences of approach, open-mindedness as to acceptable methods, and interest in exploration of new aspects of human behavior. As a «third force» in contemporary psychology, it is concerned with topics having little place in existing theories and systems: e.g., love, creativity, self, growth, organism, basic need-gratification, self-actualization, higher values, being, becoming, spontaneity, play, humor, affection, naturalness, warmth, ego-transcendence, objectivity, autonomy, responsibility, meaning, fair-play, transcendental experience, peak experience, courage, and related concepts.[110]

Existential psychology emphasizes the need to understand a client’s total orientation towards the world. Existential psychology is opposed to reductionism, behaviorism, and other methods that objectify the individual.[105] In the 1950s and 1960s, influenced by philosophers Søren Kierkegaard and Martin Heidegger, psychoanalytically trained American psychologist Rollo May helped to develop existential psychology. Existential psychotherapy, which follows from existential psychology, is a therapeutic approach that is based on the idea that a person’s inner conflict arises from that individual’s confrontation with the givens of existence. Swiss psychoanalyst Ludwig Binswanger and American psychologist George Kelly may also be said to belong to the existential school.[111] Existential psychologists tend to differ from more «humanistic» psychologists in the former’s relatively neutral view of human nature and relatively positive assessment of anxiety.[112] Existential psychologists emphasized the humanistic themes of death, free will, and meaning, suggesting that meaning can be shaped by myths and narratives; meaning can be deepened by the acceptance of free will, which is requisite to living an authentic life, albeit often with anxiety with regard to death.[113]

Austrian existential psychiatrist and Holocaust survivor Viktor Frankl drew evidence of meaning’s therapeutic power from reflections upon his own internment.[114] He created a variation of existential psychotherapy called logotherapy, a type of existentialist analysis that focuses on a will to meaning (in one’s life), as opposed to Adler’s Nietzschean doctrine of will to power or Freud’s will to pleasure.[115]

Themes

Personality

Personality psychology is concerned with enduring patterns of behavior, thought, and emotion. Theories of personality vary across different psychological schools of thought. Each theory carries different assumptions about such features as the role of the unconscious and the importance of childhood experience. According to Freud, personality is based on the dynamic interactions of the id, ego, and super-ego.[116] By contrast, trait theorists have developed taxonomies of personality constructs in describing personality in terms of key traits. Trait theorists have often employed statistical data-reduction methods, such as factor analysis. Although the number of proposed traits has varied widely, Hans Eysenck’s early biologically-based model suggests at least three major trait constructs are necessary to describe human personality, extraversion–introversion, neuroticism-stability, and psychoticism-normality. Raymond Cattell empirically derived a theory of 16 personality factors at the primary-factor level and up to eight broader second-stratum factors.[117][118][119][120]
Since the 1980s, the Big Five (openness to experience, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism) emerged as an important trait theory of personality.[121] Dimensional models of personality are receiving increasing support, and a version of dimensional assessment has been included in the DSM-V. However, despite a plethora of research into the various versions of the «Big Five» personality dimensions, it appears necessary to move on from static conceptualizations of personality structure to a more dynamic orientation, acknowledging that personality constructs are subject to learning and change over the lifespan.[122][123]

An early example of personality assessment was the Woodworth Personal Data Sheet, constructed during World War I. The popular, although psychometrically inadequate, Myers–Briggs Type Indicator[124] was developed to assess individuals’ «personality types» according to the personality theories of Carl Jung. The Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory (MMPI), despite its name, is more a dimensional measure of psychopathology than a personality measure.[125] California Psychological Inventory contains 20 personality scales (e.g., independence, tolerance).[126] The International Personality Item Pool, which is in the public domain, has become a source of scales that can be used personality assessment.[127]

Unconscious mind

Study of the unconscious mind, a part of the psyche outside the individual’s awareness but that is believed to influence conscious thought and behavior, was a hallmark of early psychology. In one of the first psychology experiments conducted in the United States, C.S. Peirce and Joseph Jastrow found in 1884 that research subjects could choose the minutely heavier of two weights even if consciously uncertain of the difference.[128] Freud popularized the concept of the unconscious mind, particularly when he referred to an uncensored intrusion of unconscious thought into one’s speech (a Freudian slip) or to his efforts to interpret dreams.[129] His 1901 book The Psychopathology of Everyday Life catalogues hundreds of everyday events that Freud explains in terms of unconscious influence. Pierre Janet advanced the idea of a subconscious mind, which could contain autonomous mental elements unavailable to the direct scrutiny of the subject.[130]

The concept of unconscious processes has remained important in psychology. Cognitive psychologists have used a «filter» model of attention. According to the model, much information processing takes place below the threshold of consciousness, and only certain stimuli, limited by their nature and number, make their way through the filter. Much research has shown that subconscious priming of certain ideas can covertly influence thoughts and behavior.[130] Because of the unreliability of self-reporting, a major hurdle in this type of research involves demonstrating that a subject’s conscious mind has not perceived a target stimulus. For this reason, some psychologists prefer to distinguish between implicit and explicit memory. In another approach, one can also describe a subliminal stimulus as meeting an objective but not a subjective threshold.[131]

The automaticity model of John Bargh and others involves the ideas of automaticity and unconscious processing in our understanding of social behavior,[132][133] although there has been dispute with regard to replication.[134][135]
Some experimental data suggest that the brain begins to consider taking actions before the mind becomes aware of them.[136] The influence of unconscious forces on people’s choices bears on the philosophical question of free will. John Bargh, Daniel Wegner, and Ellen Langer describe free will as an illusion.[132][133][137]

Motivation

Some psychologists study motivation or the subject of why people or lower animals initiate a behavior at a particular time. It also involves the study of why humans and lower animals continue or terminate a behavior. Psychologists such as William James initially used the term motivation to refer to intention, in a sense similar to the concept of will in European philosophy. With the steady rise of Darwinian and Freudian thinking, instinct also came to be seen as a primary source of motivation.[138] According to drive theory, the forces of instinct combine into a single source of energy which exerts a constant influence. Psychoanalysis, like biology, regarded these forces as demands originating in the nervous system. Psychoanalysts believed that these forces, especially the sexual instincts, could become entangled and transmuted within the psyche. Classical psychoanalysis conceives of a struggle between the pleasure principle and the reality principle, roughly corresponding to id and ego. Later, in Beyond the Pleasure Principle, Freud introduced the concept of the death drive, a compulsion towards aggression, destruction, and psychic repetition of traumatic events.[139] Meanwhile, behaviorist researchers used simple dichotomous models (pleasure/pain, reward/punishment) and well-established principles such as the idea that a thirsty creature will take pleasure in drinking.[138][140] Clark Hull formalized the latter idea with his drive reduction model.[141]

Hunger, thirst, fear, sexual desire, and thermoregulation constitute fundamental motivations in animals.[140] Humans seem to exhibit a more complex set of motivations—though theoretically these could be explained as resulting from desires for belonging, positive self-image, self-consistency, truth, love, and control.[142][143]

Motivation can be modulated or manipulated in many different ways. Researchers have found that eating, for example, depends not only on the organism’s fundamental need for homeostasis—an important factor causing the experience of hunger—but also on circadian rhythms, food availability, food palatability, and cost.[140] Abstract motivations are also malleable, as evidenced by such phenomena as goal contagion: the adoption of goals, sometimes unconsciously, based on inferences about the goals of others.[144] Vohs and Baumeister suggest that contrary to the need-desire-fulfilment cycle of animal instincts, human motivations sometimes obey a «getting begets wanting» rule: the more you get a reward such as self-esteem, love, drugs, or money, the more you want it. They suggest that this principle can even apply to food, drink, sex, and sleep.[145]

Development psychology

Developmental psychologists would engage a child with a book and then make observations based on how the child interacts with the object.

Developmental psychology refers to the scientific study of how and why the thought processes, emotions, and behaviors of humans change over the course of their lives.[146] Some credit Charles Darwin with conducting the first systematic study within the rubric of developmental psychology, having published in 1877 a short paper detailing the development of innate forms of communication based on his observations of his infant son.[147] The main origins of the discipline, however, are found in the work of Jean Piaget. Like Piaget, developmental psychologists originally focused primarily on the development of cognition from infancy to adolescence. Later, developmental psychology extended itself to the study cognition over the life span. In addition to studying cognition, developmental psychologists have also come to focus on affective, behavioral, moral, social, and neural development.

Developmental psychologists who study children use a number of research methods. For example, they make observations of children in natural settings such as preschools[148] and engage them in experimental tasks.[149] Such tasks often resemble specially designed games and activities that are both enjoyable for the child and scientifically useful. Developmental researchers have even devised clever methods to study the mental processes of infants.[150] In addition to studying children, developmental psychologists also study aging and processes throughout the life span, including old age.[151] These psychologists draw on the full range of psychological theories to inform their research.[146]

Genes and environment

All researched psychological traits are influenced by both genes and environment, to varying degrees.[152][153] These two sources of influence are often confounded in observational research of individuals and families. An example of this confounding can be shown in the transmission of depression from a depressed mother to her offspring. A theory based on environmental transmission would hold that an offspring, by virtue of their having a problematic rearing environment managed by a depressed mother, is at risk for developing depression. On the other hand, a hereditarian theory would hold that depression risk in an offspring is influenced to some extent by genes passed to the child from the mother. Genes and environment in these simple transmission models are completely confounded. A depressed mother may both carry genes that contribute to depression in her offspring and also create a rearing environment that increases the risk of depression in her child.

Behavioral genetics researchers have employed methodologies that help to disentangle this confound and understand the nature and origins of individual differences in behavior.[71] Traditionally the research has involved twin studies and adoption studies, two designs where genetic and environmental influences can be partially un-confounded. More recently, gene-focused research has contributed to understanding genetic contributions to the development of psychological traits.

The availability of microarray molecular genetic or genome sequencing technologies allows researchers to measure participant DNA variation directly, and test whether individual genetic variants within genes are associated with psychological traits and psychopathology through methods including genome-wide association studies. One goal of such research is similar to that in positional cloning and its success in Huntington’s: once a causal gene is discovered biological research can be conducted to understand how that gene influences the phenotype. One major result of genetic association studies is the general finding that psychological traits and psychopathology, as well as complex medical diseases, are highly polygenic,[154][155][156][157][158] where a large number (on the order of hundreds to thousands) of genetic variants, each of small effect, contribute to individual differences in the behavioral trait or propensity to the disorder. Active research continues to work toward understanding the genetic and environmental bases of behavior and their interaction.

Applications

Psychology encompasses many subfields and includes different approaches to the study of mental processes and behavior.

Psychological testing

Psychological testing has ancient origins, dating as far back as 2200 BC, in the examinations for the Chinese civil service. Written exams began during the Han dynasty (202 BC – AD 200). By 1370, the Chinese system required a stratified series of tests, involving essay writing and knowledge of diverse topics. The system was ended in 1906.[159]: 41–2  In Europe, mental assessment took a different approach, with theories of physiognomy—judgment of character based on the face—described by Aristotle in 4th century BC Greece. Physiognomy remained current through the Enlightenment, and added the doctrine of phrenology: a study of mind and intelligence based on simple assessment of neuroanatomy.[159]: 42–3 

When experimental psychology came to Britain, Francis Galton was a leading practitioner. By virtue of his procedures for measuring reaction time and sensation, he is considered an inventor of modern mental testing (also known as psychometrics).[159]: 44–5  James McKeen Cattell, a student of Wundt and Galton, brought the idea of psychological testing to the United States, and in fact coined the term «mental test».[159]: 45–6  In 1901, Cattell’s student Clark Wissler published discouraging results, suggesting that mental testing of Columbia and Barnard students failed to predict academic performance.[159]: 45–6  In response to 1904 orders from the Minister of Public Instruction, French psychologists Alfred Binet and Théodore Simon developed and elaborated a new test of intelligence in 1905–1911. They used a range of questions diverse in their nature and difficulty. Binet and Simon introduced the concept of mental age and referred to the lowest scorers on their test as idiots. Henry H. Goddard put the Binet-Simon scale to work and introduced classifications of mental level such as imbecile and feebleminded. In 1916, (after Binet’s death), Stanford professor Lewis M. Terman modified the Binet-Simon scale (renamed the Stanford–Binet scale) and introduced the intelligence quotient as a score report.[159]: 50–56  Based on his test findings, and reflecting the racism common to that era, Terman concluded that intellectual disability «represents the level of intelligence which is very, very common among Spanish-Indians and Mexican families of the Southwest and also among negroes. Their dullness seems to be racial.»[160]

Following the Army Alpha and Army Beta tests, which was developed by psychologist Robert Yerkes in 1917 and then used in World War 1 by industrial and organizational psychologists for large-scale employee testing and selection of military personnel.[161] Mental testing also became popular in the U.S., where it was applied to schoolchildren. The federally created National Intelligence Test was administered to 7 million children in the 1920s. In 1926, the College Entrance Examination Board created the Scholastic Aptitude Test to standardize college admissions.[159]: 61  The results of intelligence tests were used to argue for segregated schools and economic functions, including the preferential training of Black Americans for manual labor. These practices were criticized by Black intellectuals such a Horace Mann Bond and Allison Davis.[160] Eugenicists used mental testing to justify and organize compulsory sterilization of individuals classified as mentally retarded (now referred to as intellectual disability).[42] In the United States, tens of thousands of men and women were sterilized. Setting a precedent that has never been overturned, the U.S. Supreme Court affirmed the constitutionality of this practice in the 1927 case Buck v. Bell.[162]

Today mental testing is a routine phenomenon for people of all ages in Western societies.[159]: 2 Modern testing aspires to criteria including standardization of procedure, consistency of results, output of an interpretable score, statistical norms describing population outcomes, and, ideally, effective prediction of behavior and life outcomes outside of testing situations.[159]: 4–6  Psychological testing is regularly used in forensic contexts to aid legal judgments and decisions.[163] Developments in psychometrics include work on test and scale reliability and validity.[164] Developments in item-response theory,[165] structural equation modeling,[166] and bifactor analysis[167] have helped in strengthening test and scale construction.

Mental health care

The provision of psychological health services is generally called clinical psychology in the U.S. Sometimes, however, members of the school psychology and counseling psychology professions engage in practices that resemble that of clinical psychologists. Clinical psychologists typically include people who have graduated from doctoral programs in clinical psychology. In Canada, some of the members of the abovementioned groups usually fall within the larger category of professional psychology. In Canada and the U.S., practitioners get bachelor’s degrees and doctorates; doctoral students in clinical psychology usually spend one year in a predoctoral internship and one year in postdoctoral internship. In Mexico and most other Latin American and European countries, psychologists do not get bachelor’s and doctoral degrees; instead, they take a three-year professional course following high school.[60] Clinical psychology is at present the largest specialization within psychology.[168] It includes the study and application of psychology for the purpose of understanding, preventing, and relieving psychological distress, dysfunction, and/or mental illness. Clinical psychologists also try to promote subjective well-being and personal growth. Central to the practice of clinical psychology are psychological assessment and psychotherapy although clinical psychologists may also engage in research, teaching, consultation, forensic testimony, and program development and administration.[169]

Credit for the first psychology clinic in the United States typically goes to Lightner Witmer, who established his practice in Philadelphia in 1896. Another modern psychotherapist was Morton Prince, an early advocate for the establishment of psychology as a clinical and academic discipline.[168] In the first part of the twentieth century, most mental health care in the United States was performed by psychiatrists, who are medical doctors. Psychology entered the field with its refinements of mental testing, which promised to improve the diagnosis of mental problems. For their part, some psychiatrists became interested in using psychoanalysis and other forms of psychodynamic psychotherapy to understand and treat the mentally ill.[37][170]

Psychotherapy as conducted by psychiatrists blurred the distinction between psychiatry and psychology, and this trend continued with the rise of community mental health facilities. Some in the clinical psychology community adopted behavioral therapy, a thoroughly non-psychodynamic model that used behaviorist learning theory to change the actions of patients. A key aspect of behavior therapy is empirical evaluation of the treatment’s effectiveness. In the 1970s, cognitive-behavior therapy emerged with the work of Albert Ellis and Aaron Beck. Although there are similarities between behavior therapy and cognitive-behavior therapy, cognitive-behavior therapy required the application of cognitive constructs. Since the 1970s, the popularity of cognitive-behavior therapy among clinical psychologists increased. A key practice in behavioral and cognitive-behavioral therapy is exposing patients to things they fear, based on the premise that their responses (fear, panic, anxiety) can be deconditioned.[171]

Mental health care today involves psychologists and social workers in increasing numbers. In 1977, National Institute of Mental Health director Bertram Brown described this shift as a source of «intense competition and role confusion.»[37] Graduate programs issuing doctorates in clinical psychology emerged in the 1950s and underwent rapid increase through the 1980s. The PhD degree is intended to train practitioners who could also conduct scientific research. The PsyD degree is more exclusively designed to train practitioners.[60]

Some clinical psychologists focus on the clinical management of patients with brain injury. This subspecialty is known as clinical neuropsychology. In many countries, clinical psychology is a regulated mental health profession. The emerging field of disaster psychology (see crisis intervention) involves professionals who respond to large-scale traumatic events.[172]

The work performed by clinical psychologists tends to be influenced by various therapeutic approaches, all of which involve a formal relationship between professional and client (usually an individual, couple, family, or small group). Typically, these approaches encourage new ways of thinking, feeling, or behaving. Four major theoretical perspectives are psychodynamic, cognitive behavioral, existential–humanistic, and systems or family therapy. There has been a growing movement to integrate the various therapeutic approaches, especially with an increased understanding of issues regarding culture, gender, spirituality, and sexual orientation. With the advent of more robust research findings regarding psychotherapy, there is evidence that most of the major therapies have equal effectiveness, with the key common element being a strong therapeutic alliance.[173][174] Because of this, more training programs and psychologists are now adopting an eclectic therapeutic orientation.[175][176][177][178][179]

Diagnosis in clinical psychology usually follows the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM).[180] The study of mental illnesses is called abnormal psychology.

Education

An example of an item from a cognitive abilities test used in educational psychology

Educational psychology is the study of how humans learn in educational settings, the effectiveness of educational interventions, the psychology of teaching, and the social psychology of schools as organizations. Educational psychologists can be found in preschools, schools of all levels including post secondary institutions, community organizations and learning centers, Government or private research firms, and independent or private consultant.[181] The work of developmental psychologists such as Lev Vygotsky, Jean Piaget, and Jerome Bruner has been influential in creating teaching methods and educational practices. Educational psychology is often included in teacher education programs in places such as North America, Australia, and New Zealand.

School psychology combines principles from educational psychology and clinical psychology to understand and treat students with learning disabilities; to foster the intellectual growth of gifted students; to facilitate prosocial behaviors in adolescents; and otherwise to promote safe, supportive, and effective learning environments. School psychologists are trained in educational and behavioral assessment, intervention, prevention, and consultation, and many have extensive training in research.[182]

Work

Industrial and organizational (I/O) psychology involves research and practices that apply psychological theories and principles to organizations and individuals’ work-lives.[183] In the field’s beginnings, industrialists brought the nascent field of psychology to bear on the study of scientific management techniques for improving workplace efficiency. The field was at first called economic psychology or business psychology; later, industrial psychology, employment psychology, or psychotechnology.[184] An influential early study examined workers at Western Electric’s Hawthorne plant in Cicero, Illinois from 1924 to 1932. Western Electric experimented on factory workers to assess their responses to changes in illumination, breaks, food, and wages. The researchers came to focus on workers’ responses to observation itself, and the term Hawthorne effect is now used to describe the fact that people’s behavior can change when they think they’re being observed.[185] Although the Hawthorne research can be found in psychology textbooks, the research and its findings were weak at best.[186][187]

The name industrial and organizational psychology emerged in the 1960s. In 1973, it became enshrined in the name of the Society for Industrial and Organizational Psychology, Division 14 of the American Psychological Association.[184] One goal of the discipline is to optimize human potential in the workplace. Personnel psychology is a subfield of I/O psychology. Personnel psychologists apply the methods and principles of psychology in selecting and evaluating workers. Another subfield, organizational psychology, examines the effects of work environments and management styles on worker motivation, job satisfaction, and productivity.[188] Most I/O psychologists work outside of academia, for private and public organizations and as consultants.[184] A psychology consultant working in business today might expect to provide executives with information and ideas about their industry, their target markets, and the organization of their company.[189][190]

Organizational behavior (OB) is an allied field involved in the study of human behavior within organizations.[191] One way to differentiate I/O psychology from OB is to note that I/O psychologists train in university psychology departments and OB specialists, in business schools.

Military and intelligence

One role for psychologists in the military has been to evaluate and counsel soldiers and other personnel. In the U.S., this function began during World War I, when Robert Yerkes established the School of Military Psychology at Fort Oglethorpe in Georgia. The school provided psychological training for military staff.[37][192] Today, U.S. Army psychologists perform psychological screening, clinical psychotherapy, suicide prevention, and treatment for post-traumatic stress, as well as provide prevention-related services, for example, smoking cessation.[193] The United States Army’s Mental Health Advisory Teams implement psychological interventions to help combat troops experiencing mental problems.[194][195]

Psychologists may also work on a diverse set of campaigns known broadly as psychological warfare. Psychological warfare chiefly involves the use of propaganda to influence enemy soldiers and civilians. This so-called black propaganda is designed to seem as if it originates from a source other than the Army.[196] The CIA’s MKULTRA program involved more individualized efforts at mind control, involving techniques such as hypnosis, torture, and covert involuntary administration of LSD.[197] The U.S. military used the name Psychological Operations (PSYOP) until 2010, when these activities were reclassified as Military Information Support Operations (MISO), part of Information Operations (IO).[198] Psychologists have sometimes been involved in assisting the interrogation and torture of suspects, staining the records of the psychologists involved.[199]

Health, well-being, and social change

An example of the contribution of psychologists to social change involves the research of Kenneth and Mamie Phipps Clark. These two African American psychologists studied segregation’s adverse psychological impact on Black children. Their research findings played a role in the desegregation case Brown v. Board of Education (1954).[200]

The impact of psychology on social change includes the discipline’s broad influence on teaching and learning. Research has shown that compared to the «whole word» or «whole language» approach, the phonics approach to reading instruction is more efficacious.[201]

Medical applications

Medical facilities increasingly employ psychologists to perform various roles. One aspect of health psychology is the psychoeducation of patients: instructing them in how to follow a medical regimen. Health psychologists can also educate doctors and conduct research on patient compliance.[202][203] Psychologists in the field of public health use a wide variety of interventions to influence human behavior. These range from public relations campaigns and outreach to governmental laws and policies. Psychologists study the composite influence of all these different tools in an effort to influence whole populations of people.[204]

Worker health, safety and wellbeing

Psychologists work with organizations to apply findings from psychological research to improve the health and well-being of employees. Some work as external consultants hired by organizations to solve specific problems, whereas others are full-time employees of the organization. Applications include conducting surveys to identify issues and designing interventions to make work healthier. Some of the specific health areas include:

  • Accidents and injuries: A major contribution is the concept of safety climate, which is employee shared perceptions of the behaviors that are encouraged (e.g., wearing safety gear) and discouraged (not following safety rules) at work.[205] Organizations with strong safety climates have fewer work accidents and injuries.[206]
  • Cardiovascular disease: Cardiovascular disease has been related to lack of job control.[207]
  • Mental health: Exposure to occupational stress is associated with mental health disorder.[208]
  • Musculoskeletal disorder: These are injuries in bones, nerves and tendons due to overexertion and repetitive strain. They have been linked to job satisfaction and workplace stress.[209]
  • Physical health symptoms: Occupational stress has been linked to physical symptoms such as digestive distress and headache.[210]
  • Workplace violence: Violence prevention climate is related to being physically assaulted and psychologically mistreated at work.[211]

Interventions that improve climates are a way to address accidents and violence. Interventions that reduce stress at work or provide employees with tools to better manage it can help in areas where stress is an important component.

