Word problems for Year 5 are a great way to assess pupils’ number fluency. By the time primary school children reach Upper Key Stage 2, they will be building on their knowledge and understanding of the number system and place value, working with larger integers. An increased understanding of the connections between the different maths concepts is crucial, as children will be expected to tackle more complex, multi-step problems.
It is important children are regularly provided with the opportunity to solve a range of word problems and Year 5 maths worksheets with reasoning and problem solving incorporated alongside fluency, into all lessons.
To help you with this, we have put together a collection of 18 word problems aimed at Year 5 maths pupils.
All Kinds of Word Problems
Download this free pack of multiplication word problems to help your Year 5 class grow their problem solving skills
Year 5 maths word problems in the national curriculum
The National Curriculum states that by Year 5, pupils should develop their ability to solve a wider range of problems, including increasingly complex properties of numbers and arithmetic, and problems demanding efficient written and mental methods of calculation. Children also begin to use the language of algebra for solving a range of problems.
By Year 5, pupils should be starting to gain confidence in solving more complex two-step and multi-step word problems, covering topics across the Year 5 curriculum.
Place value
Solve number and practical problems involving ordering and comparing numbers to at least 1,00,000; counting forwards or backwards in steps of powers of 10; interpreting negative numbers in context and rounding to the nearest 10,100, 1000, 10,000 and 100,000.
Addition and subtraction
Solve addition and subtraction word problems and a combination of these, including understanding the meaning of the equals sign.
Multiplication and division
Solve problems involving multiplication and division, including using knowledge of factors, multiples, squares and cubes and scaling by simple fractions.
Fractions, decimals and percentages
Solve problems involving numbers up to 3 decimal places and problems which require knowing percentage and decimal equivalents.
Measurement
Solve time word problems and those involving converting between units of time and problems involving measure (for example, length, mass, volume and money word problems) using decimal notation and scaling.
Statistics
Solve comparison, sum and difference problems, using information presented in a line graph.
Why are word problems important in Year 5 maths?
Word problems are an important element of the Year 5 curriculum. By this stage, children need to be building confidence in approaching a range of one, two and multi-step word problems. They require children to be creative and apply the skills they have learnt to a range of real-life situations.
How to teach problem solving in Year 5
Children need to be taught the skills for successfully approaching and tackling word problems. Reading the question carefully and identifying the key information needed to tackle the problem, is the first step, followed by identifying which calculations are required for solving it and deciding whether it will be helpful to draw a picture/ visual representation to understand and answer the question.
Using mental maths skills to round and estimate an answer is also very helpful for children to establish whether their final answer is realistic. Children need to also be able to calculate the inverse, to be able to check their answer, once the problem has been completed.
See also: Mental maths year 5
Here is an example:
A transport museum has 1243 visitors on Monday morning and another 1387 visitors in the afternoon. On Tuesday 736 fewer visitors go to the museum than who visited on Monday.
How many visitors were there altogether on Tuesday?
How to solve:
What do you already know?
- The number of visitors on Monday morning and Monday afternoon are given separately. They need to be added together to give the total number of visitors for Monday.
- ‘Fewer’ means I will need to subtract the number of fewer visitors on Tuesday from the total number of visitors on Monday.
- Column addition and subtraction will be needed to solve this question.
How can this be drawn/represented pictorially?
We can draw a bar model to represent this problem:
- To calculate the total number of visitors on Monday, we need to add 1243 and 1387 together. 1243 + 1387 = 2630
- The number of fewer visitors on Tuesday needs to be subtracted from Monday’s total: 2630 – 736 = 1894.
- The total number of visitors on Tuesday was 1894.
Addition word problems for Year 5
In Year 5, addition word problems can involve whole numbers with over 4 digits and decimal numbers. Children should be able to round number to check the accuracy and begin to solve two step number problems
Addition question 1
Gemma picks two cards from the cards below and adds them together.
She is able to make three different totals. What will they be?
2365 6281 9782
Answer (2 marks): 12,147 16,063 8646
Addition question 2
Ahmed adds two of these numbers mentally.
357 280 294 232
In his calculation, he exchanges twice to create one ten and one hundred.
Write Ahmed’s calculation and work out the total.
Answer (1 mark): 357 + 294 = 651
Addition question 3
Change one digit in the calculation below, so that the answer is a multiple of 10.
726 + 347
Answer (1 mark): 723 + 347 = 1070
Subtraction word problems for Year 5
Subtraction word problems in Year 5 require pupils to be confident subtracting numbers over 4 digits and problems involving decimal numbers. Pupils need to be able to round numbers to check accuracy and to use subtraction when solving mixed word problems.
Subtraction question 1
A coach is travelling 4924 km across the USA
It has 2476 km to go.
How many kilometres has the coach already travelled?
Answer (1 mark): 2448 km
Subtraction question 2
A primary school printed 7283 maths worksheets in the Summer term. 2156 were for Key Stage 1 pupils. How many were printed for Key Stage 2?
Answer (1 mark): 5127 worksheets
Subtraction question 3
A clothing company made £57,605 profit in 2021 and £73,403 in 2022.
How much more profit did the company make in 2022 than in 2021?
Answer (1 mark): £15,798
Multiplication word problems for year 5
In year 5, multiplication word problems include problems involving times tables and multiplying whole numbers up to 4-digits by 1 or 2-digit numbers. Pupils also need to be able to combine multiplication with other operations, in order to solve two-step word problems.
Multiplication question 1
In this diagram, the numbers in the circles are multiplied together to make the answer in the square between them.
Complete the missing numbers.
Answer (1 mark)
Multiplication question 2
Mrs Jones was printing the end of year maths test. Each test had 18 pages and 89 pupils were sitting the test. Mrs Jones also needed to print out 12 copies for the teachers and Teaching Assistants who were helping to run the test.
How many pieces of paper did Mrs Jones need to put in the photocopier, to make sure she had enough for all the tests?
Answer (1 mark)
Multiplication question 3
A school is booking a trip to Alton Towers. Tickets cost £22 per pupil.
There are 120 children in each year group and all the children from 3 year groups will be going.
What will be the total price for all the tickets?
Answer (2 marks): £7920
Third Space Learning often ties word problems into our online one-to-one tutoring. Each lesson is personalised to the needs of the individual student, growing maths knowledge and problem solving skills.
Division word problems for year 5
In Year 5, division word problems can involve whole numbers up to 4 digits being divided by 1-digit numbers. Pupils need to understand how to answer word problems, when the answer involves a remainder.
Division question 1
Tom has 96 cubes and makes 12 equal towers.
Masie has 63 cubes and makes 9 equal towers.
Whose towers are tallest and by how many cubes?
Answer (2 marks): Tom
Tom’s tower has more cubes. His towers have 1 more cube than Maise’s towers.
96 ÷ 12 = 8
63 ÷ 9 = 7
Division question 2
A cake factory has made cakes to deliver to a large event.
265 cakes have been baked. How many boxes of 8 cakes can be delivered to the event?
Answer (2 marks): 33 boxes
Division question 3
Lily collected 1256 stickers. She shared them between her 8 friends. How many stickers did each friend