Industrial psychology became interested in worker fatigue during World War I, when government ministers in Britain were concerned about the impact of fatigue on workers in munitions factories but not other types of factories.[212][213] In the U. K. some interest in worker well-being emerged with the efforts of Charles Samuel Myers and his National Institute of Industrial Psychology (NIIP) during the inter-War years.[214] In the U. S. during the mid-twentieth century industrial psychologist Arthur Kornhauser pioneered the study of occupational mental health, linking industrial working conditions to mental health as well as the spillover of an unsatisfying job into a worker’s personal life.[215][216] Zickar accumulated evidence to show that «no other industrial psychologist of his era was as devoted to advocating management and labor practices that would improve the lives of working people.»[215]

Occupational health psychology

As interest in the worker health expanded toward the end of the twentieth century, the field of occupational health psychology (OHP) emerged. OHP is a branch of psychology that is interdisciplinary.[217][218][45][219] OHP is concerned with the health and safety of workers.[45][219] OHP addresses topic areas such as the impact of occupational stressors on physical and mental health, mistreatment of workers (e.g., bullying and violence), work-family balance, the impact of involuntary unemployment on physical and mental health, the influence of psychosocial factors on safety and accidents, and interventions designed to improve/protect worker health.[45][220] OHP grew out of health psychology, industrial and organizational psychology, and occupational medicine.[221] OHP has also been informed by disciplines outside psychology, including industrial engineering, sociology, and economics.[222][223]

Research methods

Quantitative psychological research lends itself to the statistical testing of hypotheses. Although the field makes abundant use of randomized and controlled experiments in laboratory settings, such research can only assess a limited range of short-term phenomena. Some psychologists rely on less rigorously controlled, but more ecologically valid, field experiments as well. Other research psychologists rely on statistical methods to glean knowledge from population data.[224] The statistical methods research psychologists employ include the Pearson product–moment correlation coefficient, the analysis of variance, multiple linear regression, logistic regression, structural equation modeling, and hierarchical linear modeling. The measurement and operationalization of important constructs is an essential part of these research designs.

Although this type of psychological research is much less abundant than quantitative research, some psychologists conduct qualitative research. This type of research can involve interviews, questionnaires, and first-hand observation.[225] While hypothesis testing is rare, virtually impossible, in qualitative research, qualitative studies can be helpful in theory and hypothesis generation, interpreting seemingly contradictory quantitative findings, and understanding why some interventions fail and others succeed.[226]

Controlled experiments

Flowchart of four phases (enrollment, intervention allocation, follow-up, and data analysis) of a parallel randomized trial of two groups, modified from the CONSORT 2010 Statement[227]

The experimenter (E) orders the teacher (T), the subject of the experiment, to give what the latter believes are painful electric shocks to a learner (L), who is actually an actor and confederate. The subject believes that for each wrong answer, the learner was receiving actual electric shocks, though in reality there were no such punishments. Being separated from the subject, the confederate set up a tape recorder integrated with the electro-shock generator, which played pre-recorded sounds for each shock level etc.[228]

A true experiment with random assignment of research participants (sometimes called subjects) to rival conditions allows researchers to make strong inferences about causal relationships. When there are large numbers of research participants, the random assignment (also called random allocation) of those participants to rival conditions ensures that the individuals in those conditions will, on average, be similar on most characteristics, including characteristics that went unmeasured. In an experiment, the researcher alters one or more variables of influence, called independent variables, and measures resulting changes in the factors of interest, called dependent variables. Prototypical experimental research is conducted in a laboratory with a carefully controlled environment.

A quasi-experiment refers to a situation in which there are rival conditions under study but random assignment to the different conditions is not possible. Investigators must work with preexisting groups of people. Researchers can use common sense to consider how much the nonrandom assignment threatens the study’s validity.[229] For example, in research on the best way to affect reading achievement in the first three grades of school, school administrators may not permit educational psychologists to randomly assign children to phonics and whole language classrooms, in which case the psychologists must work with preexisting classroom assignments. Psychologists will compare the achievement of children attending phonics and whole language classes and, perhaps, statistically adjust for any initial differences in reading level.

Experimental researchers typically use a statistical hypothesis testing model which involves making predictions before conducting the experiment, then assessing how well the data collected are consistent with the predictions. These predictions are likely to originate from one or more abstract scientific hypotheses about how the phenomenon under study actually works.[230]

Other types of studies

Surveys are used in psychology for the purpose of measuring attitudes and traits, monitoring changes in mood, and checking the validity of experimental manipulations (checking research participants’ perception of the condition they were assigned to). Psychologists have commonly used paper-and-pencil surveys. However, surveys are also conducted over the phone or through e-mail. Web-based surveys are increasingly used to conveniently reach many subjects.

Observational studies are commonly conducted in psychology. In cross-sectional observational studies, psychologists collect data at a single point in time. The goal of many cross-sectional studies is the assess the extent factors are correlated with each other. By contrast, in longitudinal studies psychologists collect data on the same sample at two or more points in time. Sometimes the purpose of longitudinal research is to study trends across time such as the stability of traits or age-related changes in behavior. Because some studies involve endpoints that psychologists cannot ethically study from an experimental standpoint, such as identifying the causes of depression, they conduct longitudinal studies a large group of depression-free people, periodically assessing what is happening in the individuals’ lives. In this way psychologists have an opportunity to test causal hypotheses regarding conditions that commonly arise in people’s lives that put them at risk for depression. Problems that affect longitudinal studies include selective attrition, the type of problem in which bias is introduced when a certain type of research participant disproportionately leaves a study.

Exploratory data analysis refers to a variety of practices that researchers use to reduce a great many variables to a small number overarching factors. In Peirce’s three modes of inference, exploratory data analysis corresponds to abduction.[231] Meta-analysis is the technique research psychologists use to integrate results from many studies of the same variables and arriving at a grand average of the findings.[232]

Direct brain observation/manipulation

A classic and popular tool used to relate mental and neural activity is the electroencephalogram (EEG), a technique using amplified electrodes on a person’s scalp to measure voltage changes in different parts of the brain. Hans Berger, the first researcher to use EEG on an unopened skull, quickly found that brains exhibit signature «brain waves»: electric oscillations which correspond to different states of consciousness. Researchers subsequently refined statistical methods for synthesizing the electrode data, and identified unique brain wave patterns such as the delta wave observed during non-REM sleep.[233]

Newer functional neuroimaging techniques include functional magnetic resonance imaging and positron emission tomography, both of which track the flow of blood through the brain. These technologies provide more localized information about activity in the brain and create representations of the brain with widespread appeal. They also provide insight which avoids the classic problems of subjective self-reporting. It remains challenging to draw hard conclusions about where in the brain specific thoughts originate—or even how usefully such localization corresponds with reality. However, neuroimaging has delivered unmistakable results showing the existence of correlations between mind and brain. Some of these draw on a systemic neural network model rather than a localized function model.[234][235][236]

Interventions such as transcranial magnetic stimulation and drugs also provide information about brain–mind interactions. Psychopharmacology is the study of drug-induced mental effects.

Artificial neural network with two layers, an interconnected group of nodes, akin to the vast network of neurons in the human brain

.

Computer simulation

Computational modeling is a tool used in mathematical psychology and cognitive psychology to simulate behavior.[237] This method has several advantages. Since modern computers process information quickly, simulations can be run in a short time, allowing for high statistical power. Modeling also allows psychologists to visualize hypotheses about the functional organization of mental events that couldn’t be directly observed in a human. Computational neuroscience uses mathematical models to simulate the brain. Another method is symbolic modeling, which represents many mental objects using variables and rules. Other types of modeling include dynamic systems and stochastic modeling.

Animal studies

Animal experiments aid in investigating many aspects of human psychology, including perception, emotion, learning, memory, and thought, to name a few. In the 1890s, Russian physiologist Ivan Pavlov famously used dogs to demonstrate classical conditioning. Non-human primates, cats, dogs, pigeons, and rats and other rodents are often used in psychological experiments. Ideally, controlled experiments introduce only one independent variable at a time, in order to ascertain its unique effects upon dependent variables. These conditions are approximated best in laboratory settings. In contrast, human environments and genetic backgrounds vary so widely, and depend upon so many factors, that it is difficult to control important variables for human subjects. There are pitfalls, however, in generalizing findings from animal studies to humans through animal models.[238]

Comparative psychology refers to the scientific study of the behavior and mental processes of non-human animals, especially as these relate to the phylogenetic history, adaptive significance, and development of behavior. Research in this area explores the behavior of many species, from insects to primates. It is closely related to other disciplines that study animal behavior such as ethology.[239] Research in comparative psychology sometimes appears to shed light on human behavior, but some attempts to connect the two have been quite controversial, for example the Sociobiology of E.O. Wilson.[240] Animal models are often used to study neural processes related to human behavior, e.g. in cognitive neuroscience.

Qualitative research

Qualitative research is often designed to answer questions about the thoughts, feelings, and behaviors of individuals. Qualitative research involving first-hand observation can help describe events as they occur, with the goal of capturing the richness of everyday behavior and with the hope of discovering and understanding phenomena that might have been missed if only more cursory examinations are made.

Qualitative psychological research methods include interviews, first-hand observation, and participant observation. Creswell (2003) identified five main possibilities for qualitative research, including narrative, phenomenology, ethnography, case study, and grounded theory. Qualitative researchers[241] sometimes aim to enrich our understanding of symbols, subjective experiences, or social structures. Sometimes hermeneutic and critical aims can give rise to quantitative research, as in Erich Fromm’s application of psychological and sociological theories, in his book Escape from Freedom, to understanding why many ordinary Germans supported Hitler.[242]

Phineas P. Gage survived an accident in which a large iron rod was driven completely through his head, destroying much of his brain’s left frontal lobe, and is remembered for that injury’s reported effects on his personality and behavior.[243]

Just as Jane Goodall studied chimpanzee social and family life by careful observation of chimpanzee behavior in the field, psychologists conduct naturalistic observation of ongoing human social, professional, and family life. Sometimes the participants are aware they are being observed, and other times the participants do not know they are being observed. Strict ethical guidelines must be followed when covert observation is being carried out.

Program evaluation

Program evaluation involves the systematic collection, analysis, and application of information to answer questions about projects, policies and programs, particularly about their effectiveness.[244][245] In both the public and private sectors, stakeholders often want to know the extent which the programs they are funding, implementing, voting for, receiving, or objecting to are producing the intended effects. While program evaluation first focuses on effectiveness, important considerations often include how much the program costs per participant, how the program could be improved, whether the program is worthwhile, whether there are better alternatives, if there are unintended outcomes, and whether the program goals are appropriate and useful.[246]

Contemporary issues in methodology and practice

Metascience

Metascience involves the application of scientific methodology to study science itself. The field of metascience has revealed problems in psychological research. Some psychological research has suffered from bias,[247] problematic reproducibility,[248] and misuse of statistics.[249] These findings have led to calls for reform from within and from outside the scientific community.[250]

Confirmation bias

In 1959, statistician Theodore Sterling examined the results of psychological studies and discovered that 97% of them supported their initial hypotheses, implying possible publication bias.[251][252][253] Similarly, Fanelli (2010)[254] found that 91.5% of psychiatry/psychology studies confirmed the effects they were looking for, and concluded that the odds of this happening (a positive result) was around five times higher than in fields such as space science or geosciences. Fanelli argued that this is because researchers in «softer» sciences have fewer constraints to their conscious and unconscious biases.

Replication

A replication crisis in psychology has emerged. Many notable findings in the field have not been replicated. Some researchers were even accused of publishing fraudulent results.[255][256][257] Systematic efforts, including efforts by the Reproducibility Project of the Center for Open Science, to assess the extent of the problem found that as many as two-thirds of highly publicized findings in psychology failed to be replicated.[258] Reproducibility has generally been stronger in cognitive psychology (in studies and journals) than social psychology[258] and subfields of differential psychology.[259][260] Other subfields of psychology have also been implicated in the replication crisis, including clinical psychology,[261][262][263] developmental psychology,[264][265][266] and a field closely related to psychology, educational research.[267][268][269][270][271]

Focus on the replication crisis has led to other renewed efforts in the discipline to re-test important findings.[272][273] In response to concerns about publication bias and data dredging (conducting a large number of statistical tests on a great many variables but restricting reporting to the results that were statistically significant), 295 psychology and medical journals have adopted result-blind peer review where studies are accepted not on the basis of their findings and after the studies are completed, but before the studies are conducted and upon the basis of the methodological rigor of their experimental designs and the theoretical justifications for their proposed statistical analysis before data collection or analysis is conducted.[274][275] In addition, large-scale collaborations among researchers working in multiple labs in different countries have taken place. The collaborators regularly make their data openly available for different researchers to assess.[276] Allen and Mehler[277] estimated that 61 percent of result-blind studies have yielded null results, in contrast to an estimated 5 to 20 percent in traditional research.

Misuse of statistics

Some critics view statistical hypothesis testing as misplaced. Psychologist and statistician Jacob Cohen wrote in 1994 that psychologists routinely confuse statistical significance with practical importance, enthusiastically reporting great certainty in unimportant facts.[278] Some psychologists have responded with an increased use of effect size statistics, rather than sole reliance on p-values.[279]

WEIRD bias

«WEIRD» redirects here. For other uses, see Weird.

In 2008, Arnett pointed out that most articles in American Psychological Association journals were about U.S. populations when U.S. citizens are only 5% of the world’s population. He complained that psychologists had no basis for assuming psychological processes to be universal and generalizing research findings to the rest of the global population.[280] In 2010, Henrich, Heine, and Norenzayan reported a bias in conducting psychology studies with participants from «WEIRD» («Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich, and Democratic») societies.[281][282] Henrich et al. found that «96% of psychological samples come from countries with only 12% of the world’s population» (p. 63). The article gave examples of results that differ significantly between people from WEIRD and tribal cultures, including the Müller-Lyer illusion. Arnett (2008), Altmaier and Hall (2008) and Morgan-Consoli et al. (2018) view the Western bias in research and theory as a serious problem considering psychologists are increasingly applying psychological principles developed in WEIRD regions in their research, clinical work, and consultation with populations around the world.[280][283][284] In 2018, Rad, Martingano, and Ginges showed that nearly a decade after Henrich et al.’s paper, over 80% of the samples used in studies published in the journal Psychological Science employed WEIRD samples. Moreover, their analysis showed that several studies did not fully disclose the origin of their samples; the authors offered a set of recommendations to editors and reviewers to reduce WEIRD bias.[285]

STRANGE bias

Similar to the WEIRD bias, starting in 2020, researchers of non-human behavior have started to emphasize the need to document the possibility of the STRANGE (Social background, Trappability and self-selection, Rearing history, Acclimation and habituation, Natural changes in responsiveness, Genetic makeup, and Experience) bias in study conclusions.[286]

Unscientific mental health training

Some observers perceive a gap between scientific theory and its application—in particular, the application of unsupported or unsound clinical practices.[287] Critics say there has been an increase in the number of mental health training programs that do not instill scientific competence.[288] Practices such as «facilitated communication for infantile autism»; memory-recovery techniques including body work; and other therapies, such as rebirthing and reparenting, may be dubious or even dangerous, despite their popularity.[289] These practices, however, are outside the mainstream practices taught in clinical psychology doctoral programs.

Ethics

Ethical standards in the discipline have changed over time. Some famous past studies are today considered unethical and in violation of established codes (the Canadian Code of Conduct for Research Involving Humans, and the Belmont Report). The American Psychological Association has advanced a set of ethical principles and a code of conduct for the profession.[290]

The most important contemporary standards include informed and voluntary consent. After World War II, the Nuremberg Code was established because of Nazi abuses of experimental subjects. Later, most countries (and scientific journals) adopted the Declaration of Helsinki. In the U.S., the National Institutes of Health established the Institutional Review Board in 1966, and in 1974 adopted the National Research Act (HR 7724). All of these measures encouraged researchers to obtain informed consent from human participants in experimental studies. A number of influential but ethically dubious studies led to the establishment of this rule; such studies included the MIT-Harvard Fernald School radioisotope studies, the Thalidomide tragedy, the Willowbrook hepatitis study, and Stanley Milgram’s studies of obedience to authority.

Humans

Universities have ethics committees dedicated to protecting the rights (e.g., voluntary nature of participation in the research, privacy) and well-being (e.g., minimizing distress) of research participants. University ethics committees evaluate proposed research to ensure that researchers protect the rights and well-being of participants; an investigator’s research project cannot be conducted unless approved by such an ethics committee.[291]

The ethics code of the American Psychological Association originated in 1951 as «Ethical Standards of Psychologists». This code has guided the formation of licensing laws in most American states. It has changed multiple times over the decades since its adoption. In 1989, the APA revised its policies on advertising and referral fees to negotiate the end of an investigation by the Federal Trade Commission. The 1992 incarnation was the first to distinguish between «aspirational» ethical standards and «enforceable» ones. Members of the public have a five-year window to file ethics complaints about APA members with the APA ethics committee; members of the APA have a three-year window.[292]

Some of the ethical issues considered most important are the requirement to practice only within the area of competence, to maintain confidentiality with the patients, and to avoid sexual relations with them. Another important principle is informed consent, the idea that a patient or research subject must understand and freely choose a procedure they are undergoing.[292] Some of the most common complaints against clinical psychologists include sexual misconduct.[292]

Other animals

Research on other animals is also governed by university ethics committees. Research on nonhuman animals cannot proceed without permission of the ethics committee of the researcher’s home institution. Current ethical guidelines state that using non-human animals for scientific purposes is only acceptable when the harm (physical or psychological) done to animals is outweighed by the benefits of the research.[293] Keeping this in mind, psychologists can use certain research techniques on animals that could not be used on humans.

  • Comparative psychologist Harry Harlow drew moral condemnation for isolation experiments on rhesus macaque monkeys at the University of Wisconsin–Madison in the 1970s.[294] The aim of the research was to produce an animal model of clinical depression. Harlow also devised what he called a «rape rack», to which the female isolates were tied in normal monkey mating posture.[295] In 1974, American literary critic Wayne C. Booth wrote that, «Harry Harlow and his colleagues go on torturing their nonhuman primates decade after decade, invariably proving what we all knew in advance—that social creatures can be destroyed by destroying their social ties.» He writes that Harlow made no mention of the criticism of the morality of his work.[296]

References

  1. ^ Fernald LD (2008). Psychology: Six perspectives Archived 8 June 2020 at the Wayback Machine (pp.12–15). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications.
  2. ^ Hockenbury & Hockenbury. Psychology. Worth Publishers, 2010.
  3. ^ Psychoanalysis and other forms of depth psychology are most typically associated with theories about the unconscious mind. By contrast, behaviorists consider such phenomena as classical conditioning and operant conditioning. Cognitivists explore implicit memory, automaticity, and subliminal messages, all of which are understood either to bypass or to occur outside of conscious effort or attention. Indeed, cognitive-behavioral therapists counsel their clients to become aware of maladaptive thought patterns, the nature of which the clients previously had not been conscious.
  4. ^ O’Neil, H.F.; cited in Coon, D.; Mitterer, J.O. (2008). Introduction to psychology: Gateways to mind and behavior Archived 18 September 2015 at the Wayback Machine (12th ed., pp. 15–16). Stamford, CT: Cengage Learning.
  5. ^ «The mission of the APA [American Psychological Association] is to advance the creation, communication and application of psychological knowledge to benefit society and improve people’s lives»; APA (2010). About APA. Archived 2 September 2017 at the Wayback Machine Retrieved 20 October 2010.
  6. ^ Farberow NL, Eiduson B (1971). «To petition to join APA as a section of Division 12, the Division of Clinical Psychology». Journal of Personality Assessment. Taylor & Francis Online. 35 (3): 205–206. doi:10.1080/00223891.1971.10119654. Archived from the original on 3 March 2022. Retrieved 2 March 2022. Clinical psychology is the practice of psychology, especially as a means of furthering human welfare and knowledge.
  7. ^ Bureau of Labor Statistics, U.S. Department of Labor, Occupational Outlook Handbook, 2010–11 Edition, Psychologists, on the Internet at bls.gov Archived 4 January 2012 at the Wayback Machine (visited 8 July 2010).
  8. ^ Online Etymology Dictionary. (2001). «Psychology» Archived 18 July 2017 at the Wayback Machine.
  9. ^ «Classics in the History of Psychology – Marko Marulic – The Author of the Term «Psychology»«. Psychclassics.yorku.ca. Archived from the original on 20 January 2017. Retrieved 10 December 2011.
  10. ^ (Steven Blankaart, p. 13) as quoted in «psychology n.» A Dictionary of Psychology. Edited by Andrew M. Colman. Oxford University Press 2009. Oxford Reference Online. Oxford University Press. oxfordreference.com Archived 15 September 2019 at the Wayback Machine
  11. ^ a b James, William (1890). The principles of psychology. Cambridge, Mass: Harvard University Press. ISBN 0-674-70625-0. OCLC 9557883.
  12. ^ a b c Watson, John B. (1913). «Psychology as the Behaviorist Views It» (PDF). Psychological Review. 20 (2): 158–177. doi:10.1037/h0074428. hdl:21.11116/0000-0001-9182-7. Archived (PDF) from the original on 8 January 2016. Retrieved 24 April 2015.
  13. ^ Derek Russell Davis (DRD), «psychology», in Richard L. Gregory (ed.), The Oxford Companion to the Mind, second edition; Oxford University Press, 1987/2004; ISBN 978-0-19-866224-2 (pp. 763–764).
  14. ^ The term «folk psychology» is itself contentious: see Daniel D. Hutto & Matthew Ratcliffe (eds.), Folk Psychology Re-Assessed; Dorndrecht, the Netherlands: Springer, 2007; ISBN 978-1-4020-5557-7
  15. ^ Okasha, Ahmed (2005). «Mental Health in Egypt». The Israel Journal of Psychiatry and Related Sciences. 42 (2): 116–25. PMID 16342608.
  16. ^ «Aristotle’s Psychology Archived 9 July 2010 at the Wayback Machine». Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy.
  17. ^ Green, C.D. & Groff, P.R. (2003). Early psychological thought: Ancient accounts of mind and soul. Westport, Connecticut: Praeger.
  18. ^ T.L. Brink. (2008) Psychology: A Student Friendly Approach. «Unit One: The Definition and History of Psychology.» pp 9 [1] Archived 24 July 2012 at the Wayback Machine.
  19. ^ «Psychology: Definitions, branches, history, and how to become one». www.medicalnewstoday.com. 1 February 2018. Archived from the original on 20 March 2021. Retrieved 20 September 2021.
  20. ^ a b Yeh Hsueh and Benyu Guo, «China», in Baker (ed.), Oxford Handbook of the History of Psychology (2012).
  21. ^ a b c Anand C. Paranjpe, «From Tradition through Colonialism to Globalization: Reflections on the History of Psychology in India», in Brock (ed.), Internationalizing the History of Psychology (2006).
  22. ^ PT Raju (1985), Structural Depths of Indian Thought, State University of New York Press, ISBN 978-0887061394, pages 35-36
  23. ^ a b Schwarz, Katharina A.; Pfister, Roland (2016). «Scientific psychology in the 18th century: A historical rediscovery». Perspectives on Psychological Science. SAGE Publications. 11 (3): 399–407. doi:10.1177/1745691616635601. ISSN 1745-6916. PMID 27217252. S2CID 6784135.
  24. ^ a b c d e Horst U.K. Gundlach, «Germany», in Baker (ed.), Oxford Handbook of the History of Psychology (2012).
  25. ^ Alan Collins, «England», in Baker (ed.), Oxford Handbook of the History of Psychology (2012).
  26. ^ a b Henley TB (2019). Hergenhahn’s An introduction to the history of psychology (8th ed.). Boston: Cengage. pp. 143–145. ISBN 978-1-337-56415-1. Archived from the original on 30 July 2022. Retrieved 4 March 2022.
  27. ^ a b c d e f g Leahey, Thomas (2001). A history of modern psychology (Third ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. ISBN 978-0-13-017573-1. OCLC 43657139.
  28. ^ Fechner, G. T. (1860). Elemente der Psychophysik. Breitkopf u. Härtel. (Elements of Psychophysics)
  29. ^ Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. (2006). «Wilhelm Maximilian Wundt» Archived 8 August 2019 at the Wayback Machine.
  30. ^ Wozniak, R.H. (1999). Introduction to memory: Hermann Ebbinghaus (1885/1913). Classics in the history of psychology Archived 6 June 2019 at the Wayback Machine
  31. ^ a b c d e f Ludy T. Benjamin, Jr., and David B. Baker, «The Internationalization of Psychology: A History», in Baker (ed.), Oxford Handbook of the History of Psychology (2012).
  32. ^ a b c Miki Takasuna, «Japan», in Baker (ed.), Oxford Handbook of the History of Psychology (2012).
  33. ^ a b c d C. James Goodwin, «United States», in Baker (ed.), Oxford Handbook of the History of Psychology (2012).
  34. ^ Cecilia Taiana, «Transatlantic Migration of the Disciplines of Mind: Examination of the Reception of Wundt’s and Freud’s Theories in Argentina», in Brock (ed.), Internationalizing the History of Psychology (2006).
  35. ^ a b c d Irina Sirotkina and Roger Smith, «Russian Federation», in Baker (ed.), Oxford Handbook of the History of Psychology (2012).
  36. ^ Windholz, G. (1997). «Ivan P. Pavlov: An overview of his life and psychological work». American Psychologist. 52 (9): 941–946. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.52.9.941.
  37. ^ a b c d e f Nancy Tomes, «The Development of Clinical Psychology, Social Work, and Psychiatric Nursing: 1900–1980s», in Wallace & Gach (eds.), History of Psychiatry and Medical Psychology (2008).
  38. ^ Franz Samuelson, «Organizing for the Kingdom of Behavior: Academic Battles and the Organizational Policies in the Twenties»; Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 21, January 1985.
  39. ^ Hans Pols, «The World as Laboratory: Strategies of Field Research Developed by Mental Hygiene Psychologists in Toronto, 1920–1940» in Theresa Richardson & Donald Fisher (eds.), The Development of the Social Sciences in the United States and Canada: The Role of Philanthropy; Stamford, CT: Ablex Publishing, 1999; ISBN 1-56750-405-1
  40. ^ Sol Cohen, «The Mental Hygiene Movement, the Development of Personality and the School: The Medicalization of American Education»; History of Education Quarterly 23.2, Summer 1983.
  41. ^ Vern L. Bullough, «The Rockefellers and Sex Research»; Journal of Sex Research 21.2, May 1985. «Their importance is hard to overestimate. In fact, in the period between 1914 and 1954, the Rockefellers were almost the sole support of sex research in the United States. The decisions made by their scientific advisers about the nature of the research to be supported and how it was conducted, as well as the topics eligible for research support, shaped the whole field of sex research and, in many ways, still continue to support it.»
  42. ^ a b Guthrie, Even the Rat was White (1998), Chapter 4: «Psychology and Race» (pp. 88–110). «Psychology courses often became the vehicles for eugenics propaganda. One graduate of the Record Office training program wrote, ‘I hope to serve the cause by infiltrating eugenics into the minds of teachers. It may interest you to know that each student who takes psychology here works up his family history and plots his family tree.’ Harvard, Columbia, Brown, Cornell, Wisconsin, and Northwestern were among the leading academic institutions teaching eugenics in psychology courses.»
  43. ^ Michell, J, (1999) Measurement in Psychology: A Critical History of a Methodological Concept Archived 9 February 2021 at the Wayback Machine, p.143
  44. ^ Dorwin Cartwright, «Social Psychology in the United States During the Second World War», Human Relations 1.3, June 1948, p. 340; quoted in Cina, «Social Science For Whom?» (1981), p. 269.
  45. ^ a b c d Schonfeld, I.S., & Chang, C.-H. (2017). Occupational health psychology: Work, stress, and health. New York, NY: Springer Publishing Company.
  46. ^ Catherine Lutz, «Epistemology of the Bunker: The Brainwashed and Other New Subjects of Permanent War Archived 19 September 2015 at the Wayback Machine», in Joel Pfister & Nancy Schnog (eds.), Inventing the Psychological: Toward a Cultural History of Emotional Life in America; Yale University Press, 1997; ISBN 0-300-06809-3
  47. ^ Cina, «Social Science For Whom?» (1981), pp. 315–325.
  48. ^ Herman, «Psychology as Politics» (1993), p. 288. «Had it come to fruition, CAMELOT would have been the largest, and certainly the most generously funded, behavioral research project in U.S. history. With a $4–6 million contract over a period of 3 years, it was considered, and often called, a veritable Manhattan Project for the behavioral sciences, at least by many of the intellectuals whose services were in heavy demand.»
  49. ^ Cocks, Psychotherapy in the Third Reich (1997), pp. 75–77.
  50. ^ Cocks, Psychotherapy in the Third Reich (1997), p. 93.
  51. ^ Cocks, Psychotherapy in the Third Reich (1997), pp. 86–87. «For Schultz-Hencke in this 1934 essay, life goals were determined by ideology, not by science. In the case of psychotherapy, he defined health in terms of blood, strong will, proficiency, discipline, (Zucht und Ordnung), community, heroic bearing, and physical fitness. Schultz-Hencke also took the opportunity in 1934 to criticize psychoanalysis for providing an unfortunate tendency toward the exculpation of the criminal.»
  52. ^ Jürgen Brunner, Matthias Schrempf, & Florian Steger, «Johannes Heinrich Schultz and National Socialism Archived 12 September 2014 at the Wayback Machine», Israel Journal of Psychiatry & Related Sciences 45.4, 2008. «Bringing these people to a right and deep understanding of every German’s duty in the New Germany, such as preparatory mental aid and psychotherapy in general and in particular for persons to be sterilized, and for people having been sterilized, is a great, important and rewarding medical duty.»
  53. ^ Cocks, Psychotherapy in the Third Reich (1997), Chapter 14: «Reconstruction and Repression», pp. 351–375.
  54. ^ a b c d e Kozulin, Alex (1984). Psychology in Utopia: toward a social history of Soviet psychology. Cambridge, Mass: MIT Press. ISBN 0-262-11087-3. OCLC 10122631.
  55. ^ c.f. Hannah Proctor, «Reason Displaces All Love Archived 27 May 2015 at the Wayback Machine», The New Inquiry, 14 February 2014.
  56. ^ a b c Chin, Robert; Chin, Ai-li S. (1969). Psychological research in Communist China, 1949-1966. Cambridge, Mass: M.I.T. Press. ISBN 978-0-262-03032-8. OCLC 192767.
  57. ^ a b c d e f g h Wade Pickren & Raymond D. Fowler, «Professional Organizations», in Weiner (ed.), Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 1: History of Psychology.
  58. ^ a b Irmingard Staeuble, «Psychology in the Eurocentric Order of the Social Sciences: Colonial Constitution, Cultural Imperialist Expansion, Postcolonial Critique» in Brock (ed.), Internationalizing the History of Psychology (2006).
  59. ^ For example, see Oregon State Law, Chapter 675 (2013 edition) at Statutes & Rules Relating to the Practice of Psychology Archived 21 May 2016 at the Portuguese Web Archive.
  60. ^ a b c Judy E. Hall and George Hurley, «North American Perspectives on Education, Training, Licensing, and Credentialing», in Weiner (ed.), Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 8: Clinical Psychology.
  61. ^ T.S. Kuhn, The Structure of Scientific Revolutions, 1st. ed., Chicago: Univ. of Chicago Pr., 1962.
  62. ^ Beveridge, Allan (2002). «Time to abandon the subjective–objective divide?». Psychiatric Bulletin. 26 (3): 101–103. doi:10.1192/pb.26.3.101.
  63. ^ Peterson, C. (23 May 2009). «Subjective and objective research in positive psychology: A biological characteristic is linked to well-being» Archived 30 July 2022 at the Wayback Machine. Psychology Today. Retrieved 20 April 2010.
  64. ^ Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions Archived 18 September 2015 at the Wayback Machine. New York: Oxford University Press, p. 9.
  65. ^ a b Teo, Thomas (2005). The critique of psychology: from Kant to postcolonial theory. New York: Springer. ISBN 978-0-387-25355-8. OCLC 209833302.
  66. ^ Michela Gallagher & Randy J. Nelson, «Volume Preface», in Weiner (ed.), Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 3: Biological Psychology.
  67. ^ a b Richard F. Thompson & Stuart M. Zola, «Biological Psychology», in Weiner (ed.), Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 1: History of Psychology.
  68. ^ Luria, A. R. (1973). The working brain: an introduction to neuropsychology. Translated by Haigh, Basil. New York: Basic Books. ISBN 0-465-09208-X. OCLC 832187.
  69. ^ Pinel, John (2010). Biopsychology. New York: Prentice Hall. ISBN 978-0-205-83256-9.
  70. ^ Richard Frankel; Timothy Quill; Susan McDaniel (2003). The Biopsychosocial Approach: Past, Present, Future. Boydell & Brewer. ISBN 978-1-58046-102-3.
  71. ^ a b McGue M, Gottesman II (2015). «Behavior Genetics». The Encyclopedia of Clinical Psychology. pp. 1–11. doi:10.1002/9781118625392.wbecp578. ISBN 978-1-118-62539-2.
  72. ^ Guthrie, Even the Rat was White (1998), Chapter 1: «‘The Noble Savage’ and Science» (pp. 3–33)
  73. ^ Guthrie, Even the Rat was White (1998), Chapter 5: «The Psychology of Survival and Education» (pp. 113–134)
  74. ^ Guthrie, Even the Rat was White (1998), Chapter 2: «Brass Instruments and Dark Skins» (pp. 34–54)
  75. ^ J.B. Watson & R. Rayner, «Conditioned emotional responses», Journal of Experimental Psychology 3, 1920; in Hock, Forty Studies (2002), pp. 70–76.
  76. ^ Harris, B. (1979). Whatever happened to Little Albert? American Psychologist, 34, 151-160. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.34.2.151 Archived 30 July 2022 at the Wayback Machine https://archive.today/20120803155410/http://htpprints.yorku.ca/archive/00000198/01/BHARRIS.HTM#selection-15.0-15.35
  77. ^ Overskeid, Geir (2007). «Looking for Skinner and finding Freud». American Psychologist. 62 (6): 590–595. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.321.6288. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.62.6.590. PMID 17874899.
  78. ^ Miller, S.; Konorski, J. (1928). «Sur une forme particulière des reflexes conditionels» [On a particular form of conditional reflexes]. Comptes Rendus des Séances de la Société de Biologie et de Ses Filiales (in French). 99: 1155–1157.
  79. ^ Skinner, B.F. (1932) The Behavior of Organisms[page needed]
  80. ^ Chomsky, N. (1959). Review of Skinner’s Verbal Behavior. Language, 35, 26–58. [2] Archived 29 September 2015 at the Wayback Machine
  81. ^ a b Schlinger, H.D. (2008). «The long good-bye: why B.F. Skinner’s Verbal Behavior is alive and well on the 50th anniversary of its publication». The Psychological Record. 58 (3): 329–337. doi:10.1007/BF03395622. S2CID 18114690. Archived from the original on 17 January 2020. Retrieved 21 July 2019.
  82. ^ Seligman M.E.P.; Maier S.F. (1967). «Failure to escape traumatic shock». Journal of Experimental Psychology. 74 (1): 1–9. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.611.8411. doi:10.1037/h0024514. PMID 6032570.
  83. ^ Overmier J.B.; Seligman M.E.P. (1967). «Effects of inescapable shock upon subsequent escape and avoidance responding». Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 63 (1): 28–33. doi:10.1037/h0024166. PMID 6029715. S2CID 17310110. Archived from the original on 30 July 2022. Retrieved 7 January 2020.
  84. ^ Tolman, Edward C. (1948). «Cognitive maps in rats and men». Psychological Review. 55 (4): 189–208. doi:10.1037/h0061626. PMID 18870876. S2CID 42496633. Archived from the original on 30 July 2022. Retrieved 7 January 2020.
  85. ^ Ruben Ardila, «Behavior Analysis in an International Context», in Brock (ed.), Internationalizing the History of Psychology (2006).
  86. ^ Pierce, W. David; Cheney, Carl D. (16 June 2017) [1995]. Behavior Analysis and Learning: A Biobehavioral Approach (6 ed.). New York: Routledge. pp. 1–622. ISBN 978-1138898585. Archived from the original on 3 June 2021. Retrieved 3 June 2021.
  87. ^ «American Psychological Association (2013). Glossary of psychological terms». Apa.org. Archived from the original on 27 January 2019. Retrieved 13 August 2014.
  88. ^ Gardner, H. (1985). The mind’s new science: A history of the cognitive revolution. New York: Basic Books. ISBN 0-465-04635-5
  89. ^ a b Mandler, G. (2007). A history of modern experimental psychology: From James and Wundt to cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.[page needed]
  90. ^ Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A social learning analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.
  91. ^ Juslin, Peter (2013), «Availability Heuristic», Encyclopedia of the Mind, SAGE Publications, Inc., doi:10.4135/9781452257044.n39, ISBN 978-1-4129-5057-2
  92. ^ Thagard, Paul (2020), «Cognitive Science», in Zalta, Edward N. (ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Winter 2020 ed.), Metaphysics Research Lab, Stanford University, archived from the original on 1 March 2021, retrieved 22 January 2021
  93. ^ Allport, G. W (1985). «The Historical Background of Social Psychology». In G. Lindzey and E. Aronson (ed.). The Handbook of Social Psychology. New York: McGraw Hill. p. 5.
  94. ^ Tausig, M., & Fenwick, R. (2011). Work and mental health in social context. New York: Springer. doi:10.1007/978-1-4614-0625-9
  95. ^ Thompson, C. & Mullahy, P. (1951). Psychoanalysis: Evolution and development (3rd ed.). New York: Hermitage House.
  96. ^ Brenner, C. (1974). An elementary textbook of psychoanalysis. Garden City, NY: Anchor.
  97. ^ Moore, B.E.; Fine, B.D. (1968), A Glossary of Psychoanalytic Terms and Concepts, Amer Psychoanalytic Assn, p. 78, ISBN 978-0-318-13125-2
  98. ^ Freud, S (1900). The Interpretation of Dreams. Vol. IV and V (2nd ed.). Hogarth Press, 1955.
  99. ^ Freud, S (1915). The Unconscious. Vol. XIV (2nd ed.). Hogarth Press, 1955.
  100. ^ Karl Popper, Conjectures and Refutations, London: Routledge and Keagan Paul, 1963, pp. 33–39; from Theodore Schick, ed., Readings in the Philosophy of Science, Mountain View, CA: Mayfield Publishing Company, 2000, pp. 9–13. Faculty.washington.edu Archived 26 March 2009 at the Wayback Machine
  101. ^ Cohen, Patricia (25 November 2007). «June 2008 study». The New York Times. Archived from the original on 30 May 2012. Retrieved 23 February 2017.
  102. ^ See:
    • Damásio, A. (1994). Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain.
    • Damásio, A. (1996). The somatic marker hypothesis and the possible functions of the prefrontal cortex.
    • Damásio, A. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness.
    • Damásio, A. (2003). Looking for Spinoza: Joy, sorrow, and the feeling brain.
    • LeDoux, J.E. (1998). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life (Touchstone ed.). Simon & Schuster. ISBN 0-684-83659-9
    • Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. New York and Oxford: Oxford University Press.
    • Sacks, O. (1984). A leg to stand on. New York: Summit Books/Simon and Schuster.

  103. ^ «Maslow’s Hierarchy of Needs». Honolulu.hawaii.edu. Archived from the original on 11 February 2010. Retrieved 10 December 2011.
  104. ^ Benjafield, John G. (2010). A History of Psychology: Third Edition. Don Mills, ON: Oxford University Press. pp. 357–362. ISBN 978-0-19-543021-9.
  105. ^ a b Oxford University Press. (2015). A Dictionary of Psychology, 4th ed. Edited by Andrew M. Colman. Oxford Reference Online. Oxford: Author. ISBN 9780199657681
  106. ^ Gazzaniga, Michael (2010). Psychological Science. New York: W.W. Norton & Company. p. 23. ISBN 978-0-393-93421-2.
  107. ^ Rowan, John. (2001). Ordinary Ecstasy: The Dialectics of Humanistic Psychology. London, UK: Brunner-Routledge. ISBN 0-415-23633-9
  108. ^ Ehrenreich, B. (2009). Bright-sided: How the relentless promotion of positive thinking has undermined America. New York: Henry Holt. ISBN 978-0-8050-8749-9
  109. ^ Singal, J. (2021, June 7). Positive psychology goes to war: How the Army adopted an untested, evidence-free approach to fighting PTSD. Chronicle of Higher Education.
  110. ^ A.J. Sutich, American association for humanistic psychology, Articles of association. Palo Alto, CA (mimeographed): August 28, 1963; in Severin (ed.), Humanistic Viewpoints in Psychology (1965), pp. xv–xvi.
  111. ^ Hergenhahn, B.R. (2005). An introduction to the history of psychology. Belmont, California: Thomson Wadsworth. pp. 528–536.
  112. ^ Hergenhahn, B.R. (2005). An introduction to the history of psychology. Belmont, California: Thomson Wadsworth. pp. 546–547.
  113. ^ Hergenhahn, B.R. (2005). An introduction to the history of psychology. Belmont, California: Thomson Wadsworth. pp. 523–532.
  114. ^ Frankl, V.E. (1984). Man’s search for meaning (rev. ed.). New York: Washington Square Press. p. 86.
  115. ^ Seidner, Stanley S. (10 June 2009) «A Trojan Horse: Logotherapeutic Transcendence and its Secular Implications for Theology» Archived 1 May 2011 at the Wayback Machine. Mater Dei Institute. p 2.
  116. ^ Carver, C., & Scheier, M. (2004). Perspectives on Personality (5th ed.). Boston: Pearson.
  117. ^ Cattell, R.B. (1995). «The fallacy of five factors in the personality sphere». The Psychologist, May, 207–208.
  118. ^ Cattell, Raymond B.; Nichols, K. Ernest (1972). «An Improved Definition, from 10 Researchers, of Second Order Personality Factors in Q Data (with Cross-Cultural Checks)». The Journal of Social Psychology. 86 (2): 187–203. doi:10.1080/00224545.1972.9918617.
  119. ^ Block, Jack (1995). «A contrarian view of the five-factor approach to personality description». Psychological Bulletin. 117 (2): 187–215. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.117.2.187. PMID 7724687.
  120. ^ Boyle, G.J. (2008). Critique of Five-Factor Model (FFM). In G.J. Boyle, G. Matthews, & D.H. Saklofske. (Eds.), ThBy coe SAGE handbook of personality theory and assessment: Vol. 1 – Personality theories and models. Los Angeles, CA: SAGE. ISBN 978-1-4129-4651-3
  121. ^ Lucas, Richard E.; Baird, Brendan M. (2004). «Extraversion and Emotional Reactivity». Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 86 (3): 473–485. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.86.3.473. PMID 15008650.
  122. ^ Boyle, G.J. (2011). Changes in personality traits in adulthood. In D. Westen, L. Burton, & R. Kowalski (Eds.), Psychology: Australian and New Zealand 3rd edition (pp. 448–449). Milton, Queensland: Wiley. ISBN 978-1-74216-644-5
  123. ^ Cattell, Raymond B.; Boyle, Gregory J.; Chant, David (2002). «Enriched behavioral prediction equation and its impact on structured learning and the dynamic calculus». Psychological Review. 109 (1): 202–205. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.109.1.202. PMID 11863038.
  124. ^ Boyle, Gregory J. (1995). «Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI): Some Psychometric Limitations». Australian Psychologist. 30: 71–74. doi:10.1111/j.1742-9544.1995.tb01750.x.
  125. ^ Leslie C. Morey, «Measuring Personality and Psychopathology» in Weiner (ed.), Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 2: Research Methods in Psychology.
  126. ^ Gough, H.G. (1987) California Psychological Inventory Administrator’s Guide. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press, Inc.
  127. ^ Goldberg, L. R., Johnson, J. A., Eber, H. W., Hogan, R., Ashton, M. C., Cloninger, C. R., & Gough, H. C. (2006). The International Personality Item Pool and the future of public-domain personality measures. Journal of Research in Personality, 40, 84-96.[3] Archived 20 August 2019 at the Wayback Machine
  128. ^ Charles Sanders Peirce & Joseph Jastrow, «On Small Differences in Sensation Archived 9 June 2019 at the Wayback Machine», Memoirs of the National Academy of Sciences 3, 17 October 1884; cited in William P. Banks & Ilya Farber, «Consciousness», in Weiner (ed.), Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 4: Experimental Psychology; and in Deber, James A; Jacoby, Larry L. (1994). «Unconscious Perception: Attention, Awareness, and Control». Journal of Experimental Psychology. 20 (2): 304–317. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.412.4083. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.20.2.304. PMID 8151275.
  129. ^ The full text of The Interpretation of Dreams at Wikisource, a faithful copy of the third edition translated in English by Abraham Arden Brill and published in 1913 by The Macmillan Company
  130. ^ a b John F. Kihlstrom, «The Psychological Unconscious Archived 19 September 2015 at the Wayback Machine», in Lawrence Pervin & Oliver John (eds.), Handbook of Personality; New York: Guilford Press, 1999. Also see web version Archived 9 October 2016 at the Wayback Machine.
  131. ^ William P. Banks & Ilya Farber, «Consciousness», in Weiner (ed.), Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 4: Experimental Psychology.[page needed]
  132. ^ a b Bargh, John A.; Chartrand, Tanya L. (1999). «The unbearable automaticity of being». American Psychologist. 54 (7): 462–479. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.462. S2CID 5726030. Archived from the original on 30 July 2022. Retrieved 7 January 2020. Also see: John A. Bargh, «The Automaticity of Everyday Life», in Robert S. Wyer Jr. (ed.), The Automaticity of Everyday Life, Advances in Social Cognition, Volume X; Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 1997; ISBN 978-0-8058-1699-0[page needed]
  133. ^ a b John F. Kihlstrom, «The Automaticity Juggernaut—or, Are We Automatons After All? Archived 10 March 2015 at the Wayback Machine», in John Baer, James C. Kaufmna, & Roy F. Baumeister (eds.), Are We Free? Psychology and Free Will; Oxford University Press, 2008. ISBN 978-0-19-518963-6
  134. ^ S. Doyen, O. Klein, C. L. Pichon and A. Cleeremans. (2012). Behavioral priming: it’s all in the mind, but whose mind? PLoS One, 7, e29081 Archived 30 July 2022 at the Wayback Machine
  135. ^ Pashler, H; Harris, C; Coburn, N (15 September 2011). «Elderly-Related Words Prime Slow Walking». psychfiledrawer.org. Archived from the original on 21 October 2016. Retrieved 17 October 2016.
  136. ^ Soon, Chun Siong; Brass, Marcel; Heinze, Hans-Jochen; Haynes, John-Dylan (2008). «Unconscious determinants of free decisions in the human brain». Nature Neuroscience. 11 (5): 543–545. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.520.2204. doi:10.1038/nn.2112. PMID 18408715. S2CID 2652613.
  137. ^ Baumeister, Roy F. (2008). «Free Will in Scientific Psychology». Perspectives on Psychological Science. 3 (1): 14–19. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.476.102. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00057.x. PMID 26158665. S2CID 9630921.
  138. ^ a b Forgas, Williams, & Laham, «Social Motivation: Introduction and Overview», in Forgas, Williams, & Laham, Social Motivation (2005).
  139. ^ Weiner, Human Motivation (2013), Chapter 2, «The Psychoanalytic Theory of Motivation» (pp. 9–84).
  140. ^ a b c Bill P. Godsil, Matthew R. Tinsley, & Michael S. Fanselow, «Motivation», in Weiner (ed.), Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 4: Experimental Psychology.
  141. ^ Weiner, Human Motivation (2013), Chapter 3, «Drive Theory» (pp. 85–138).
  142. ^ E. Tory Higgins, Beyond Pleasure and Pain: How Motivation Works; Oxford University Press, 2012; ISBN 978-0-19-976582-9[page needed]
  143. ^ Shah & Gardner, Handbook of Motivation Science (2008), entire volume.[page needed]
  144. ^ Hank Aarts, Ap Dijksterhuis, & Giel Dik, «Goal Contagion: Inferring goals from others’ actions—and what it leads to», in Shah & Gardner, Handbook of Motivation Science (2008). «In short, then, the studies presented above indicate that humans are keen to act on the goals of other social beings that are implied in behavioral scenarios or scripts.» Also see: Aarts; Hassin; Gollwitzer (2004). «Goal Contagion: Perceiving is for Pursuing». Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 87 (1): 23–37. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.312.5507. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.87.1.23. PMID 15250790.
  145. ^ Kathleen D. Vohs & Roy F. Baumeister, «Can Satisfaction Reinforce Wanting? A new theory about long-term changes in strength of motivation», in Shah & Gardner, Handbook of Motivation Science (2008).
  146. ^ a b Crain, W. (2014). Theories of development: Concepts and applications. 6th ed. Edinburgh: Pearson. ISBN 978-0205810468
  147. ^ «Developmental Psychology | Simply Psychology». www.simplypsychology.org. Archived from the original on 29 October 2021. Retrieved 15 October 2021.
  148. ^ Lionetti, F., Aron, E. N., Aron, A., Klein, D. N., & Pluess, M. (2019). Observer-rated environmental sensitivity moderates children’s response to parenting quality in early childhood. Developmental Psychology, 55, 2389–2402. doi:10.1037/dev0000795.supp (Supplemental)
  149. ^ Schonfeld, I. S. (1986). The Genevan and Cattell-Horn conceptions of intelligence compared: The early implementation of numerical solution aids. Developmental Psychology, 22, 204–212. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.22.2.204 [4] Archived 14 April 2021 at the Wayback Machine
  150. ^ Hunnius, S., & Bekkering, H. (2010). The early development of object knowledge: A study of infants’ visual anticipations during action observation. Developmental Psychology, 46, 446–454. doi:10.1037/a0016543
  151. ^ Finkel, D., Reynolds, C. A., McArdle, J. J., Gatz, M., & Pedersen, N. L. (2003). Latent growth curve analyses of accelerating decline in cognitive abilities in late adulthood. Developmental Psychology, 39, 535–550. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.39.3.535
  152. ^ Polderman, Tinca J C.; Benyamin, Beben; De Leeuw, Christiaan A.; Sullivan, Patrick F.; Van Bochoven, Arjen; Visscher, Peter M.; Posthuma, Danielle (2015). «Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies» (PDF). Nature Genetics. 47 (7): 702–709. doi:10.1038/ng.3285. PMID 25985137. S2CID 205349969.
  153. ^ Turkheimer, Eric (2000). «Three Laws of Behavior Genetics and What They Mean». Current Directions in Psychological Science. 9 (5): 160–164. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00084. S2CID 2861437.
  154. ^ Visscher, Peter M.; Brown, Matthew A.; McCarthy, Mark I.; Yang, Jian (2012). «Five Years of GWAS Discovery». The American Journal of Human Genetics. 90 (1): 7–24. doi:10.1016/j.ajhg.2011.11.029. PMC 3257326. PMID 22243964.
  155. ^ Ripke, Stephan; Neale, Benjamin M.; Corvin, Aiden; Walters, James T. R.; Farh, Kai-How; Holmans, Peter A.; Lee, Phil; Bulik-Sullivan, Brendan; Collier, David A.; Huang, Hailiang; Pers, Tune H.; Agartz, Ingrid; Agerbo, Esben; Albus, Margot; Alexander, Madeline; Amin, Farooq; Bacanu, Silviu A.; Begemann, Martin; Belliveau, Richard A.; Bene, Judit; Bergen, Sarah E.; Bevilacqua, Elizabeth; Bigdeli, Tim B.; Black, Donald W.; Bruggeman, Richard; Buccola, Nancy G.; Buckner, Randy L.; Byerley, William; Cahn, Wiepke; et al. (2014). «Biological insights from 108 schizophrenia-associated genetic loci». Nature. 511 (7510): 421–427. Bibcode:2014Natur.511..421S. doi:10.1038/nature13595. PMC 4112379. PMID 25056061.
  156. ^ Lee, S Hong; Decandia, Teresa R.; Ripke, Stephan; Yang, Jian; Sullivan, Patrick F.; Goddard, Michael E.; Keller, Matthew C.; Visscher, Peter M.; Wray, Naomi R. (2012). «Estimating the proportion of variation in susceptibility to schizophrenia captured by common SNPs». Nature Genetics. 44 (3): 247–250. doi:10.1038/ng.1108. PMC 3327879. PMID 22344220.
  157. ^ Sullivan, Patrick F.; Daly, Mark J.; O’Donovan, Michael (2012). «Genetic architectures of psychiatric disorders: The emerging picture and its implications». Nature Reviews Genetics. 13 (8): 537–551. doi:10.1038/nrg3240. PMC 4110909. PMID 22777127.
  158. ^ De Moor, Marleen H. M.; Van Den Berg, Stéphanie M.; Verweij, Karin J. H.; Krueger, Robert F.; Luciano, Michelle; Arias Vasquez, Alejandro; Matteson, Lindsay K.; Derringer, Jaime; Esko, Tõnu; Amin, Najaf; Gordon, Scott D.; Hansell, Narelle K.; Hart, Amy B.; Seppälä, Ilkka; Huffman, Jennifer E.; Konte, Bettina; Lahti, Jari; Lee, Minyoung; Miller, Mike; Nutile, Teresa; Tanaka, Toshiko; Teumer, Alexander; Viktorin, Alexander; Wedenoja, Juho; Abecasis, Goncalo R.; Adkins, Daniel E.; Agrawal, Arpana; Allik, Jüri; Appel, Katja; et al. (2015). «Meta-analysis of Genome-wide Association Studies for Neuroticism, and the Polygenic Association with Major Depressive Disorder». JAMA Psychiatry. 72 (7): 642–50. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0554. PMC 4667957. PMID 25993607.
  159. ^ a b c d e f g h i Gregory, Robert (2011). Psychological testing: history, principles, and applications (Sixth ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. ISBN 978-0-205-78214-7. OCLC 620302854.
  160. ^ a b Guthrie, Even the Rat was White (1998), Chapter 3: «Psychometric Scientism» (pp. 55–87)
  161. ^ «Army Alpha and Beta tests». Oxford Reference. Archived from the original on 19 January 2022. Retrieved 18 January 2022.
  162. ^ Berry, Robert M. (2012). «From Involuntary Sterilization to Genetic Enhancement: The Unsettled Legacy of Buck v. Bell». Notre Dame Journal of Law, Ethics, & Public Policy. 12. Archived from the original on 4 March 2016. Retrieved 24 April 2015.
  163. ^ Neal, Tess M.S. (2018). «Forensic psychology and correctional psychology: Distinct but related subfields of psychological science and practice». American Psychologist. 73 (5): 651–662. doi:10.1037/amp0000227. ISSN 1935-990X. PMID 29431456. S2CID 46817929.
  164. ^ Nunnally, J. C., & Bernstein, I. H. (1994). Psychometric theory, 3rd ed., New York: McGraw-Hill.
  165. ^ Embretson, S.E., & Reise, S.P. (2000). Item Response Theory for Psychologists. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
  166. ^ Kline, R. B. (2016). Principles and practice of structural equation modeling, 4th ed.New York: Guilford Press.
  167. ^ Rodriguez, A., Reise, S. P., & Haviland, M. G. (2016). Evaluating bifactor models: Calculating and interpreting statistical indices. Psychological Methods, 21, 137–150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/met0000045 Archived 28 July 2020 at the Wayback Machine
  168. ^ a b George Stricker & Thomas A. Widiger, «Volume Preface», in Weiner (ed.), Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 8: Clinical Psychology.
  169. ^ Brain, Christine. (2002). Advanced psychology: applications, issues and perspectives. Cheltenham: Nelson Thornes. ISBN 0-17-490058-9
  170. ^ Nancy McWilliams and Joel Weinberger, «Psychodynamic Psychotherapy», in Weiner (ed.), Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 8: Clinical Psychology.
  171. ^ W. Edward Craighead & Linda Wilcoxon Craighead, «Behavioral and Cognitive-Behavioral Psychotherapy» in Weiner (ed.), Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 8: Clinical Psychology.
  172. ^ Teri L. Elliott, «Disaster Psychology: Keep Clients out of Your Office—Get into the Field!» in Morgan et al. (ed.), Life After Graduate School in Psychology (2005). «…it is the disaster psychologist’s role to utilize crisis intervention processes with the goal of preventing natural distress due to the critical event from developing into a more harmful, long-term psychological condition.»
  173. ^ Leichsenring, Falk; Leibing, Eric (2003). «The effectiveness of psychodynamic therapy and cognitive behavior therapy in the treatment of personality disorders: A meta-analysis». The American Journal of Psychiatry. 160 (7): 1223–1233. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.7.1223. PMID 12832233.
  174. ^ Reisner, Andrew (2005). «The common factors, empirically validated treatments, and recovery models of therapeutic change». The Psychological Record. 55 (3): 377–400. doi:10.1007/BF03395517. S2CID 142840311. Archived from the original on 6 August 2020. Retrieved 21 July 2019.
  175. ^ Jensen, J.P.; Bergin, A.E.; Greaves, D.W. (1990). «The meaning of eclecticism: New survey and analysis of components». Professional Psychology: Research and Practice. 21 (2): 124–130. doi:10.1037/0735-7028.21.2.124.
  176. ^ Palmer, S.; Woolfe, R. (eds.) (1999). Integrative and eclectic counselling and psychotherapy. London: Sage.
  177. ^ Clarkson, P. (1996). The eclectic and integrative paradigm: Between the Scylla of confluence and the Charybdis of confusion. In Handbook of Counselling Psychology (R. Woolfe & W.L. Dryden, eds.). London: Sage, pp. 258–283. ISBN 0-8039-8991-1
  178. ^ Goldfried, M.R.; Wolfe, B.E. (1998). «Toward a more clinically valid approach to therapy research» (PDF). Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 66 (1): 143–150. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.475.7156. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.66.1.143. PMID 9489268. Archived (PDF) from the original on 9 August 2017. Retrieved 25 October 2017.
  179. ^ Seligman, M.E.P. (1995). «The effectiveness of psychotherapy: The Consumer Reports study» (PDF). American Psychologist. 50 (12): 965–974. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.50.12.965. PMID 8561380. Archived from the original (PDF) on 21 July 2018. Retrieved 25 October 2017.
  180. ^ Peter E. Nathan & James Langenbucher, «Diagnosis and Classification», in Weiner (ed.), Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 8: Clinical Psychology.
  181. ^ «What Does An Educational Psychologist Do | Touro University | Psychology». Touro University WorldWide. Archived from the original on 30 September 2021. Retrieved 30 September 2021.
  182. ^ National Association of School Psychologists. «Who are school psychologists?». Archived from the original on 17 May 2008. Retrieved 1 June 2008.
  183. ^ Truxillo, D. M., Bauer, T. N., & Erdogan, B. (2016). Psychology and work: Perspectives on industrial and organizational psychology. New York: Psychology Press. ISBN 1134705697
  184. ^ a b c Laura L. Koppes, «Industrial-Organizational Psychology», in Weiner (ed.), Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 1: History of Psychology.
  185. ^ Yeh Hsueh, «The Hawthorne experiments and the introduction of Jean Piaget in American industrial psychology, 1929–1932»; History of Psychology 5.2, May 2002.
  186. ^ Parsons, H. M. (1974). «What happened at Hawthorne?: New evidence suggests the Hawthorne effect resulted from operant reinforcement contingencies». Science. 183 (4128): 922–932. doi:10.1126/science.183.4128.922. PMID 17756742. S2CID 38816592.
  187. ^ Levitt, Steven D.; List, John A. (2011). «Was There Really a Hawthorne Effect at the Hawthorne Plant? An Analysis of the Original Illumination Experiments» (PDF). American Economic Journal: Applied Economics. 3 (1): 224–238. doi:10.1257/app.3.1.224. S2CID 16678444. Archived (PDF) from the original on 2 June 2018. Retrieved 9 June 2021.
  188. ^ Myers (2004). Motivation and work. Psychology. New York, NY: Worth Publishers
  189. ^ Steven Williams, «Executive Management: Helping Executives Manage Their Organizations through Organizational and Market Research» in Morgan et al. (ed.), Life After Graduate School in Psychology (2005).
  190. ^ See also for example Baden Eunson: Behaving – Managing Yourself and Others. McGraw-Hill, Sidney/New York City 1987, ISBN 978-0-0745-2022-2.
  191. ^ Moorhead, G., & Griffin, R. W. (2017). Organizational behavior: Managing people and organizations, 12th ed.. Boston: Cengage. ISBN 978-1-305-50139-3
  192. ^ Robert M. Yerkes, «Measuring the Mental Strength of an Army Archived 20 January 2017 at the Wayback Machine»; Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 4.10, 15 October 1918.
  193. ^ Joshua N. Friedlander, «Military Psychology: An Army Clinical Psychologist» in Morgan et al. (ed.), Life After Graduate School in Psychology (2005).
  194. ^ Thomas, J.L. (2008). OHP Research and Practice in the US Army: Mental Health Advisory Teams. Newsletter of the Society for Occupational Health Psychology, 4, 4–5. [5] Archived 4 November 2021 at the Wayback Machine
  195. ^ Genderson, M.R., Schonfeld, I.S., Kaplan, M.S., & Lyons, M.J. (2009).Suicide associated with military service. Newsletter of the Society for Occupational Health Psychology, 6, 5–7. [6] Archived 22 September 2017 at the Wayback Machine
  196. ^ Paul M.A. Linebarger, Psychological Warfare; Washington: Combat Forces Press, 1954.
  197. ^ See «Project MKULTRA, the CIA’s Program of Research in Behavioral Modification Archived 29 April 2011 at the Wayback Machine»; Joint Hearing before the Senate Committee on Intelligence and the Subcommittee on Health and Scientific Research of the Committee on Human Resources, United States Senate, Ninety Fifth Congress, First Session, 3 August 1997; and John D. Marks, The Search for the Manchurian Candidate, New York: Times Books, 1979.
  198. ^ Alfred Paddock, Jr., «PSYOP: On a Complete Change in Organization, Practice, and Doctrine Archived 12 July 2015 at the Wayback Machine», Small Wars Journal 2010.
  199. ^ US torture report: psychologists should no longer aid military, group says Archived 14 December 2016 at the Wayback Machine The Guardian, 11 July 2015
  200. ^ Guthrie, Even the Rat was White (1998), Chapter 7: «Production of Black Psychologists in America» (pp. 155–213).
  201. ^ Chall, J. S. (1995). Learning to read: The great debate, 3rd ed. Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace.
  202. ^ Marilu Price Berry, «Interdisciplinary Medical Setting: The Multiple Roles of a Health Psychologist» in Morgan et al. (ed.), Life After Graduate School in Psychology (2005).
  203. ^ Robiner, W. N., & Seime, R. J. (2008). Psychologists in academic health centers: Traditions and innovations in education, science, and practice. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 15, 3–6. doi:10.1007/s10880-008-9091-1
  204. ^ Monica L. Baskin, «Public Health: Career Opportunities for Psychologists in Public Health», in Morgan et al. (ed.), Life After Graduate School in Psychology (2005). «Prevention strategies of late have largely concentrated on community-based interventions, which have been shown to be effective in changing the health of large populations. Behavioral and social scientists, such as psychologists, are helpful in this arena as we are trained to view individuals as belonging to complex and dynamic social systems, including immediate and extended family systems, acquaintance and friendship networks, neighborhood and community systems, and cultural groups (Schneiderman & Spee4, 2001).»
  205. ^ Zohar, D. (2010). Thirty years of safety climate research: Reflections and future directions. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 42, 1517-1522.
  206. ^ Beus, J. M., McCord, M. A., & Zohar, D. (2016). Workplace safety:A review and research synthesis. Organizational Psychology Review, 6, 352-381. doi:10.1177/2041386615626243
  207. ^ Clays, E., De Bacquer, D., Delanghe, J., Kittel, F., Van Renterghem, L., & De Backer, G. (2005). Associations Between Dimensions of Job Stress and Biomarkers of Inflammation and Infection. Journal of Occupational & Environmental Medicine, 47, 878-883
  208. ^ Brown, A. D., Schultebraucks, K., Qian, M., Li, M., Horesh, D., Siegel, C., Brody, Y., Amer, A. M., Lev-Ari, R. K., Mas, F., Marmar, C. R., & Farmer, J. (2020). Mental health disorders and utilization of mental healthcare services in United Nations personnel. Global Mental Health, 7. doi:10.1017/gmh.2019.29
  209. ^ Amiri, S., & Behnezhad, S. (2020). Is job strain a risk factor for musculoskeletal pain? A systematic review and meta-analysis of 21 longitudinal studies. Public Health, 181, 158-167. doi:10.1016/j.puhe.2019.11.023
  210. ^ Nixon, A. E., Mazzola, J. J., Bauer, J., Krueger, J. R., & Spector, P. E. (2011). Can work make you sick? A meta-analysis of the relationships between job stressors and physical symptoms. Work & Stress, 25, 1-22. doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02678373.2011.569175
  211. ^ Yang, L.-Q., Caughlin, D. E., Gazica, M. W., Truxillo, D. M., & Spector, P. E. (2014). Workplace mistreatment climate and potential employee and organizational outcomes: A meta-analytic review from the target’s perspective. Journal of Occupational Health Psychology, 19, 315-335. doi:10.1037/a0036905
  212. ^ Hochschild, A. (2011). To End All Wars: A Story of Loyalty and Rebellion, 1914–1918. ISBN 978-0-547-75031-6
  213. ^ Kreis, S. (1995). Early experiments in British scientific management: the Health of Munitions Workers’ Committee, 1915-1920. Journal of Management History (archive), 1, 65-78. doi.org/10.1108/13552529510088330
  214. ^ Kwiatkowski, R., Duncan, D. C., & Shimmin, S. (2006). What have we forgotten — and why? Journal of Occupational and Organizational Psychology, 79(2), 183-201. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1348/096317905X70832
  215. ^ a b Zickar, M. J. (2003). Remembering Arthur Kornhauser: Industrial psychology’s advocate for worker well-being. Journal of Applied Psychology, 88, 363–369. doi:10.1037/0021-9010.88.2.363
  216. ^ Kornhauser, A. (1965). Mental health of the industrial worker. New York: Wiley.
  217. ^ «Spector, P. (2019). What Is Occupational Health Psychology?». 30 December 2019. Archived from the original on 28 June 2021. Retrieved 14 July 2021.
  218. ^ Spector, P. E. (2021). From occupational fatigue to occupational health. In L. M. Lapierre & C. Cooper (Eds.). Cambridge companion to organizational stress and well-being. New York: Cambridge University Press.
  219. ^ a b Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Occupational Health Psychology (OHP). [7] Archived 10 September 2019 at the Wayback Machine
  220. ^ Houdmont, J., & Leka, S. (2010). An introduction to occupational health psychology. In S. Leka & J. Houdmont (Eds.). Occupational health psychology (pp. 1–30). John Wiley: Hoboken, NJ.
  221. ^ Everly, G.S., Jr. (1986). An introduction to occupational health psychology. In P.A. Keller & L.G. Ritt (Eds.), Innovations in clinical practice: A source book (Vol. 5, pp. 331–338). Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource Exchange.
  222. ^ Society for Occupational Health Psychology. Field of OHP. What is occupational health psychology [8] Archived 4 March 2016 at the Wayback Machine
  223. ^ Tetrick, L.E., & Quick, J.C. (2011). Overview of occupational health psychology: Public health in occupational settings. In J.C. Quick & L.E. Tetrick (Eds.), Handbook of occupational health psychology (2nd ed., pp. 3–20). Washington DC: American Psychological Association.
  224. ^ John A. Schinka & Wayne F. Velicer, «Volume Preface» in Weiner (ed.), Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 2: Research Methods in Psychology.
  225. ^ Alasuutari, Pertti (2010). «The rise and relevance of qualitative research». International Journal of Social Research Methodology. 13 (2): 139–55. doi:10.1080/13645570902966056. S2CID 143736805.
  226. ^ Schonfeld, I.S., & Mazzola, J.J. (2013). Strengths and limitations of qualitative approaches to research in occupational health psychology. In R. Sinclair, M. Wang, & L. Tetrick (Eds.), Research methods in occupational health psychology: State of the art in measurement, design, and data analysis (pp. 268-289). New York: Routledge.
  227. ^ Schulz, K.F.; Altman, D.G.; Moher, D.; for the CONSORT Group (2010). «CONSORT 2010 Statement: updated guidelines for reporting parallel group randomised trials». BMJ. 340: c332. doi:10.1136/bmj.c332. PMC 2844940. PMID 20332509.
  228. ^ Milgram, Stanley (1963). «Behavioral Study of Obedience». Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology. 67 (4): 371–378. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.599.92. doi:10.1037/h0040525. PMID 14049516. Archived from the original on 17 July 2012. Retrieved 24 May 2010. Full-text PDF. Archived 11 June 2011 at the Wayback Machine
  229. ^ Melvin M. Mark, «Program Evaluation» in Weiner (ed.), Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 2: Research Methods in Psychology.
  230. ^ Roger E. Kirk, «Experimental Design» in Weiner (ed.), Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 2: Research Methods in Psychology.
  231. ^ John T. Behrens and Chong-Ho Yu, «Exploratory Data Analysis» in Weiner (ed.), Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 2: Research Methods in Psychology.
  232. ^ Frank L. Schmidt and John E. Hunter, «Meta-Analysis», Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 2: Research Methods in Psychology.
  233. ^ Rösler, Frank (2005). «From Single-Channel Recordings to Brain-Mapping Devices: The Impact of Electroencephalography on Experimental Psychology» (PDF). History of Psychology. 8 (1): 95–117. doi:10.1037/1093-4510.8.1.95. PMID 16021767. Archived from the original (PDF) on 7 September 2015. Retrieved 24 April 2015.
  234. ^ Moran, Joseph M.; Zaki, Jamil (2013). «Functional Neuroimaging and Psychology: What Have You Done for Me Lately?». Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 25 (6): 834–842. doi:10.1162/jocn_a_00380. PMID 23469884. S2CID 12546790. Archived from the original on 2 February 2016. Retrieved 24 April 2015.
  235. ^ Cacioppo, John T.; Berntson, Gary G.; Nusbaum, Howard C. (2008). «Neuroimaging as a New Tool in the Toolbox of Psychological Science» (PDF). Current Directions in Psychological Science. 17 (2): 62–67. doi:10.1111/j.1467-8721.2008.00550.x. S2CID 14565940. Archived from the original (PDF) on 31 October 2014.
  236. ^ Aue, Tatjana; Lavelle, Leah A.; Cacioppo, John T. (2009). «Great expectations: What can fMRI research tell us about psychological phenomena?» (PDF). International Journal of Psychophysiology. 73 (1): 10–16. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2008.12.017. PMID 19232374. Archived from the original (PDF) on 9 January 2016.
  237. ^ Ron Sun, (2008). The Cambridge Handbook of Computational Psychology. Cambridge University Press, New York. 2008.
  238. ^ «Ncabr.Org: About Biomedical Research: Faq». Archived from the original on 8 July 2008. Retrieved 1 July 2008.
  239. ^ Shettleworth, S.J. (2010) Cognition, Evolution and Behavior (2nd Ed), New York: Oxford.
  240. ^ Wilson, E.O. (1978) On Human Nature p. x, Cambridge, Ma: Harvard
  241. ^ Glaser, B. & Strauss, A. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory: Strategies for qualitative research. Chicago: Aldine.
  242. ^ Fromm, E. (1941). Escape from freedom. New York: Farrar & Rinehart. ISBN 0-7448-0014-5
  243. ^ Harlow (1868), Fig. 2, p. 347 Harlow, John Martyn (1868). «Recovery from the Passage of an Iron Bar through the Head.» Publications of the Massachusetts Medical Society 2:327–347 (Republished in Macmillan 2000).
  244. ^ Fitzpatrick, J.L., Sanders, J.R., & Worthen, B.R. (2011). Program evaluation: Alternative approaches and practical guidelines. New York: Pearson Higher Education. ISBN 978-0-205-57935-8.
  245. ^ Administration for Children and Families (2010) The Program Manager’s Guide to Evaluation Archived 25 August 2012 at the Wayback Machine. Chapter 2: What is program evaluation?.
  246. ^
  247. ^ Franco, Annie; Malhotra, Neil; Simonovits, Gabor (1 January 2016). «Underreporting in Psychology Experiments: Evidence From a Study Registry». Social Psychological and Personality Science. 7 (1): 8–12. doi:10.1177/1948550615598377. ISSN 1948-5506. S2CID 143182733.
  248. ^ Munafò, Marcus (29 March 2017). «Metascience: Reproducibility blues». Nature. 543 (7647): 619–620. Bibcode:2017Natur.543..619M. doi:10.1038/543619a. ISSN 1476-4687.
  249. ^ StokstadSep. 20, Erik (19 September 2018). «This research group seeks to expose weaknesses in science—and they’ll step on some toes if they have to». Science | AAAS. Archived from the original on 18 March 2022. Retrieved 24 May 2019.
  250. ^ Stevens, Jeffrey R. (2017). «Replicability and Reproducibility in Comparative Psychology». Frontiers in Psychology. 8: 862. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00862. ISSN 1664-1078. PMC 5445189. PMID 28603511.
  251. ^ Arjo Klamer; Robert M. Solow; Donald N. McCloskey (1989). The Consequences of economic rhetoric. Cambridge University Press. pp. 173–174. ISBN 978-0-521-34286-5.
  252. ^ Lehrer, Jonah (13 December 2010). «The Truth Wears Off». The New Yorker. Archived from the original on 12 July 2014. Retrieved 10 April 2011.
  253. ^ Sterling, Theodore D. (March 1959). «Publication decisions and their possible effects on inferences drawn from tests of significance—or vice versa». Journal of the American Statistical Association. 54 (285): 30–34. doi:10.2307/2282137. JSTOR 2282137.
  254. ^ Fanelli, Daniele (2010). Enrico Scalas (ed.). «Positive’ Results Increase Down the Hierarchy of the Sciences». PLOS ONE. 5 (4): e10068. Bibcode:2010PLoSO…510068F. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0010068. PMC 2850928. PMID 20383332.
  255. ^ Marcus, Gary (1 May 2013). «The Crisis in Social Psychology That Isn’t». The New Yorker. Archived from the original on 7 July 2018. Retrieved 19 December 2018.
  256. ^ Meyer, Michelle N.; Chabris, Christopher (31 July 2014). «Why Psychologists’ Food Fight Matters». Slate. Archived from the original on 10 January 2019. Retrieved 19 December 2018.
  257. ^ Aschwanden, Christie (27 August 2015). «Psychology Is Starting To Deal With Its Replication Problem». FiveThirtyEight. Archived from the original on 19 August 2017. Retrieved 19 December 2018.
  258. ^ a b Open Science Collaboration (2015). «Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science» (PDF). Science. 349 (6251): aac4716. doi:10.1126/science.aac4716. hdl:10722/230596. PMID 26315443. S2CID 218065162. Archived (PDF) from the original on 29 October 2019. Retrieved 9 February 2019.
  259. ^ Hunt, Earl B. (2011). Human Intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press. p. 94. ISBN 978-0521707817.
  260. ^ Baumeister, Roy (September 2016), «Charting the future of social psychology on stormy seas: Winners, losers, and recommendations», Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 66: 153–158, doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2016.02.003, …shifting the dominant conceptual paradigm from Freudian psychoanalytic theory to Big Five research has reduced the chances of being wrong but palpably increased the fact of being boring. In making that transition, personality psychology became more accurate but less broadly interesting.
  261. ^ Duncan, Laramie E.; Keller, Matthew C. (October 2011). «A critical review of the first 10 years of candidate gene-by-environment interaction research in psychiatry». The American Journal of Psychiatry. 168 (10): 1041–1049. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020191. PMC 3222234. PMID 21890791.
  262. ^ Leichsenring, F.; Abbass, A.; Hilsenroth, M. J.; Leweke, F.; Luyten, P.; Keefe, J. R.; Midgley, N.; Rabung, S.; Salzer, S.; Steinert, C. (2017). «Biases in research: Risk factors for non-replicability in psychotherapy and pharmacotherapy research». Psychological Medicine. 47 (6): 1000–1011. doi:10.1017/S003329171600324X. PMID 27955715. S2CID 1872762. Archived from the original on 6 July 2019. Retrieved 28 December 2018.
  263. ^ Hengartner, Michael P. (2018). «Raising Awareness for the Replication Crisis in Clinical Psychology by Focusing on Inconsistencies in Psychotherapy Research: How Much Can We Rely on Published Findings from Efficacy Trials?». Frontiers in Psychology. 9: 256. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00256. PMC 5835722. PMID 29541051.
  264. ^ Frank, Michael C.; Bergelson, Elika; Bergmann, Christina; Cristia, Alejandrina; Floccia, Caroline; Gervain, Judit; Hamlin, J. Kiley; Hannon, Erin E.; Kline, Melissa; Levelt, Claartje; Lew-Williams, Casey; Nazzi, Thierry; Panneton, Robin; Rabagliati, Hugh; Soderstrom, Melanie; Sullivan, Jessica; Waxman, Sandra; Yurovsky, Daniel (2017). «A Collaborative Approach to Infant Research: Promoting Reproducibility, Best Practices, and Theory-Building». Infancy. 22 (4): 421–435. doi:10.1111/infa.12182. PMC 6879177. PMID 31772509. Archived from the original on 27 February 2020. Retrieved 21 July 2019.
  265. ^ Harris, Judith Rich (2009) [1998]. The Nurture Assumption: Why Children Turn Out the Way They Do (2nd ed.). New York: Free Press. ISBN 978-1439101650.
  266. ^ Harris, Judith Rich (2006). No Two Alike: Human Nature and Human Individuality. New York: W. W. Norton & Company. ISBN 978-0393329711.
  267. ^ Tyson, Charlie (14 August 2014). «Failure to Replicate». Inside Higher Ed. Archived from the original on 23 December 2019. Retrieved 19 December 2018.
  268. ^ Makel, Matthew C.; Plucker, Jonathan A. (2014). «Facts Are More Important Than Novelty». Educational Researcher. 43 (6): 304–316. doi:10.3102/0013189X14545513. S2CID 145571836.
  269. ^ Kirschner, Paul A.; Sweller, John; Clark, Richard E. (2006). «Why Minimal Guidance During Instruction Does Not Work: An Analysis of the Failure of Constructivist, Discovery, Problem-Based, Experiential, and Inquiry-Based Teaching». Educational Psychologist. Routledge. 41 (2): 75–86. doi:10.1207/s15326985ep4102_1. S2CID 17067829.
  270. ^ Foundations for Success: The Final Report of the National Mathematics Advisory Panel (PDF) (Report). United States Department of Education. 2008. pp. 45–46. Archived (PDF) from the original on 18 January 2018. Retrieved 3 November 2020.
  271. ^ Pashler, Harold; McDaniel, Mark; Rohrer, Doug; Bjork, Robert (2008). «Learning Styles: Concepts and Evidence». Psychological Science in the Public Interest. SAGE Publications. 9 (3): 105–119. doi:10.1111/j.1539-6053.2009.01038.x. PMID 26162104. S2CID 2112166.
  272. ^ Simmons, Joseph P.; Nelson, Leif D.; Simonsohn, Uri (2011). «False-Positive Psychology». Psychological Science. 22 (11): 1359–1366. doi:10.1177/0956797611417632. PMID 22006061.
  273. ^ Stroebe, Wolfgang; Strack, Fritz (2014). «The Alleged Crisis and the Illusion of Exact Replication» (PDF). Perspectives on Psychological Science. 9 (1): 59–71. doi:10.1177/1745691613514450. PMID 26173241. S2CID 31938129. Archived (PDF) from the original on 15 June 2020. Retrieved 21 July 2019.
  274. ^ Aschwanden, Christie (6 December 2018). «Psychology’s Replication Crisis Has Made The Field Better». FiveThirtyEight. Archived from the original on 20 December 2018. Retrieved 19 December 2018.
  275. ^ «Registered Reports». Center for Open Science. Archived from the original on 21 May 2021. Retrieved 20 May 2021.
  276. ^ Chartier, Chris; Kline, Melissa; McCarthy, Randy; Nuijten, Michele; Dunleavy, Daniel J.; Ledgerwood, Alison (December 2018), «The Cooperative Revolution Is Making Psychological Science Better», Observer, 31 (10), archived from the original on 20 December 2018, retrieved 19 December 2018
  277. ^ Allen, Christopher; Mehler, David M. A. (1 May 2019). «Open science challenges, benefits and tips in early career and beyond». PLOS Biology. Public Library of Science (PLoS). 17 (5): e3000246. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.3000246. ISSN 1545-7885. PMC 6513108. PMID 31042704. S2CID 240061030. (Erratum: doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.3000587,  [9])
  278. ^ Cohen, Jacob (1994). «The earth is round (p < .05)». American Psychologist. 49 (12): 997–1003. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.49.12.997. S2CID 380942. Archived from the original on 30 July 2022. Retrieved 7 January 2020.
  279. ^ McLeod, S. A. (2019). «McLeod, S. (2019). What does effect size tell you?». Archived from the original on 10 June 2021. Retrieved 10 June 2021.
  280. ^ a b Arnett, J. J. (2008). «The neglected 95%: Why American psychology needs to become less American». American Psychologist. 63 (7): 602–614. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.63.7.602. PMID 18855491.
  281. ^ Henrich, Joseph; Heine, Steven J.; Norenzayan, Ara (2010). «The weirdest people in the world?» (PDF). Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 33 (2–3): 61–83. doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999152X. hdl:11858/00-001M-0000-0013-26A1-6. PMID 20550733. S2CID 220918842. Archived (PDF) from the original on 4 April 2022. Retrieved 6 January 2022.
  282. ^ Collins, L. H.; Machizawa, S.; Rice, J. K. (2019). Transnational Psychology of Women: Expanding International and Intersectional Approaches. Washington, D. C.: American Psychological Association. ISBN 978-1-4338-3069-3.
  283. ^ Altmaier, E. M.; Hall, J. E. (2008). Global promise: Quality assurance and accountability in professional psychology. New York: Oxford University Press. ISBN 978-0-19-530608-8.
  284. ^ Morgan-Consoli, M. L.; Inman, A. G.; Bullock, M.; Nolan, S. A. (2018). «Framework for competencies for U.S. psychologists engaging internationally». International Perspectives in Psychology: Research, Practice, Consultation. 7 (3): 174–188. doi:10.1037/ipp0000090. S2CID 159028411.
  285. ^ Rad, Mostafa Salari; Martingano, Alison Jane; Ginges, Jeremy (2018). «Toward a psychology of Homo sapiens: Making psychological science more representative of the human population». Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 115 (45): 11401–11405. Bibcode:2018PNAS..11511401R. doi:10.1073/pnas.1721165115. ISSN 0027-8424. PMC 6233089. PMID 30397114.
  286. ^ Preston, Elizabeth (19 March 2023). «A Cognitive Revolution in Animal Research». The Atlantic. Retrieved 22 March 2023.
  287. ^ Dawes, Robyn (1994). House of Cards – Psychology and Psychotherapy Built on Myth. Free Press. ISBN 978-0-02-907205-9.
  288. ^ Beyerstein, Barry L. (Spring 2001). «Fringe Psychotherapies: The Public at Risk». The Scientific Review of Alternative Medicine. 5 (2): 70–9. CiteSeerX 10.1.1.462.3147.
  289. ^ «SRMHP: Our Raison d’Être». Archived from the original on 11 July 2007. Retrieved 1 July 2008.
  290. ^ American Psychological Association. (2016). Ethical principles of psychologists and code of conduct.[10] Archived 6 February 2018 at the Wayback Machine
  291. ^ The American Psychological Society: Responsible Conduct of Research Archived 24 November 2018 at the Wayback Machine
  292. ^ a b c Stanley E. Jones, «Ethical Issues in Clinical Psychology», in Weiner (ed.), Handbook of Psychology (2003), Volume 8: Clinical Psychology.
  293. ^ Sherwin, C.M.; Christionsen, S.B.; Duncan, I.J.; Erhard, H.W.; Lay Jr., D.C.; Mench, J.A.; O’Connor, C.E.; Petherick, J.C. (2003). «Guidelines for the Ethical use of animals in the applied ethology studies». Applied Animal Behaviour Science. 81 (3): 291–305. doi:10.1016/s0168-1591(02)00288-5.
  294. ^ Blum 1994, p. 95, Blum 2002, pp. 218–219. Blum 1994, p. 95: «… the most controversial experiment to come out of the Wisconsin laboratory, a device that Harlow insisted on calling the ‘pit of despair.«
  295. ^ Blum, Deborah (2002). Love at Goon Park: Harry Harlow and the science of affection. Cambridge, MA: Perseus Pub. ISBN 0-7382-0278-9. OCLC 50763066.
  296. ^ Booth, Wayne C. Modern Dogma and the Rhetoric of Assent, Volume 5, of University of Notre Dame, Ward-Phillips lectures in English language and literature, University of Chicago Press, 1974, p. 114. Booth is explicitly discussing this experiment. His next sentence is, «His most recent outrage consists of placing monkeys in ‘solitary’ for twenty days—what he calls a ‘vertical chamber apparatus …. designed on an intuitive basis’ to produce ‘a state of helplessness and hopelessness, sunken in a well of despair.«

Sources

  • Baker, David B. (ed.). The Oxford Handbook of the History of Psychology. Oxford University Press (Oxford Library of Psychology), 2012. ISBN 978-0-19-536655-6
  • Brock, Adrian C. (ed.). Internationalizing the History of Psychology. New York University Press, 2006. ISBN 978-0-8147-9944-4
  • Cina, Carol. «Social Science for Whom? A Structural History of Social Psychology.» Doctoral dissertation, accepted by the State University of New York at Stony Brook, 1981.
  • Cocks, Geoffrey. Psychotherapy in the Third Reich: The Göring Institute, second edition. New Brunswick, NJ: Transaction Publishers, 1997. ISBN 1-56000-904-7
  • Forgas, Joseph P., Kipling D. Williams, & Simon M. Laham. Social Motivation: Conscious and Unconscious Processes. Cambridge University Press, 2005. ISBN 0-521-83254-3
  • Guthrie, Robert. Even the Rat was White: A Historical View of Psychology. Second edition. Boston, Allyn and Bacon (Viacon), 1998. ISBN 0-205-14993-6
  • Herman, Ellen. «Psychology as Politics: How Psychological Experts Transformed Public Life in the United States 1940–1970.» Doctoral dissertation accepted by Brandeis University, 1993.
  • Hock, Roger R. Forty Studies That Changed Psychology: Explorations Into the History of Psychological Research. Fourth edition. Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall, 2002. ISBN 978-0-13-032263-0
  • Morgan, Robert D., Tara L. Kuther, & Corey J. Habben. Life After Graduate School in Psychology: Insider’s Advice from New Psychologists. New York: Psychology Press (Taylor & Francis Group), 2005. ISBN 1-84169-410-X
  • Severin, Frank T. (ed.). Humanistic Viewpoints in Psychology: A Book of Readings. New York: McGraw Hill, 1965. ISBN
  • Shah, James Y., and Wendi L. Gardner. Handbook of Motivation Science. New York: The Guilford Press, 2008. ISBN 978-1-59385-568-0
  • Wallace, Edwin R., IV, & John Gach (eds.), History of Psychiatry and Medical Psychology; New York: Springer, 2008; ISBN 978-0-387-34708-0
  • Weiner, Bernard. Human Motivation. Hoboken, NJ: Taylor and Francis, 2013. ISBN 978-0-8058-0711-0
  • Weiner, Irving B. Handbook of Psychology. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, 2003. ISBN 0-471-17669-9
    • Volume 1: History of Psychology. Donald K. Freedheim, ed. ISBN 0-471-38320-1
    • Volume 2: Research Methods in Psychology. John A. Schinka & Wayne F. Velicer, eds. ISBN 0-471-38513-1
    • Volume 3: Biological Psychology. Michela Gallagher & Randy J. Nelson, eds. ISBN 0-471-38403-8
    • Volume 4: Experimental Psychology. Alice F. Healy & Robert W. Proctor, eds. ISBN 0-471-39262-6
    • Volume 8: Clinical Psychology. George Stricker, Thomas A. Widiger, eds. ISBN 0-471-39263-4

Further reading

  • Badcock, Christopher R. (2015). «Nature-Nurture Controversy, History of». International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. pp. 340–344. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.03136-6. ISBN 978-0-08-097087-5.
  • Cascio, Wayne F. (2015). «Industrial–Organizational Psychology: Science and Practice». International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. pp. 879–884. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.22007-2. ISBN 978-0-08-097087-5.
  • Chryssochoou, Xenia (2015). «Social Psychology». International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. pp. 532–537. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.24095-6. ISBN 978-0-08-097087-5.
  • Deakin, Nicholas (2015). «Philosophy, Psychiatry, and Psychology». International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. pp. 31–36. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.27049-9. ISBN 978-0-08-097087-5.
  • Demetriou, Andreas (2015). «Intelligence in Cultural, Social and Educational Context». International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. pp. 313–322. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.92147-0. ISBN 978-0-08-097087-5.
  • Gelso, Charles J. (2015). «Counseling Psychology». International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. pp. 69–72. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.21073-8. ISBN 978-0-08-097087-5.
  • Henley, Tracy B. (2015). «Psychology, History of (Early Period)». International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. pp. 406–411. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.03235-9. ISBN 978-0-08-097087-5.
  • Knowland, Victoria C.P.; Purser, Harry; Thomas, Michael S.C. (2015). «Cross-Sectional Methodologies in Developmental Psychology». International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. pp. 354–360. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.23235-2. ISBN 978-0-08-097087-5.
  • Louw, Dap (2015). «Forensic Psychology». International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. pp. 351–356. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.21074-X. ISBN 978-0-08-097087-5.
  • McWilliams, Spencer A. (2015). «Psychology, History of (Twentieth Century)». International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. pp. 412–417. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.03046-4. ISBN 978-0-08-097087-5.
  • Pe-Pua, Rogelia (2015). «Indigenous Psychology». International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. pp. 788–794. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.24067-1. ISBN 978-0-08-097087-5.
  • Peterson, Roger L.; Peterson, Donald R.; Abrams, Jules C.; Stricker, George; Ducheny, Kelly (2015). «Training in Clinical Psychology in the United States: Practitioner Model». International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. pp. 517–523. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.21086-6. ISBN 978-0-08-097087-5.
  • Poortinga, Ype H. (2015). «Cross-Cultural Psychology». International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. pp. 311–317. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.24011-7. ISBN 978-0-08-097087-5.
  • Spinath, Frank M.; Spinath, Birgit; Borkenau, Peter (2015). «Developmental Behavioral Genetics and Education». International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. pp. 320–325. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.92009-9. ISBN 978-0-08-097087-5.
  • Smith, Edward E. (2015). «Cognitive Psychology: History». International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. pp. 103–109. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.03028-2. ISBN 978-0-08-097087-5.
  • Staerklé, Christian (2015). «Political Psychology». International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences. pp. 427–433. doi:10.1016/B978-0-08-097086-8.24079-8. ISBN 978-0-08-097087-5.

External links

  • Psychology at Curlie
  • American Psychological Association
  • Association for Psychological Science

While the psychology of today reflects the discipline’s rich and varied history, the origins of psychology differ significantly from contemporary conceptions of the field. In order to gain a full understanding of psychology, you need to spend some time exploring its history and origins.

How did psychology originate? When did it begin? Who were the people responsible for establishing psychology as a separate science?

Why Study Psychology History?

Contemporary psychology is interested in an enormous range of topics, looking at human behavior and mental process from the neural level to the cultural level. Psychologists study human issues that begin before birth and continue until death. By understanding the history of psychology, you can gain a better understanding of how these topics are studied and what we have learned thus far.

From its earliest beginnings, psychology has been faced with a number of questions. The initial question of how to define psychology helped establish it as a science separate from physiology and philosophy.

Additional questions that psychologists have faced throughout history include:

  • Is psychology really a science?
  • Should psychologists use research to influence public policy, education, and other aspects of human behavior?
  • Should psychology focus on observable behaviors, or on internal mental processes?
  • What research methods should be used to study psychology?
  • Which topics and issues should psychology be concerned with?

Background: Philosophy and Physiology

While psychology did not emerge as a separate discipline until the late 1800s, its earliest history can be traced back to the time of the early Greeks. During the 17th-century, the French philosopher Rene Descartes introduced the idea of dualism, which asserted that the mind and body were two entities that interact to form the human experience.

Many other issues still debated by psychologists today, such as the relative contributions of nature vs. nurture, are rooted in these early philosophical traditions.

So what makes psychology different from philosophy? While early philosophers relied on methods such as observation and logic, today’s psychologists utilize scientific methodologies to study and draw conclusions about human thought and behavior.

Physiology also contributed to psychology’s eventual emergence as a scientific discipline. Early physiological research on the brain and behavior had a dramatic impact on psychology, ultimately contributing to applying scientific methodologies to the study of human thought and behavior.

Psychology Emerges as a Separate Discipline

During the mid-1800s, a German physiologist named Wilhelm Wundt was using scientific research methods to investigate reaction times. His book published in 1873, «Principles of Physiological Psychology,» outlined many of the major connections between the science of physiology and the study of human thought and behavior.

He later opened the world’s first psychology lab in 1879 at the University of Leipzig. This event is generally considered the official start of psychology as a separate and distinct scientific discipline.

How did Wundt view psychology? He perceived the subject as the study of human consciousness and sought to apply experimental methods to studying internal mental processes. While his use of a process known as introspection is seen as unreliable and unscientific today, his early work in psychology helped set the stage for future experimental methods.

An estimated 17,000 students attended Wundt’s psychology lectures, and hundreds more pursued degrees in psychology and studied in his psychology lab. While his influence dwindled as the field matured, his impact on psychology is unquestionable.

Structuralism: Psychology’s First School of Thought

Edward B. Titchener, one of Wundt’s most famous students, would go on to found psychology’s first major school of thought. According to the structuralists, human consciousness could be broken down into smaller parts. Using a process known as introspection, trained subjects would attempt to break down their responses and reactions to the most basic sensation and perceptions.

While structuralism is notable for its emphasis on scientific research, its methods were unreliable, limiting, and subjective. When Titchener died in 1927, structuralism essentially died with him.

The Functionalism of William James

Psychology flourished in America during the mid- to late-1800s. William James emerged as one of the major American psychologists during this period and publishing his classic textbook, «The Principles of Psychology,» established him as the father of American psychology.

His book soon became the standard text in psychology and his ideas eventually served as the basis for a new school of thought known as functionalism.

Functionalism

The focus of functionalism was about how behavior actually works to help people live in their environment. Functionalists utilized methods such as direct observation to study the human mind and behavior.

Both of these early schools of thought emphasized human consciousness, but their conceptions of it were significantly different. While the structuralists sought to break down mental processes into their smallest parts, the functionalists believed that consciousness existed as a more continuous and changing process.

While functionalism quickly faded a separate school of thought, it would go on to influence later psychologists and theories of human thought and behavior.

The Emergence of Psychoanalysis

Up to this point, early psychology stressed conscious human experience. An Austrian physician named Sigmund Freud changed the face of psychology in a dramatic way, proposing a theory of personality that emphasized the importance of the unconscious mind.

Freud’s clinical work with patients suffering from hysteria and other ailments led him to believe that early childhood experiences and unconscious impulses contributed to the development of adult personality and behavior.

In his book «The Psychopathology of Everyday Life« Freud detailed how these unconscious thoughts and impulses are expressed, often through slips of the tongue (known as «Freudian slips») and dreams. According to Freud, psychological disorders are the result of these unconscious conflicts becoming extreme or unbalanced.

The psychoanalytic theory proposed by Sigmund Freud had a tremendous impact on 20th-century thought, influencing the mental health field as well as other areas including art, literature, and popular culture. While many of his ideas are viewed with skepticism today, his influence on psychology is undeniable.

The Rise of Behaviorism

Psychology changed dramatically during the early 20th-century as another school of thought known as behaviorism rose to dominance. Behaviorism was a major change from previous theoretical perspectives, rejecting the emphasis on both the conscious and unconscious mind. Instead, behaviorism strove to make psychology a more scientific discipline by focusing purely on observable behavior.

Behaviorism had its earliest start with the work of a Russian physiologist named Ivan Pavlov. Pavlov’s research on the digestive systems of dogs led to his discovery of the classical conditioning process, which proposed that behaviors could be learned via conditioned associations.

Pavlov demonstrated that this learning process could be used to make an association between an environmental stimulus and a naturally occurring stimulus.

An American psychologist named John B. Watson soon became one of the strongest advocates of behaviorism. Initially outlining the basic principles of this new school of thought in his 1913 paper Psychology as the Behaviorist Views It, Watson later went on to offer a definition in his classic book «Behaviorism« (1924), writing:

«Behaviorism…holds that the subject matter of human psychology is the behavior of the human being. Behaviorism claims that consciousness is neither a definite nor a usable concept. The behaviorist, who has been trained always as an experimentalist, holds, further, that belief in the existence of consciousness goes back to the ancient days of superstition and magic.»

The impact of behaviorism was enormous, and this school of thought continued to dominate for the next 50 years. Psychologist B.F. Skinner furthered the behaviorist perspective with his concept of operant conditioning, which demonstrated the effect of punishment and reinforcement on behavior.

While behaviorism eventually lost its dominant grip on psychology, the basic principles of behavioral psychology are still widely in use today.

Therapeutic techniques such as behavior analysis, behavioral modification, and token economies are often utilized to help children learn new skills and overcome maladaptive behaviors, while conditioning is used in many situations ranging from parenting to education.

The Third Force in Psychology

While the first half of the 20th century was dominated by psychoanalysis and behaviorism, a new school of thought known as humanistic psychology emerged during the second half of the century. Often referred to as the «third force» in psychology, this theoretical perspective emphasized conscious experiences.

American psychologist Carl Rogers is often considered to be one of the founders of this school of thought. While psychoanalysts looked at unconscious impulses and behaviorists focused on environmental causes, Rogers believed strongly in the power of free will and self-determination.

Psychologist Abraham Maslow also contributed to humanistic psychology with his famous hierarchy of needs theory of human motivation. This theory suggested that people were motivated by increasingly complex needs. Once the most basic needs are fulfilled, people then become motivated to pursue higher level needs.

Cognitive Psychology

During the 1950s and 1960s, a movement known as the cognitive revolution began to take hold in psychology. During this time, cognitive psychology began to replace psychoanalysis and behaviorism as the dominant approach to the study of psychology. Psychologists were still interested in looking at observable behaviors, but they were also concerned with what was going on inside the mind. 

Since that time, cognitive psychology has remained a dominant area of psychology as researchers continue to study things such as perception, memory, decision-making, problem-solving, intelligence, and language.

The introduction of brain imaging tools such as MRI and PET scans have helped improve the ability of researchers to more closely study the inner workings of the human brain.

Psychology Continues to Grow

As you have seen in this brief overview of psychology’s history, this discipline has seen dramatic growth and change since its official beginnings in Wundt’s lab. The story certainly does not end here.

Psychology has continued to evolve since 1960 and new ideas and perspectives have been introduced. Recent research in psychology looks at many aspects of the human experience, from the biological influences on behavior on the impact of social and cultural factors.

Today, the majority of psychologists do not identify themselves with a single school of thought. Instead, they often focus on a particular specialty area or perspective, often drawing on ideas from a range of theoretical backgrounds. This eclectic approach has contributed new ideas and theories that will continue to shape psychology for years to come.

Women in Psychology History

As you read through any history of psychology, you might be particularly struck by the fact that such texts seem to center almost entirely on the theories and contributions of men. This is not because women had no interest in the field of psychology, but is largely due to the fact that women were excluded from pursuing academic training and practice during the early years of the field.

There are a number of women who made important contributions to the early history of psychology, although their work is sometimes overlooked. 

A few pioneering women psychologists included:

  • Mary Whiton Calkins, who rightfully earned a doctorate from Harvard, although the school refused to grant her degree because she was a woman. She studied with major thinkers of the day like William James, Josiah Royce, and Hugo Munsterberg. Despite the obstacles she faced, she became the American Psychological Association’s first woman president.
  • Anna Freud, who made important contributions to the field of psychoanalysis. She described many of the defense mechanisms and is known as the founder of child psychoanalysis. She also had an influence on other psychologists including Erik Erikson.
  • Mary Ainsworth, who was a developmental psychologist, made important contributions to our understanding of attachment. She developed a technique for studying child and caregiver attachments known as the «Strange Situation» assessment.

A Word From Verywell

In order to understand how psychology became the science that it is today, it is important to learn more about some of the historical events that have influenced its development.

While some of the theories that emerged during the earliest years of psychology may now be viewed as simplistic, outdated, or incorrect, these influences shaped the direction of the field and helped us form a greater understanding of the human mind and behavior.

In earliest times, basic forms of psychology were practiced by priests, shamans, wizards, seers, medicine men, sorcerers, and enchanters, all of whom offered some blend of magic, religion, and herbal remedies to ease the physical and mental suffering of their patients. These early efforts evolved into the current fields of medicine, religion, and psychology, with a residual overlap of influence among the three.

History of Psychology in Classical Antiquity

The period we know as classical antiquity begins with the Homeric period and concludes with Hellenic cultural diffusion.

  • History of Psychology in Classical Antiquity
    • Homeric Foundations
    • Pre-Socratic Philosophy
    • Plato’s Psychology
    • Hippocrates’ Psychology
    • Aristotle’s Psychology
    • Stoic and Epicurean Psychology

History of Psychology in the Middle Ages

Medieval PsychologyThe Middle Ages has had a poor reputation among twentieth-century psychologists. Edwin Boring, in A History of Experimental Psychology (New York, 1929), held that late medieval thinking was based largely on theology and hence tended to be opposed to science. Gregory Zilboorg, in his History of Medical Psychology (New York, 1941), claimed that medieval medical practitioners were afraid to look into either normal or abnormal psychology and that the mentally ill were frequently regarded either as possessed by a devil or as witches. Introductory texts have occasionally taken this argument several stages further, claiming that the mentally ill in the Middle Ages were liable to be tortured or burnt at the stake as a consequence of the belief that they were possessed by a devil.

Historians of psychology in the later twentieth century did not substantiate claims that the mentally ill were routinely treated with cruelty in the Middle Ages. Moreover, they generally took a more sympathetic view of the period, finding that medieval philosophers, physicians, and even theologians produced and debated interesting theories of human behavior, although they seem to have done little to test them experimentally.

  • History of Psychology in the Middle Ages
    • The Middle Ages
    • Medieval Psychology
    • Medieval Cognitive Psychology
    • Mental Illness in the Middle Ages

History of Psychology during Renaissance through the Enlightenment

Renaissance PsychologyThe Renaissance revived interest in individual personality, setting the stage for the emergence of psychology in the seventeenth century. Subsequently, psychology became socially important in the eighteenth century, and became a science in the nineteenth century. The history of the word psychology reflected this development. Although its etymological roots are Greek, psuche (“soul”) + logos (“word”) = psychology, it appeared fitfully beginning in the 1600s, and gained general currency only in the nineteenth century. Instead, people wrote about a “science of human nature.” or “mental” or “moral” science. Moreover, psychology was one of several human sciences that appeared in the five centuries after 1400, but it took many years for them to assume their modern forms. For a long time, a student of human nature was equally psychologist, sociologist, anthropologist, economist, and political scientist.

  • History of Psychology during Renaissance through the Enlightenment
    • The Renaissance
    • The Scientific Revolution
    • The Rise of Psychology
    • Human Nature, Morality, and Society

History of Psychology in the Nineteenth Century

Nineteenth Century PsychologyPsychology became a science in the nineteenth century. The roots of this new science were many. Philosophers provided psychology’s conceptual framework: physiologists provided knowledge of the nervous system and experimental methods; and social reformers and psychiatrists provided motives for using science to improve the human condition. This article will describe the movements, ideas, and discoveries leading to and shaping scientific psychology, concentrating on developments in the United States, the home of twentieth century psychology.

  • History of Psychology in the Nineteenth Century
    • Central Controversies
    • Mind or Matter?
    • Innovations
    • Methods
    • Institutions
    • Founding Psychologies

World War I brought the nineteenth century, and nineteenth-century psychology, to an end. The unspeakable horrors of the war shattered nineteenth-century faith in the inevitability of progress. To many, the war was the Freudian unconscious made manifest. Germany especially experienced a deep crisis in politics, culture, and philosophy, and psychologists offered worldviews to replace those that had failed. For example, Gestalt psychology and its philosophical ally, phenomenology, proposed a new holistic analysis of consciousness meant to support old humanistic values. Everywhere during the war, psychology turned in applied directions, as psychologists worked on varied military projects. This trend was most powerful in the United States, where applied psychology was already valued. After the war, the numbers of American applied psychologists grew exponentially, creating powerful tensions between academic scientists and working professionals. In 1913, just before the war broke out, John B. Watson launched the movement of behaviorism, which would dominate twentieth-century American psychology. Increasingly, psychological ideas affected how people understood themselves, others, and their societies. By 1918, what had begun as an obscure science pursued by small bands of men and women was emerging as the foundation of a new, psychological, society.

History of Psychology in the Early Twentieth Century

20th Century PsychologyIn the early 1900s American psychology was still a modestly sized enterprise, but it was in a propitious position to advance, for it had a rich and diverse cache of intellectual ideas about mental life from which to draw that had been developed in the previous century. In addition, the young profession had ample room to locate in the ongoing expansion of colleges and universities, and could make contact with an engaging public interested in things “psychological.” Psychology did prosper within this climate. By the end of World War II the science had secured an influential place in American society and had created a host of subspecialties applicable to a myriad of intellectual and social problems. Thus, in less than half a century psychology had expanded rapidly in terms of scientific work, scope of subject matter, and practical utility. However, while increasing in number of practitioners and domains, the science drastically reduced the eclectic intellectual perspectives and methods that had been present at the century’s beginning. The intervening years, then, comprised first a flourishing followed by a diminuation of the scientific possibilities for explaining and examining the human psyche and actions.

  • History of Psychology in the Early Twentieth Century
    • Ideas Galore
    • Psychology in Society
    • Methods of Inquiry
    • Schools and Systems
    • Behaviorism
    • Testing and Classifying

History of Psychology after World War II

Cognitive PsychologyPsychology in the pre-World War II era was on the threshold of becoming both a major academic discipline and an applied profession. It had some unresolved problems, particularly involving mind-body issues, but in the main the academic wing of psychology had adopted British empiricism as its philosophy and was therefore behavioristic in orientation. The professional wing had been struggling to achieve its unique identity. We will survey the post-World War II era in terms of these academic and professional developments. Our focus is on the United States, which was actually the leader in psychological studies before the war.

  • History of Psychology after World War II
    • Behaviorism vs. Gestalt Psychology
    • Cognitive Revelation
    • Humanistic Psychology
    • Professional Developments

It may appear, given our overview glance at psychology since World War II, that we have here a discipline in serious difficulty. It must confront other specialties like biology and computer science which threaten to explain it away. There is a seemingly unbridgeable split between its basic science and applied wings. Critics dismiss it as not being a legitimate science to begin with, and at the very least find that it has no business seeking to apply its “unsatisfactorily” established principles. Even some of psychology’s advocates want its academic home shifted over from the sciences to the humanities. There are unsettled internal questions concerning its concepts, especially regarding whether human beings are purposive organisms or robots. Its therapies are under fire as supposedly ineffective. And its gender recruiting is increasingly one-sided, which can bias its developmental direction. Indeed, psychology’s very popularity in the broader culture may dilute its potential to be a focused discipline.

How one interprets these issues will obviously influence any conclusion regarding the future of psychology. We can frame them with hopeful optimism or dour pessimism. There are surely challenges ahead for this discipline, but pessimism hardly seems warranted. It is probably more reasonable to view these challenges as reflecting the inevitable growing pains of a vibrant and much needed realm of knowledge in human affairs. Growing pains are rarely fatal and usually overcome. There is every indication that psychology will be successful in this regard and thereby continue to make a unique and important contribution to knowledge in the twenty-first century.

Psychology in The 21st Century

Humanistic PsychologyAs the old millennium closed, there was a shift from behavioral perspectives in psychology to cognitive perspectives—that is, a shift from a strict reliance on empirical sources for knowledge to an acceptance of rationality as a source for knowledge. In the first decade of the new millennium, psychology is taking on another new look, or perhaps it is simply returning to whence it came. Psychology, and in fact all of society, is embracing spirituality, accepting faith as a once-again legitimate source of knowledge. The media is awash with words such as intuition and faith (Malcolm Gladwell’s book Blink, a best seller, attests to the popularity of intuitive thinking), thus reminding us of this acceptance of non-empirical sources of knowledge. Advances in information technology, such as the Internet and the 24-hour news networks, have made psychology in all of its forms accessible to everyone. This increased globalization of psychology is mirrored in the most recent edition of the DSM, which now acknowledges that mental health practitioners should be aware of cultural issues when diagnosing and treating mental illness.

Read more about Psychology in The 21st Century.

In the fast-paced world of the late 20th and early 21st centuries, in which economic decisions are made based on the “bottom line,” it seems inevitable (i.e., the zeitgeist is right) that Francis Bacon’s (1620) notions at the beginning of the scientific revolution should come full circle. “Human knowledge and human power meet in one; for where the cause is not known the effect cannot be produced.” All fields of science, in order to continue to exist, must provide useful information. As seen in the mission statement of the APA, “The objects of the American Psychological Association shall be to advance psychology as a science and profession and as a means of promoting health, education, and human welfare.” Perhaps psychology is finally starting to grow up enough to prove its usefulness.

Women and Minorities in the History of Psychology

History of Psychology

In July 1892 well-known Clark University psychologist G. Stanley Hall met with a small group of his peers and founded the American Psychological Association (APA). At their first official meeting the following December, 31 additional members were voted in; all were white, and all were male (see Fernberger, 1932). However, as psychology grew throughout the first half of the 20th century, the proportion of women in the field increased. In 1946, psychologist Alice Bryan and her colleague Edwin Boring conducted a survey of American psychology and found that of the 2,672 doctoral-level psychologists who responded to their survey, 24 percent were women (Bryan & Boring, 1946). At this point in history, very little attention was paid to the representation of non-white psychologists in the field.

The proportion of women remained relatively stable until the late 1960s. The greatest growth in the numbers of women in psychology began in the early 1970s, largely due to the impact of the second wave of the women’s movement, and has continued steadily since that time. Whereas in 1960, 17.5 percent of all doctoral degrees in psychology in the United States were awarded to women, by the year 2000 the proportion of women receiving doctorates in the field had risen to 66.6 percent (Women’s Programs Office, 2006). Psychology, especially its applied branches, is quickly becoming a female-dominated profession.

The 1960s also saw important cultural and political shifts that affected the number and representation of minority psychologists. The civil rights movement and the development of black nationalism provided the cultural and political foundations for the institutional and theoretical challenges of black psychologists. Their activism paved the way for other minority groups such as Latino/Latina psychologists and Asian American psychologists to demand greater receptivity to and support for their concerns and agendas within a largely white, Eurocentric psychological establishment. Although growth in numbers has been slow relative to the influx of women psychologists, ethnic minority men and women psychologists steadily continue to challenge and change the institutional, theoretical, and practical bases of the field.

  • Women and Minorities in the History of Psychology
    • First-Wave “Feminist” Psychologists
    • Women in Psychology at Mid-Century
    • Second-Wave Feminism and Psychology
    • Early African American Psychologists
    • The Clarks’ Contributions
    • The Association of Black Psychologists

Conducting Research on the History of Psychology

History of PsychologyEvery day, psychologists make history. It can be in an act as small as sending an e-mail or as large as winning a Nobel Prize. What remains of these acts and the contexts in which they occur are the data of history. When transformed by historians of psychology to produce narrative, these data represent our best attempts to make meaning of our science and profession.

The meaning that is derived from the data of history is most often made available to students of psychology through a course in the history of psychology. For a variety of reasons, the history of psychology has maintained a strong presence in the psychology curriculum at both the undergraduate and graduate levels for as long as there has been a psychology curriculum in America (Fuchs & Viney, 2002; Hilgard, Leary, & McGuire, 1991). As a result, most students will have some exposure to the subject matter and some sense of its importance.

Why are psychologists so interested in their own history? In trying to answer this question, consider the following quotations from two eminent British historians. One, Robin Collingwood (1946), wrote that the “proper object of historical study… is the human mind, or more properly the activities of the human mind” (p. 215). And the other, Edward H. Carr (1961), proposed that “the historian is not really interested in the unique, but what is general in the unique” and that “the study of history is a study of causes… the historian… continuously asks the question: Why?” (pp. 80, 113). Thus, according to these historians, to study history is to study the human mind, to be able to generalize beyond the characteristics of a single individual or single event to other individuals and other events, and to be able to answer the “why” of human behavior in terms of motivation, personality, past experience, expectations, and so forth. Historians are not satisfied, for example, with a mere description of the events of May 4, 1970, in which National Guard troops killed four unarmed students on a college campus in Ohio. Description is useful, but it is not the scholarly end product that is sought. By itself, description is unlikely to answer the questions that historians want to answer. They want to understand an event, like the shootings at Kent State University, so completely that they can explain why it happened.

Collingwood (1946) has described history as “the science of human nature” (p. 206). In defining history in that way, Collingwood has usurped psychology’s definition for itself. One can certainly argue about the scientific nature of history and thus his use of the term science in his definition. Whereas historians do not do experimental work, they are engaged in empirical work, and they approach their questions in much the same way that psychologists do, by generating hypotheses and then seeking evidence that will confirm or disconfirm those hypotheses. Thus the intellectual pursuits of the historian and the psychologist are not really very different. And so as psychologists or students of psychology, we are not moving very far from our own field of interest when we study the history of psychology.

Historians of psychology seek to understand the development of the discipline by examining the confluence of people, places, and events within larger social, economic, and political contexts. Over the last forty years the history of psychology has become a recognized area of research and scholarship in psychology. Improvements in the tools, methods, and training of historians of psychology have created a substantial body of research that contributes to conversations about our shared past, the meaning of our present divergence, and the promise of our future. In this research paper you will learn about the theory and practice of research on the history of psychology.

  • Conducting Research on the History of Psychology
    • Theory Research
    • Methods Research
    • Applications Research
Psychology

Senses brain.png

History
Psychologists
Divisions
Abnormal
Applied
Biological
Clinical
Cognitive
Comparative
Developmental
Differential
Industrial
Parapsychology
Personality
Positive
Religion
Social
Approaches
Behaviorism
Depth
Experimental
Gestalt
Humanistic
Information processing

Many ancient cultures speculated on the nature of the human mind, soul, and spirit. Psychology as a scholarly study of the mind and behavior in Europe dates back to the Late Middle Ages. It was widely regarded to a branch of philosophy until the middle of the nineteenth century. During the last quarter of the nineteenth century, however, psychology in the West began to be seriously pursued as a scientific enterprise.

In 1879, Wilhelm Wundt founded the first laboratory dedicated exclusively to psychological research in Leipzig, Germany. Other important early contributors to the field include Hermann Ebbinghaus (a pioneer in studies on memory), William James, and Ivan Pavlov (who developed the procedures associated with classical conditioning). Sigmund Freud developed his psychoanalytic method, which revolutionized the treatment of psychological disorders.

Soon after the development of experimental psychology, various kinds of applied psychology began to appear. G. Stanley Hall brought scientific pedagogy to the United States from Germany in the early 1880s. John Dewey’s educational theory of the 1890s was another early example. In the 1890s, James McKeen Cattell adapted Francis Galton’s anthropometric methods to generate the first program of mental testing.

The twentieth century saw a rejection of Freud’s theories of mind as being too unscientific. This led to the formulation of behaviorism by John B. Watson, which was popularized by B. F. Skinner. Behaviorism proposed epistemologically limiting psychological study to overt behavior, since that could be quantified and easily measured. Scientific knowledge of the «mind» was considered too metaphysical, hence impossible to achieve.

The final decades of the twentieth century saw the rise of a new interdisciplinary approach to studying human psychology, known collectively as cognitive science. Cognitive science again considered the «mind» as a subject for investigation, using the tools of evolutionary psychology, linguistics, computer science, philosophy, and neurobiology. This approach proposed that a wide understanding of the human mind is possible, and that such an understanding may be applied to other research domains, such as artificial intelligence.

With the dawning of the twenty-first century there arose another new approach to psychology, known as Positive psychology. Originally a development of humanistic psychologists’ research on happiness and their focus on treating mental health rather than mental illness it is intended to complement, not to replace, traditional psychology. Positive psychology brought an emphasis on the importance of using the scientific method to understand normal development, including nurturing talent and genius and studying how each individual can fulfill their potential as a human being.

Etymology

The first use of the term «Psychology» is often attributed to the Yucologia hoc est de hominis perfectione, anima, ortu, written by the German scholastic philosopher Rudolf Göckel (1547-1628, often known under the Latin form Rudolph Goclenius), and published in Marburg in 1590. Another early use of the term was by Otto Casmann (1562-1607). Among his numerous works from the field of philosophy, theology, and natural sciences is one that includes the word «psychology» in its title: Psychologia anthropologica printed in Hanau in 1594. Filip Melanchton is often cited as having used the term in his lectures about forty years earlier (Krstic, 1964).

However, the term was used more than six decades earlier by the Croatian humanist Marko Marulić (1450-1524) in the title of his Latin treatise Psichiologia de ratione animae humanae. Although the treatise itself has not been preserved, its title appears in a list of Marulic’s works compiled by his younger contemporary, Franjo Bozicevic-Natalis in his Vita Marci Maruli Spalatensis. This, of course, may well not have been the very first usage, but it is the earliest documented use at present (Krstic, 1964).

The term did not come into popular usage until the German idealist philosopher, Christian Wolff used it in his Psychologia empirica and Psychologia rationalis (1732-1734). This distinction between empirical and rational psychology was picked up in Denis Diderot’s Encyclopédie (1751-1784) and was popularized in France by Maine de Biran (1766-1824). In England, the term «psychology» did not overtake «mental philosophy» until the middle of the nineteenth century, in the work of William Hamilton (1788-1856) (Danziger 1997).

Early Psychological Thought

Many cultures throughout history have speculated on the nature of the mind, soul, and spirit. In Ancient Egypt, the Edwin Smith Papyrus (1550 B.C.E.) contains an early description of the brain, and some speculations on its functions (though in a medical/surgical context). Though other medical documents of ancient times were full of incantations and applications meant to turn away disease-causing demons and other superstition, the Edwin Smith Papyrus gives remedies to almost 50 conditions and only one contains incantations to ward off evil.

Ancient Greek philosophers from Thales (550 B.C.E.) through to the Roman period developed an elaborate theory of what they termed the psuchẽ (from which the first half of «psychology» is derived), as well as other (loosely speaking) «psychological» terms—nous, thumos, logistikon, and so forth (Everson 1991; Green and Groff 2003). The most influential of these are the accounts of Plato, especially in the Republic, and of Aristotle, especially in De Anima.

The Dead Sea Scrolls Manual of Discipline (ca. 21 B.C.E.–61 C.E.) notes the division of human nature into two temperaments.

In Asia, China had a long history of administering psychological tests as part of its education system. In the sixth century C.E., Lin Xie carried out an early psychological experiment, in which he asked people to draw a square with one hand and at the same time draw a circle with the other in order to test people’s vulnerability to distraction.

India, too, had an elaborate theory of the self in its Vedanta philosophical writings (Paranjpe 2010).

During the Islamic Golden Age (ninth–thirteenth centuries). Islamic scholars developed the science of the Nafs (Haque 2004). Muslim scholarship was strongly influenced by Greek and Indian philosophy as well as by the study of scripture. In the writings of Muslim scholars, the term Nafs (self or soul) was used to denote individual personality, and encompassed a broad range of faculties including the qalb (heart), the ruh (spirit), the aql (intellect) and irada (will). The study of mental illness was a specialty of its own, known as al-‘ilaj al-nafs, approximately translated as «curing/treatment of the ideas/soul (Haque 2004). The Iraqi Arab scientist, Ibn al-Haytham (Alhacen), carried out a number of investigations on visual perception, including sensation, variations in sensitivity, sensation of touch, perception of colors, perception of darkness, the psychological explanation of the moon illusion, and binocular vision. In his Book of Optics Alhacen (1011 to 1021) argued that vision occurs in the brain, rather than the eyes. Alhacen’s pioneering work on the psychology of visual perception and optical illusions led some to suggest that he could be considered be the «founder of experimental psychology» (Khaleefa 1999).

Beginnings of Western psychology

Early Western psychology was regarded as the study of the soul (in the Christian sense of the term). Until the middle of the nineteenth century, psychology was widely regarded as a branch of philosophy, and was heavily influenced by the works of René Descartes (1596-1650).

The philosophers of the British Empiricist and Associationist schools had a profound impact on the later course of experimental psychology. John Locke’s An Essay Concerning Human Understanding (1689), George Berkeley’s Treatise Concerning the Principles of Human Knowledge (1710), and David Hume’s A Treatise of Human Nature (1739-1740) were particularly influential, as were David Hartley’s Observations on Man (1749) and John Stuart Mill’s A System of Logic (1843). Also notable was the work of some Continental Rationalist philosophers, especially Baruch Spinoza’s On the Improvement of the Understanding (1662) and Gottfried Wilhelm Leibniz’s New Essays on Human Understanding (completed 1705, published 1765).

Also influential on the emerging discipline of psychology were debates surrounding the efficacy of Mesmerism (hypnosis) and the value of phrenology. The former was developed in the 1770s by Austrian physician Franz Mesmer who claimed to use the power of gravity, and later of «animal magnetism,» to cure various physical and mental ills. As Mesmer and his treatment became increasingly fashionable in both Vienna and Paris, it also began to come under the scrutiny of suspicious officials. Although discredited by an investigation commissioned by King Louis which included American ambassador Benjamin Franklin, chemist Antoine Lavoisier, and physician Joseph-Ignace Guillotin (later the popularizer of the guillotine), the «magnetic» tradition continued among Mesmer’s students and others, resurfacing in England in the nineteenth century in the work of physicians John Elliotson (1791-1868), James Esdaile (1808-1859), and James Braid (1795-1860), who renamed it «hypnotism.» In France the practice regained a strong following after it was investigated by the Nancy physician Hippolyte Bernheim (1840-1919) and adopted for the treatment of hysteria by the director of Paris’s Salpêtrière Hospital, Jean-Martin Charcot (1825-1893).

Phrenology began as «organology,» a theory of brain structure developed by the German physician, Franz Joseph Gall (1758-1828). Gall argued that the brain is divided into a large number of functional «organs,» each responsible for particular human mental abilities and dispositions—hope, love, spirituality, greed, language, the abilities to detect the size, form, and color of objects, and so forth. He argued that the larger each of these organs are, the greater the power of the corresponding mental trait. Further, he argued that one could detect the sizes of the organs in a given individual by feeling the surface of that person’s skull. Although Gall had been a serious (albeit misguided) researcher, his theory was taken by his assistant, Johann Gaspar Spurzheim (1776-1832), and developed into the profitable, popular enterprise of phrenology, which soon spawned, especially in Britain, a thriving industry of independent practitioners. In the hands of Scottish religious leader George Combe (1788-1858) phrenology became strongly associated with political reform movements and egalitarian principles. Phrenology soon spread to America as well, where itinerant practical phrenologists assessed the mental well-being of willing customers.

Emergence of German experimental psychology

In its beginnings psychology was long regarded as a branch of philosophy. Immanuel Kant declared in his Metaphysical Foundations of Natural Science (1786) that a scientific psychology «properly speaking» is impossible. Johann Friedrich Herbart (1776-1841) took issue with Kant’s conclusion and attempted to develop a mathematical basis for a scientific psychology. Although he was unable to render his theory empirically testable, his efforts did lead scientists such as Ernst Heinrich Weber (1795-1878) and Gustav Theodor Fechner (1801-1887) to try to measure the mathematical relationships between the physical magnitudes of external stimuli and the psychological intensities of the resulting sensations. Fechner is the originator of the term psychophysics.

Meanwhile, individual differences in reaction time had become a critical issue in the field of astronomy, under the name of the «personal equation.» The nineteenth century was also the period in which physiology, including neurophysiology, professionalized and saw some of its most significant discoveries. Charles Bell (1774-1843) and François Magendie (1783-1855) independently discovered the distinction between sensory and motor nerves in the spinal column, Johannes Müller (1801-1855) proposed the doctrine of specific nerve energies, Emil du Bois-Reymond (1818-1896) studied the electrical basis of muscle contraction, Pierre Paul Broca (1824-1880) and Carl Wernicke (1848-1905) identified areas of the brain responsible for different aspects of language, and Gustav Fritsch (1837-1927), Eduard Hitzig (1839-1907), and David Ferrier (1843-1924) localized sensory and motor areas of the brain.

One of the principal founders of experimental physiology, Hermann von Helmholtz (1821-1894), conducted studies of a wide range of topics that would later be of interest to psychologists—the speed of neural transmission, the natures of sound and color, and of our perception of them, and so on. In the 1860s, while he held a position in Heidelberg, Helmholtz engaged as an assistant a young doctor named Wilhelm Wundt. Wundt employed the equipment of the physiology laboratory to address more complicated psychological questions than had until then been considered experimentally. In particular he was interested in the nature of apperception—the point at which a perception comes into the central focus of conscious awareness.

In 1874 Wundt took up a professorship in Zurich, where he published his landmark textbook, Grundzüge der physiologischen Psychologie (Principles of Physiological Psychology, 1874). Moving to a more prestigious professorship in Leipzig in 1875, Wundt founded a laboratory specifically dedicated to original research in experimental psychology in 1879, the first laboratory of its kind in the world. In 1883, he launched a journal in which to publish the results of his, and his students’, research, Philosophische Studien (Philosophical Studies). Wundt attracted a large number of students not only from Germany but also from abroad. Among his most influential American students were Granville Stanley Hall (who had already obtained a Ph.D. from Harvard under the supervision of William James), James McKeen Cattell (who was Wundt’s first assistant), and Frank Angell. The most influential British student was Edward Bradford Titchener (who later became professor at Cornell).

Experimental psychology laboratories were soon also established at Berlin by Carl Stumpf (1848-1936) and at Göttingen by Georg Elias Müller (1850-1934). Another major German experimental psychologist of the era, though he did not direct his own research institute, was Hermann Ebbinghaus (1850-1909).

Experimentation was not the only approach to psychology in the German-speaking world at this time. Starting in the 1890s, employing the case study (traditional in medicine at the time), the Viennese physician Sigmund Freud developed and applied the methods of hypnosis, free association, and dream interpretation to reveal putatively unconscious beliefs and desires that he argued were the underlying causes of his patients’ «hysteria.» He dubbed this approach psychoanalysis. Psychoanalysis is particularly notable for the emphasis it places on the course of an individual’s sexual development in pathogenesis. Freud based his model of child development on his own and his patients’ recollections of their childhood. He developed a stage model of development in which the libido, or sexual energy, of the child focuses on different «zones» or areas of the body as the child grows to adulthood. Although the details of Freud’s developmental theory have been widely criticized, his emphasis on the importance of early childhood experiences, prior to five years of age, has had a lasting impact. His psychoanalytic concepts have also had a strong and lasting influence on Western culture, particularly on the arts.

Early American Psychology

Around 1875, the Harvard physiology instructor, William James, opened a small experimental psychology demonstration laboratory for use with his courses. In 1878, James gave a series of lectures at Johns Hopkins University entitled “The Senses and the Brain and their Relation to Thought” in which he argued, contra Thomas Henry Huxley, that consciousness is not epiphenomenal, but must have an evolutionary function, or it would not have been naturally selected in humans. The same year James was contracted by Henry Holt to write a textbook on the «new» experimental psychology. If he had written it quickly, it would have been the first English-language textbook on the topic. It was twelve years, however, before his two-volume Principles of Psychology would be published. In the meantime textbooks were published by George Trumbull Ladd of Yale (1887) and James Mark Baldwin then of Lake Forest College (1889).

In 1879 Charles Sanders Peirce was hired as a philosophy instructor at Johns Hopkins University. Although better known for his astronomical and philosophical work, Peirce also conducted what are perhaps the first American psychology experiments, on the subject of color vision, published in 1877 in the American Journal of Science. Peirce and his student Joseph Jastrow also published «On Small Differences in Sensation» in the Memoirs of the National Academy of Sciences, in 1884. In 1882, Peirce was joined at Johns Hopkins by Granville Stanley Hall, who opened the first American research laboratory devoted to experimental psychology in 1883. Peirce was forced out of his position by scandal and Hall was awarded the only professorship in philosophy at Johns Hopkins. In 1887 Hall founded the American Journal of Psychology, which published work primarily emanating from his own laboratory. In 1888 Hall left his Johns Hopkins professorship for the presidency of the newly-founded Clark University, where he remained for the rest of his career.

Soon, experimental psychology laboratories were opened at the University of Pennsylvania (in 1887, by James McKeen Cattell), Indiana University (1888 by William Lowe Bryan), the University of Wisconsin-Madison (1888 by Joseph Jastrow), Clark University (1889 by Edmund Clark Sanford), the McLean Asylum (1889 by William Noyes), and the University of Nebraska (1889 by Harry Kirke Wolfe).

In 1890, William James’ Principles of Psychology finally appeared, and rapidly became the most influential textbook in the history of American psychology. It laid many of the foundations for the questions that American psychologists would focus on for years to come. The book’s chapters on consciousness, emotion, and habit were particularly agenda-setting.

One of those who felt the impact of James’ Principles was John Dewey, then professor of philosophy at the University of Michigan. With his junior colleagues, James Hayden Tufts (who founded the psychology laboratory at Michigan) and George Herbert Mead, and his student James Rowland Angell, this group began to reformulate psychology, focusing more strongly on the social environment and on the activity of mind and behavior than the psychophysics-inspired physiological psychology of Wundt and his followers had heretofore. Tufts left Michigan for another junior position at the newly-founded University of Chicago in 1892. A year later, the senior philosopher at Chicago resigned, and Tufts recommended to Chicago president William Rainey Harper that Dewey be offered the position. After initial reluctance, Dewey was hired in 1894. Dewey soon filled out the department with his Michigan companions Mead and Angell. These four formed the core of the Chicago School of psychology.

In 1892, G. Stanley Hall invited 30-some psychologists and philosophers to a meeting at Clark with the purpose of founding a new American Psychological Association (APA). The first annual meeting of the APA was held later that year, hosted by George S. Fullerton at the University of Pennsylvania. Almost immediately tension arose between the experimentally- and philosophically-inclined members of the APA. Edward Bradford Titchener and Lightner Witmer launched an attempt to either establish a separate «Section» for philosophical presentations, or to eject the philosophers altogether.

In 1894, a number of psychologists, unhappy with the parochial editorial policies of the American Journal of Psychology approached Hall about appointing an editorial board and opening the journal out to more psychologists not within Hall’s immediate circle. Hall refused, so James McKeen Cattell (then of Columbia) and James Mark Baldwin (then of Princeton) co-founded a new journal, Psychological Review, which rapidly grew to become a major outlet for American psychological researchers.

Beginning in 1895, James Mark Baldwin (Princeton) and Edward Bradford Titchener (Cornell) entered into an increasingly acrimonious dispute over the correct interpretation of some anomalous reaction time findings that had come from the Wundt laboratory (originally reported by Ludwig Lange and James McKeen Cattell). In 1896, James Rowland Angell and Addison W. Moore (Chicago) published a series of experiments in Psychological Review appearing to show that Baldwin was the more correct of the two. However, they interpreted their findings in light of John Dewey’s new approach to psychology, which rejected the traditional stimulus-response understanding of the reflex arc in favor of a «circular» account in which what serves as «stimulus» and what as «response» depends on how one views the situation. The full position was laid out in Dewey’s landmark article «The Reflex Arc Concept in Psychology» which also appeared in Psychological Review in 1896. Titchener responded in Philosophical Review (1898, 1899) by distinguishing his austere «structural» approach to psychology from what he termed the Chicago group’s more applied «functional» approach, and thus began the first major theoretical rift in American psychology between Structuralism and Functionalism.

Early French Psychology

Academic philosophy in France through the middle part of the nineteenth century was controlled by members of the eclectic and spiritualist schools, led by figures such as Victor Cousin (1792-1867), Théodore Jouffroy (1796-1842), and Paul Janet (1823-1899). These were traditional metaphysical schools, opposed to regarding psychology as a natural science. From 1870 forward, a steadily increasing interest in positivist, materialist, evolutionary, and deterministic approaches to psychology developed, influenced by, among others, the work of Hyppolyte Taine (1828-1893) (such as De L’Intelligence, 1870) and Théodule Ribot (1839-1916) (such as La Psychologie Anglaise Contemporaine, 1870).

In 1876, Ribot founded Revue Philosophique (the same year as Mind was founded in Britain), which for the next generation would be virtually the only French outlet for the «new» psychology. Although not a working experimentalist himself, Ribot’s many books were to have profound influence on the next generation of psychologists. These included especially his L’Hérédité Psychologique (1873) and La Psychologie Allemande Contemporaine (1879). In the 1880s, Ribot’s interests turned to psychopathology, writing books on disorders of memory (1881), will (1883), and personality (1885), and where he attempted to bring to these topics the insights of general psychology.

France’s primary psychological strength lay in the field of psychopathology. The chief neurologist at the Salpêtrière Hospital in Paris, Jean-Martin Charcot (1825-1893), had been using the recently revived and renamed practice of hypnosis to «experimentally» produce hysterical symptoms in some of his patients. Two of his students, Alfred Binet (1857-1911) and Pierre Janet (1859-1947), adopted and expanded this practice in their own work.

In 1889, Binet and his colleague Henri Beaunis (1830-1921) co-founded, at the Sorbonne, the first experimental psychology laboratory in France. Just five years later, in 1894, Beaunis, Binet, and a third colleague, Victor Henri (1872-1940), co-founded the first French journal dedicated to experimental psychology, L’Année Psychologique. In the first years of the twentieth century, Binet was requested by the French government to develop a method for the newly-founded universal public education system to identify students who would require extra assistance to master the standardized curriculum. In response, with his collaborator Théodore Simon (1873-1961), he developed the Binet-Simon Intelligence test, first published in 1905 (revised in 1908 and 1911). With Binet’s death in 1911, the Sorbonne laboratory and L’Année Psychologique fell to Henri Piéron (1881-1964), whose orientation was more physiological that Binet’s.

Pierre Janet became the leading psychiatrist in France, being appointed to the Salpêtrière (1890-1894), the Sorbonne (1895-1920), and the Collège de France (1902-1936). In 1904, he co-founded the Journale de Psychologie Normale et Pathologique with fellow Sorbonne professor Georges Dumas (1866-1946), a student and faithful follower of Ribot. Whereas Janet’s teacher, Jean-Martin Charcot, had focused on the neurological bases of hysteria, Janet was concerned to develop a scientific approach to psychopathology as a mental disorder. His theory that mental pathology results from conflict between unconscious and conscious parts of the mind, and that unconscious mental contents may emerge as symptoms with symbolic meanings led to a public dispute with Sigmund Freud.

Early British Psychology

Although the first scholarly journal dedicated to the topic of psychology—Mind, founded in 1876 by Alexander Bain and edited by George Croom Robertson—was British, experimental psychology did not develop there for some time due to the strong tradition of «mental philosophy.» The experimental reports that appeared in Mind in the first two decades of its existence were almost entirely authored by Americans, especially G. Stanley Hall and his students (notably Henry Herbert Donaldson) and James McKeen Cattell.

In 1884, Francis Galton (1822-1911) opened his anthropometric laboratory where people were tested on a wide variety of physical (such as strength of blow) and perceptual (such as visual acuity) attributes. In 1886 Galton was visited by James McKeen Cattell who would later adapt Galton’s techniques in developing his own mental testing research program in the United States. Galton was not primarily a psychologist, however. The data he accumulated in the anthropometric laboratory primarily went toward supporting his case for eugenics. To help interpret the mounds of data he accumulated, Galton developed a number of important statistical techniques, including the precursors to the scatterplot and the product-moment correlation coefficient (later perfected by Karl Pearson, 1857-1936).

Soon after, Charles Spearman (1863-1945) developed the correlation-based statistical procedure of factor analysis in the process of building a case for his two-factor theory of intelligence, published in 1901. Spearman believed that people have an inborn level of general intelligence or g which can be crystallized into a specific skill in any of a number of narrow content area (s, or specific intelligence).

Although the philosopher James Ward (1843-1925) urged Cambridge University to establish a psychophysics laboratory from the mid-1870s forward, it was not until the 1891 that they put so much as £50 toward some basic apparatus (Bartlett 1937). A laboratory was established through the assistance of the physiology department in 1897 and a lectureship in psychology was established which first went to W.H.R. Rivers (1864-1922). Soon Rivers was joined by C.S. Myers (1873-1946) and William McDougall (1871-1938). This group showed as much interest in anthropology as psychology, going with Alfred Cort Haddon (1855-1940) on the famed Torres Straits expedition of 1898. In 1901 the Psychological Society was established (which renamed itself the British Psychological Society in 1906), and in 1904 Ward and Rivers co-founded the British Journal of Psychology.

C. Lloyd Morgan (1852-1936) was an early British psychologist who pursued an experimental approach to animal psychology, and thus contributed to the establishment of psychology as a science. «Morgan’s canon» (perhaps the most quoted statement in the history of comparative psychology) states that higher psychological processes should not be used to explain behavior that can be explained by processes lower on the evolutionary scale, without independent evidence of the use of such higher processes on other occasions. This Canon, misrepresented as a specialized form of Occam’s razor namely that the simplest process should always be invoked as the explanation for behavior, played a critical role in the acceptance of Behaviorism in twentieth century academic psychology.

Second generation German Psychology

Würzburg School

In 1896, one of Wilhelm Wundt’s former Leipzig laboratory assistants, Oswald Külpe (1862-1915), founded a new laboratory in Würzburg. Külpe soon surrounded himself with a number of younger psychologists, most notably Karl Bühler (1879-1963), Ernst Dürr (1878-1913), Karl Marbe (1869-1953), and Scottish experimental psychologist Henry Jackson Watt (1879-1925). Collectively, they developed a new approach to psychological experimentation that flew in the face of many of Wundt’s restrictions. Wundt had drawn a distinction between the old philosophical style of self-observation (Selbstbeobachtung) in which one introspected for extended durations on higher thought processes and inner-perception (innere Wahrnehmung) in which one could be immediately aware of a momentary sensation, feeling, or image (Vorstellung). The former was declared to be impossible by Wundt, who argued that higher thought could not be studied experimentally through extended introspection: «we learn little about our minds from casual, haphazard self-observation…It is essential that observations be made by trained observers under carefully specified conditions for the purpose of answering a well-defined question» (Wundt 1904).

The Würzburgers, by contrast, designed experiments in which the experimental subject was presented with a complex stimulus (such as a Nietzschean aphorism or a logical problem) and after processing it for a time (interpreting the aphorism or solving the problem), retrospectively reported to the experimenter all that had passed through his consciousness during the interval. In the process, the Würzburgers claimed to have discovered a number of new elements of consciousness (over and above Wundt’s sensations, feelings, and images) including Bewußtseinslagen (conscious sets), Bewußtheiten (awarenesses), and Gedanken (thoughts). In the English-language literature, these are often collectively termed «imageless thoughts,» and the debate between Wundt and the Würzburgers as the «imageless thought controversy.» This debate is often said to have been instrumental in undermining the legitimacy of all introspective methods in experimental psychology and, ultimately, in bringing about the behaviorist revolution in American psychology.

Gestalt Psychology

Gestalt psychology, emerging in Germany in the early twentieth century, was a radical change from the psychology of Wilhelm Wundt who sought to understand the human mind by identifying the constituent parts of human consciousness in the same way that a chemical compound is broken into various elements. It also offered an alternative to the approach of Sigmund Freud, which was complex yet fraught with the complications of psychopathology. This group was not interested in mental illness; they sought to understand the processes of the healthy human mind, and in a scientific yet holistic fashion. They argued that the psychological «whole» has priority and that the «parts» are defined by the structure of the whole, rather than vice versa. Thus, the school was named Gestalt, a German term meaning approximately «form» or «configuration.» It was led by Max Wertheimer (1880-1943), Wolfgang Köhler (1887-1967), and Kurt Koffka (1886-1941).

The key argument in Gestalt psychology is that the nature of the parts and the whole are interdependent—the whole is not just the sum of its parts. The whole must be examined to discover what its part are, rather than trying to abstract the whole from analyzing the parts. For example, when one listens to music one hears the melody first and only then may perceptually divide it up into notes. We are directly aware of the configuration as a whole structure, its properties are perceived subsequent and secondarily to the perception of the whole; thus, the melody may be transposed into a different key using completely different notes, yet still be instantly recognizable to the listener.

Starting with the observation of apparent movement, an illusion known as the «phi phenomenon,» Wertheimer and his colleagues, Koffka and Köhler who served as his first experimental subjects, devised numerous experiments on visual perception, addressing not just the physiological capabilities of human eyes and brain, but the complexity of our interpretation of sensory input. In 1912, Wertheimer published a seminal paper on Experimentelle studien über das Sehen von Bewegung («Experimental Studies in the Perception of Movement»), which his students referred to informally as his Punkerbeit or “dot paper” because its illustrations were abstract patterns made of dots.

The collaborative work of the three Gestalt psychologists was interrupted by World War I. Both Wertheimer and Koffka were assigned to war-related research, while Köhler was appointed the director of an anthropoid research station on Teneriffe, in the Canary Islands. In 1917 Köhler published the results of four years of research on learning in chimpanzees. He showed, contrary to the claims of most other learning theorists, that animals can learn by «sudden insight» into the «structure» of a problem, over and above the associative and incremental manner of learning that Ivan Pavlov (1849-1936) and Edward Lee Thorndike (1874-1949) had demonstrated with dogs and cats, respectively.

After the war, Koffka returned to Frankfurt, while Köhler became the director of the Psychological Institute at the University of Berlin, where Wertheimer was already on the faculty. Using the abandoned rooms of the Imperial Palace, they established a now-famous graduate school, in tandem with a journal called Psychologische Forschung (Psychological Research: Journal of Psychology and its Neighboring Fields), in which they published their students’ and their own research.

With the help of American psychologist Robert Ogden, Koffka introduced the Gestalt point of view to an American audience in 1922 by way of a paper in Psychological Bulletin. Ogden also translated Koffka’s The Growth of the Mind in 1924, and that same year he arranged for Koffka to spend a year at Cornell. Koffka remained in the United States, eventually settling at Smith College in 1927. In the 1930s, with the rise of Adolf Hitler and Nazism, all the core members of the Gestalt movement were forced out of Germany to the United States.

In 1935 Koffka published his Principles of Gestalt Psychology. This textbook laid out the Gestalt vision, systematizing and advancing the ideas to the academic community. After his move to the United States, Wertheimer pursued research on problem solving, which he preferred to call «productive thinking» to distinguish it from «reproductive» thinking, the simple associative or trial and error learning that involved no insight. Such problem solving involves a transition from a situation which is meaningless or incomprehensible to one in which the meaning is clear, and Wertheimer argued that this is more than just making new connections, it involves structuring the information in a new way, forming a new Gestalt. He maintained contact with Koffka and Köhler, whose earlier work with chimpanzees on insight was along similar lines.

In 1940, Köhler published another book, Dynamics in Psychology, but thereafter the Gestalt movement suffered a series of setbacks. Koffka died in 1941 and Wertheimer in 1943. Wertheimer’s long-awaited book on mathematical problem solving, Productive Thinking was published posthumously in 1945, but Köhler was left to guide the movement without his two long-time colleagues.

Developmental psychology

The scientific study of children began in the late nineteenth century, and blossomed in the early twentieth century as pioneering psychologists sought to uncover the secrets of human behavior by studying psychological development.

Granville Stanley Hall, circa 1910.

Three early scholars, John Locke, Jean-Jacques Rousseau, and Charles Darwin proposed theories of human behavior that are the «direct ancestors of the three major theoretical traditions» (Vasta et al 1998, 10) of developmental psychology today. Locke, a British empiricist, adhered to a strict environmentalist position, that the mind of the newborn as a tabula rasa («blank slate») on which knowledge is written through experience and learning. Rousseau, a Swiss philosopher who spent much of his life in France, proposed a nativistic model in his famous novel Emile, in which development occurs according to innate processes progressing through three stages: infans (infancy), puer (childhood), and adolescence. Finally, the work of Darwin, the British biologist famous for his theory of evolution, led others to suggest that development proceeds through evolutionary recapitulation, with many human behaviors having their origins in successful adaptations in the past as «ontogeny recapitulates phylogeny.»

G. Stanley Hall, called the «father» of developmental psychology, is credited with conducting the first systematic studies of children. These involved questionnaires, which unfortunately were not structured in a way as to produce useful data. He was also unsuccessful in research that attempted to show that the child’s development recapitulates the evolution of the species. His major contributions to the field are that he taught the first courses in child development, several of his students becoming leading researchers in the field, and he established scientific journals for the publication of child development research.

Photo of the Jean Piaget Foundation with Pierre Bovet (1878-1965) first row (with large beard) and Jean Piaget (1896-1980) first row (on the right, with glasses) in front of the Rousseau Institute (Geneva), 1925

Arnold Gesell, a student of G. Stanley Hall, carried out the first large-scale detailed study of children’s behavior. His research revealed consistent patterns of development, supporting his view that human development depends on biological «maturation,» with the environment providing only minor variations in the age at which a skill might emerge but never affecting the sequence or pattern. Gesell’s research produced norms, such as the order and the normal age range in which a variety of early behaviors such as sitting, crawling, and walking emerge. In conducting his studies, Gesell developed sophisticated observational techniques, including one-way viewing screens and recording methods that did not disturb the child.

The Swiss psychologist Jean Piaget’s stage theory of cognitive development revolutionized our view of children’s thinking and learning. His work inspired more research than any other theorist, and many of his concepts are foundational to developmental psychology. His interest lay in children’s knowledge, their thinking, and the qualitative changes in their thinking as it develops. Piaget’s theory is «interactionist»—assigning importance to both «nature» and «nurture»—he called his field «genetic epistemology» stressing the role of biological determinism, but also emphasized the role of experience. In his view, children «construct» their knowledge through processes of «assimilation,» in which they evaluate and try to understand new information, based on their existing knowledge of the world, and «accommodation,» in which they expand and modify their cognitive structures based on new experiences. In addition to impacting the direction of developmental psychology, Piaget’s prolific output also stimulated the development of the field of cognitive psychology, in large part by those who sought to disprove his theory.

Emergence of Behaviorism in America

As a result of the conjunction of a number of events in the early twentieth century, Behaviorism gradually emerged as the dominant school in American psychology. First among these was the increasing skepticism with which many viewed the concept of consciousness: Although still considered to be the essential element separating psychology from physiology, its subjective nature and the unreliable introspective method it seemed to require, troubled many. C. Lloyd Morgan’s famous «Canon,» stating that higher psychological processes should not be used to explain behavior that can be explained by processes lower on the evolutionary scale without independent evidence of the use of such higher processes on other occasions (Morgan 1894), appeared to support the view that an entity should be considered conscious only if there was no other explanation for its behavior. William James’ 1904 article «Does Consciousness Exist?» laid out the worries explicitly; and Robert M. Yerkes’s 1905 article «Animal Psychology and the Criteria of the Psychic» raised the general question of when one is entitled to attribute consciousness to an organism.

Second was the gradual rise of a rigorous animal psychology. Edward Lee Thorndike worked with cats in puzzle boxes in 1898, and of research in which rats learn to navigate mazes was begun by Willard Small, who published two articles in the American Journal of Psychology (1900, 1901). The work of Russian Ivan Pavlov on conditioning in dogs began to be published in English in 1909.

A third factor was the rise of John B. Watson to a position of significant power within the psychological community. In 1908, Watson was offered a junior position at Johns Hopkins by James Mark Baldwin. In addition to heading the Johns Hopkins department, Baldwin was the editor of the influential journals, Psychological Review and Psychological Bulletin. Only months after Watson’s arrival, Baldwin was forced to resign his professorship due to scandal. Watson was suddenly made head of the department and editor of Baldwin’s journals. In 1913 he published in Psychological Review the article that is often called the «manifesto» of the Behaviorist movement, «Psychology as the Behaviorist Views It.» There he argued that psychology «is a purely objective experimental branch of natural science,» «introspection forms no essential part of its methods…» and «The behaviorist… recognizes no dividing line between man and brute.» The following year, 1914, his first textbook, Behavior went to press.

The central tenet of early behaviorism was that psychology should be a science of behavior, not of the mind, and rejected internal mental states such as beliefs, desires, or goals. Watson’s 1928 book, Psychological Care of the Infant and Child, presented his view that all behavior is the product of environment and experience with no important contribution by biological factors, and that all learning takes place through a process of association or «conditioning,» as proposed by Pavlov.

Watson himself, however, was forced out of Johns Hopkins by scandal in 1920. Although he continued to publish during the 1920s, he eventually moved on to a career in advertising. Behaviorism as a guiding psychological theory, however, was embraced and extended by such as Edward Thorndike, Clark L. Hull, Edward C. Tolman, and later B. F. Skinner. Throughout the first half of the twentieth century, Behaviorism reigned as the dominant model in American psychology, creating conditioning theories as scientific models of human behavior and successfully applying them in the workplace and fields such as advertising and military science.

Cognitivism

Cognitive psychology developed as a separate area within the discipline in the late 1950s and early 1960s, following the «cognitive revolution» ignited by Noam Chomsky’s 1959 critique of Behaviorism and Empiricism in general. Chomsky reviewed Skinner’s book Verbal Behavior (that aimed to explain language acquisition in a behaviorist framework), showing that language could not be learned solely from the sort of operant conditioning that Skinner postulated. Chomsky’s argument was that as people could produce an infinite variety of sentences unique in structure and meaning, and that these could not possibly be generated solely through experience of natural language. As an alternative, he concluded that there must be internal mental structures—states of mind of the sort that Behaviorism rejected as illusory.

Ulric Neisser coined the term «cognitive psychology» in his book Cognitive Psychology, published in 1967, in which he characterized people as dynamic information processing systems whose mental operations might be described in computational terms. The rise of computer technology and artificial intelligence also promoted the metaphor of mental function as information processing. This, combined with a scientific approach to studying the mind, as well as a belief in internal mental states, led to the rise of cognitivism as the dominant model of the mind.

Links between brain and nervous system function also became common, partly due to the experimental work of people like Charles Sherrington and Donald Hebb, and partly due to studies of people with brain injury. With the development of technologies for accurately measuring brain function, neuropsychology and cognitive neuroscience became some of the most active areas in psychology. With the increasing involvement of these other disciplines in the quest to understand the mind, the umbrella discipline of cognitive science was created as a means of focusing such efforts in a constructive way.

Humanistic movement

Not all psychologists, however, were happy with what they perceived as mechanical models of the mind and human nature associated with the Behaviorist approach (the «first force»). Nor were they satisfied with the field of «depth psychology» (the «second force») that grew out of Freud’s psychoanalytic approach and the work of Alfred Adler, Erik H. Erikson, Carl Jung, Erich Fromm, Otto Rank, Melanie Klein, and others. These theorists focused on the «depth» or unconscious realm of the human psyche, which, they stressed, must be combined with the conscious mind in order to produce a healthy human personality.

Humanistic psychology, sometimes called the «third force» in psychology, emerged in the late 1950s with two meetings held in Detroit, Michigan attended by psychologists who were interested in founding a professional association dedicated to a new vision of human development: a complete description of what it is to be a human being, especially the uniquely human aspects of experience, such as love and hope. Thus, they were also dissatisfied with the almost contemporary cognitivist view of the human mind as a computer, just processing information.

The humanistic approach stresses a phenomenological view of human experience and seeks to understand human beings and their behavior by conducting qualitative research. Many humanist psychologists completely reject a scientific approach, arguing that trying to turn human experience into measurements strips it of all meaning and relevance to lived existence. Some of the founding theorists behind this school of thought are Abraham Maslow, who formulated a hierarchy of human needs; Carl Rogers, who created and developed Client-centered therapy; and Fritz Perls, who helped create and develop Gestalt therapy. A further development of Humanistic psychology emerging in the 1970s was Transpersonal psychology, which studies the spiritual dimension of humanity.

With the dawning of the twenty-first century Positive psychology arose, originally a development of humanistic psychologists’ research on happiness and their focus on treating mental health rather than mental illness. The term «positive psychology» originates with Maslow whose last chapter of Motivation and Personality (Maslow 1970) is entitled «Toward a Positive Psychology.» It was Martin Seligman, though, who is considered the father of the modern positive psychology movement, after he introduced it as the theme for his term as president of the American Psychological Association. This approach is intended to complement, not replace, traditional psychology as it focuses on nurturing genius and to understand normal growth and development: «We believe that a psychology of positive human functioning will arise, which achieves a scientific understanding and effective interventions to build thriving in individuals, families, and communities» (Seligman and Csikszentmihalyi 2000).

References

ISBN links support NWE through referral fees

  • Ash, Mitchell G. 1998. Gestalt Psychology in German Culture, 1890-1967. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0521646277
  • Bartlett, F.C. 1937. Cambridge, England: 1887-1937. American Journal of Psychology 50: 97-110.
  • Bringmann, W.G., and R.D. Tweney (eds.) 1980. Wundt studies. Toronto: Hogrefe & Huber Publishing. ISBN 978-0889370012
  • Cadwallader, T.C. 1974. Charles S. Peirce (1839-1914): The first American experimental psychologist. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 10: 291-298.
  • Chomsky, Noam. 1967. «A Review of B. F. Skinner’s Verbal Behavior.» In Leon A. Jakobovits and Murray S. Miron (eds.), Readings in the Psychology of Language. Prentice-Hall. Includes «A Review of B. F. Skinner’s Verbal Behavior» published in 1959 in Language 35(1): 26-58. Retrieved April 13, 2012.
  • Danziger, K. 1997. Naming the Mind: How Psychology Found its Language. London: Sage. ISBN 978-0803977631
  • Evans, Rand B., Virginia Staudt Sexton, and Thomas C. Cadwallader (eds.). 1992. The American Psychological Association: A Historical Perspective. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. ISBN 978-1557981363
  • Everson, Stephen. (ed.) 1991. Companions to Ancient Thought 2: Psychology. Cambridge University Press. ISBN 978-0521358613
  • Fechner, Gustav T. 1860. Elemente der psychophysik. Thoemmes Continuum. ISBN 978-1855066571
  • Froh, Jeffrey J. 2004. The History of Positive Psychology: Truth Be Told. NYS Psychologist (May/June): 18-20. Retrieved April 13, 2012.
  • Green, Christopher D. 2000. Introduction to «Perception: An introduction to the Gestalt-Theorie» by Kurt Koffka (1922). Classics in the History of Psychology. Retrieved April 18, 2012.
  • Green, Christopher D., and Philip R. Groff. 2003. Early Psychological Thought: Ancient Accounts of Mind and Soul. Westport, CT: Praeger. ISBN 978-0313318450
  • Green, Christopher D., and Marlene G. Shore (eds.). 2001. The Transformation of Psychology: Influences of 19th-Century Philosophy, Technology, and Natural Science. Washington DC: American Psychological Association. ISBN 978-1557987761
  • Haque, Amber. 2004. Psychology from Islamic perspective: Contributions of early Muslim scholars and challenges to contemporary Muslim psychologists. Journal of Religion and Health 43(4): 357-377. Retrieved April 11, 2012.
  • Henle, Mary. 1978. One man against the Nazis: Wolfgang Köhler. American Psychologist 33: 939-944.
  • Henle, Mary. 1984. Robert M. Ogden and Gestalt Psychology in America. Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences 20: 9-19.
  • Khaleefa, Omar. 1999. Who Is the Founder of Psychophysics and Experimental Psychology? American Journal of Islamic Social Sciences (16) (2):1-26. Retrieved April 17, 2012.
  • Koch, Sigmund and David E. Leary (eds.) 1992. A Century of Psychology as Science. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. ISBN 155798171X
  • Koffka, Kurt. 1922. Perception: An Introduction to the Gestalt-theorie. Psychological Bulletin (19): 531-585. Retrieved April 17, 2012.
  • Koffka, Kurt. [1924] 2007. The Growth of the Mind (R. M. Ogden, Trans.). Kessinger Publishing, LLC. ISBN 978-0548102435
  • Koffka, Kurt. [1935] 1963. Principles of Gestalt psychology. New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace, & World. ASIN B0007DEO70
  • Köhler, Wolfgang. [1917] 1976. The Mentality of Apes. W. W. Norton. ISBN 0871401088
  • Köhler, Wolfgang. [1940] 1996. Dynamics in Psychology. Liveright Publishing Corporation. ISBN 0871402777
  • Krstic, K. 1964. Marko Marulic—The Author of the Term «Psychology» Acta Instituti Psychologici Universitatis Zagrabiensis 36: 7-13. Retrieved April 11, 2012.
  • Maslow, A.H. [1970] 1987. Motivation and Personality. New York, NY: Harper Collins. ISBN 978-0060419875
  • Mesmer, Anton. 1779. Propositions Concerning Animal Magnetism. Retrieved April 17, 2012.
  • Morgan, C. Lloyd. [1894] 2000. An Introduction to Comparative Psychology. Adamant Media Corporation. ISBN 978-1421227757
  • Neisser, Ulrich. 1967. Cognitive Psychology. Prentice Hall. ISBN 978-0131396678
  • Paranjpe, A.C. 2010. Self and Identity in Modern Psychology and Indian Thought. New York, NY: Springer. ISBN 978-1441932952
  • Piaget, Jean, and Barbel Inhelder. [1969] 2000. The Psychology of the Child. Basic Books. ISBN 978-0465095001
  • Peirce, Charles S., and Joseph Jastrow. 1884. On small differences of sensation. Memoirs of the National Academy of Sciences (3): 75-83. Retrieved April 17, 2012.
  • Rieber, Robert W., and David K. Robinson (eds.). 2001. Wilhelm Wundt in History: The Making of a Scientific Psychology. New York, NY: Kluwer & Plenum. ISBN 978-0306465994
  • Seligman, Martin E.P., and Mihaly Csikszentmihalyi. 2000. Positive Psychology: An Introduction. American Psychologist (55)(1): 5–14.
  • van Wyhe, J. 2004. Phrenology and the Origins of Scientific Naturalism. Aldershot, Hants, UK: Ashgate Pub Ltd. ISBN 978-0754634089
  • Vasta, Ross, Marshall M. Haith, and Scott A. Miller. 1998. Child Psychology: The Modern Science. Third Edition. New York, NY: John Wiley. ISBN 047119221X
  • Watson, John B. 1913. Psychology as the Behaviorist Views it. Psychological Review 20: 158-177. Retrieved April 17, 2012.
  • Watson, John B. [1928] 1976. Psychological Care of Infant and Child. Arno Press. ISBN 0405038763
  • Wertheimer, Max. 1912. Experimentelle Studien über das Sehen Bewegung. Zeitschrift für Psychologie 61: 247-250.
  • Wertheimer, Max. [1945] 1982. Productive Thinking. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 978-0226893761
  • Winter, Alison. 2000. Mesmerized: Powers of mind in Victorian Britain. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. ISBN 978-0226902234
  • Wozniak, Robert H. 1997. Experimental and Comparative Roots of Early Behaviorism: An Introduction. Retrieved April 14, 2012.
  • Wundt, Wilhelm M. 1904. Principles of Physiological Psychology. Translated by E.B. Titchener. Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Library. ASIN B0037NX8EC

External links

All links retrieved January 11, 2018.

  • The History of Psychology — e-text about the historical and philosophical background of psychology by C. George Boeree
  • Mind and Body: René Descartes to William James e-text by Robert H. Wozniak
  • Classics in the History of Psychology — on-line full texts of 250+ historically-significant primary source articles, chapters, & books, ed. by Christopher D. Green
  • Fondation Jean Piaget — Collection of primary sources by, and secondary sources about, Jean Piaget (in French; edited by Jean-Jacques Ducret and Wolfgang Schachner)
  • The Mead Project — collection of writings by George Herbert Mead and other related thinkers (e.g., Dewey, James, Baldwin, Cooley, Veblen, Sapir), ed. by Lloyd Gordon Ward and Robert Throop
  • Sir Francis Galton, F.R.S.
  • William James Site
  • Center for the History of Psychology — Large collection of documents and objects at the University of Akron

Credits

New World Encyclopedia writers and editors rewrote and completed the Wikipedia article
in accordance with New World Encyclopedia standards. This article abides by terms of the Creative Commons CC-by-sa 3.0 License (CC-by-sa), which may be used and disseminated with proper attribution. Credit is due under the terms of this license that can reference both the New World Encyclopedia contributors and the selfless volunteer contributors of the Wikimedia Foundation. To cite this article click here for a list of acceptable citing formats.The history of earlier contributions by wikipedians is accessible to researchers here:

  • History_of_psychology  history

The history of this article since it was imported to New World Encyclopedia:

  • History of «History of psychology»

Note: Some restrictions may apply to use of individual images which are separately licensed.

Psychology was not always its own discipline. When it comes to the origin of psychology, it helps to dig deeper to see how it has evolved and become the modern scientific discipline it is today. Psychology has gone through multiple stages and notable developments both in its approaches and measuring techniques to establish itself as an esteemed scientific discipline. Let’s explore the origin of psychology.

  • We are going to explore the origin of psychology by examining its history.
  • First, we will establish the history of psychology, working our way through the ages to see how it developed as a discipline.
  • Then, we will highlight the father of modern psychology before exploring the various views in the early days of psychology, including structuralism and functionalism.
  • We will also discuss psychoanalysis and humanism in an attempt to understand the origin and development of psychology.
  • We will, throughout our discussion, establish the founders of psychology and explore what we need to know for the origins of psychology at A Level.

History and Origin of Psychology

The word ‘psychology’ comes from two Greek words, psyche (meaning breath, soul, life, or spirit) and logos (meaning the study of). Wilhelm Wundt was the first to establish himself as a psychologist and opened a laboratory dedicated to the scientific study of psychology. Previously, psychology was more a subdivision of philosophy.

Fig. 1 - Depiction of psykheFig. 1 — Depiction of the psyche.

The word psychology appeared in literature as early as the 16th and 17th centuries. In the sixteenth century, a theologian named Philip Melanchthon, whom many believe, was the first to mention psychology through Latinised forms of the original Greek words.

Later psychology began to take on a new meaning, as popularised by Christian Wolff in his Psychologia Empirica. His work acknowledged that psychology is the science that examines mental phenomena, which is psychology’s early attempt to disconnect from philosophy and religion. However, many scholars still attach philosophy to their study of the mind during this time. But what does the origin of psychology have to do with philosophy?

Psychology originated in the ancient study of philosophy, the discipline of great historical minds such as Hippocrates, Plato, and Aristotle. Early philosophers spent their time theorising about how the world works, why we are here, and why people behave as they do.

Renowned French philosopher René Descartes (1596-1650) laid the foundation for studying the mind with his proposed dualism.

The idea of dualism is that the mind and body are separate entities that can explain how we experience human life.

Descartes suggested that the mind was non-physical, a state of consciousness and self-awareness that is not purely biological. In contrast, the body is a mechanical system that requires research and investigation. Separating the concepts of mind and body clarified that a study of the mind was needed.

Another important antecedent to psychology is Franz Joseph Gall (1758-1828), who introduced phrenology, the study of bumps and indentations in the skull. He believed that feeling the skull can reveal certain traits associated with a specific function and region in the brain.

It wasn’t until around the 19th century that psychology became more widely embraced, which developed into its modern meaning it is now as the study of mind and behaviour. In the next section, we will see psychology’s development and how it became a scientific discipline.

Father of Modern Psychology

Many consider Sigmund Freud to be the father of modern psychology. Wilhelm Wundt (1832-1920) was the first to establish a psychology laboratory in 1879 in Leipzig, Germany, later earning him the title “father of psychology” alongside his work entitled, Principles of Physiological Psychology, published in 1873.

Wundt attempted to study the mind objectively through a process that he called introspection.

Introspection is a technique in psychology involving observing one’s thoughts and feelings objectively.

Fig. 2 - Wilhelm WundtFig. 2 — Wilhelm Wundt

Although many credit Wundt for founding psychology in 1879, some researchers believe that Gustav Fechner was as qualified for the title given the important connection he made in 1850 between the mind and the body, later creating Fechner’s law.

Fechner’s law states that stimulus perception is related to stimulus intensity. His techniques opened doors for measuring behaviours in psychology.

Wundt’s attempts at the objective study of the mind involved training individuals in introspection and repeating tests to produce similar results. In this way, Wundt believed that he could identify the components that make up our mind and how these lead to a conscious experience, later forming one of psychology’s early approaches, structuralism.

Origin and Development of Psychology: Structuralism

With the need to study the mind came new ideas, methods, and practices that helped shape psychology into what it is today.

The very first psychological approach was called structuralism. Structuralism created a foundation on which early psychologists based their work. Edward Titchener (a student of Wundt) was a structuralist psychologist who assumed that human consciousness could be broken down into smaller parts and used Wundt’s process of introspection to understand these parts.

Aside from Wundt and Titchener, Granville Stanley Hall also contributed to the growth of psychology through their expansion of structuralism. G. Stanley Hall also set up a psychology laboratory at Johns Hopkins University, applying introspection, and was elected the American Psychological Association’s first president.

Origin and Development of Psychology: Functionalism

With the rise of structuralism in psychology came an opposing view: Functionalism.

Functionalism emphasises the function of thoughts and presents the mind as an entity to be examined holistically rather than broken into smaller components.

William James (1842-1910) looked at how humans are actively involved in their behaviour by studying the relationship between body processes (e.g., sensations) and psychology (e.g., behaviour).

James Cattell and John Dewey were functionalists who focused on mental tests and assessments of human abilities. Functionalism emphasises practical applications of psychology and how the mind adapts to the environment.

The approach laid the groundwork for more modern approaches such as behaviourism and impacted how we approach education and applied psychology today.

Origin and Development of Psychology: Behaviourism

While structuralism and functionalism focused on mental processes, behaviourism rejected studying the mind because of the belief that consciousness is unobservable. Instead, behaviourism emphasises observable behaviour in which the environment determines one’s actions. A person learns certain behaviours in response to a stimulus in the environment.

Important figures in Behaviourism included Edward Thorndike, Ivan Pavlov, Burrhus Frederic Skinner and John Watson. Edward Thorndike’s experiment on cats using puzzle boxes showed that one could learn behaviour in response to a stimulus if a reward follows it. Ivan Pavlov further expanded this idea by teaching dogs to salivate with the sound of a bell using his technique, classical conditioning.

Later, Skinner introduced his version of conditioning using rewards and punishment called operant conditioning. He experimented on mice and pigeons using the Skinner box. The experiment demonstrated that behaviour can be modified using positive and negative reinforcements.

John Watson, the father of behaviourism, mentioned that behaviour should be the subject of psychology. To Watson, psychology is about understanding how the environment shapes behaviour.

Origin and Development of Psychology: Psychoanalysis

Sigmund Freud (1856-1939) rejected behaviourism and developed psychoanalysis to understand and treat mental disorders. Freud’s approach assumes that we all have an unconscious ‘layer’ to our minds that controls most of our thoughts and behaviours. He also emphasised the role of early childhood experiences in shaping personality.

To Freud, allowing his clients to speak freely about their lives and feelings could help uncover unconscious ‘repressed’ memories from early childhood trauma causing behavioural symptoms.

Other researchers questioned its scientific validity, as it is hard to prove or disprove the existence of an unconscious part of the brain.

Origin and Development of Psychology: Humanism

Abraham Maslow and Carl Rogers disagreed with Behaviourism and psychoanalysis. To them, actions and thoughts are governed by the individual’s free will and capacity for development.

Humanists value emotion and think people are basically good. The premise of this approach is that humans have an innate desire to reach self-actualisation, the best version of themself. According to humanists reaching this level is quite difficult, and there are several other factors that the individual must achieve before reaching self-actualisation, such as physiological needs.

Indeed, the origin of psychology and its development showed how the need to study the mind led to an objective study from the early to late approaches of psychology, making it truly a scientific discipline.

Origin of Social Psychology

Being the social beings that we are, psychology also began investigating individual behaviour in the presence of others.

Social psychology is the scientific discipline that looks at how people interact with and are influenced by those in their social environments.

In 1924, Floyd Allport proposed in his text that social groups are one of the many stimuli an individual responds to in their environment. He also highlighted experimental research in this new psychology subfield.

Following the Great Depression in the 1930s and the impact of the First and Second World Wars, social psychologists aimed at social issues, which brought about ethics and values into social research. This opened doors for expanding social psychology and included areas such as intergroup relations, propaganda, voting, and organisational behaviour.

The 1940s to 1960s showed a rapid expansion of social psychology as evidenced by new research on authoritarian personality, obedience, persuasion, cognitive dissonance, aggression, prejudice and interpersonal attraction. By the 1970s and 1980s, social psychology faced a crisis, such as accusations of racial and gender biases, that made psychologists in this field reassess their methods.

Additionally, during this time, the cognitive revolution renewed interest in understanding the self among social psychologists. New ideas also arose from multicultural research, where there was an exchange of ideas among researchers.

With the dominance of social cognitive influences in the 1990s, some social psychologists considered emotions and motives to balance out both social and cognitive perspectives in social psychology. Until now, social psychology continues expanding as social psychologists innovate ways of measuring social behaviours.

Origin of Cognitive Psychology

With some psychologists rejecting behaviourism came another subfield of psychology, cognitive psychology, which aims to study higher mental processes.

Cognitive psychology investigates how the mind processes and influences behaviour.

In 1925, a German psychologist, Wolfgang Kohler, published his findings on The Mentality of Apes, showing that chimpanzees can learn insights and solve problems. In this experiment, the chimpanzee reached for the banana using a bamboo stick. Come 1948, Norbert Weiner, a mathematician, introduced the terms input, output, and feedback in his work Cybernetics: or Control and Communication in the Animal and the Machine.

Around the same time, Edward Tolman proposed the term cognitive maps, which are mental representations formed from environmental cues. He demonstrated cognitive maps by training rats to navigate a maze. Between the 1950s and 1960s, George Miller and Jerome Bruner established the Center for Cognitive Studies at Harvard University. Additionally, Miller popularised the concept that the mind can store up to seven objects in its short-term memory.

At this time, Ulric Neisser, the father of cognitive psychology, published Cognitive Psychology, signifying the beginning of the cognitive approach.

These concepts have shaped the popular cognitive psychology we know today.

Origin of Psychology — Key takeaways

  • Psychology is the scientific discipline of understanding the mind, its functions and behaviour.
  • The field originated in philosophy, with roots going back to the sixteenth century, beginning with René Descartes’ dualism, which created the need to study the mind and body separately.
  • Wilhelm Wundt is considered the father of psychology and established the first psychology laboratory in 1879.
  • Early psychological approaches focused on breaking down consciousness into smaller parts and understanding its relation to each other (structuralism) and holistic view of the mind and understanding of its functions (functionalism).
  • Later psychological approaches highlighted stimulus-response in behaviourism, the unconscious mind in psychoanalysis, personal development in humanism, and social behaviour and higher mental processes in social and cognitive approaches, respectively.

References

  1. Fig. 2 – Wilhelm Wundt (https://commons.wikimedia.org/wiki/File:Wilhelm_Wundt._Photogravure_by_Synnberg_Photo-gravure_Co.,_1_Wellcome_L0023076.jpg) by Wellcome Library (https://wellcomecollection.org/works/a3eqvhj9?wellcomeImagesUrl=/indexplus/image/L0023076.html) is licensed by CC BY 4.0 (https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/deed.en)

Понравилась статья? Поделить с друзьями:
  • Highlight letters in word
  • History of the word pollution
  • Highlight keywords in word
  • History of the word patient
  • Highlight image in word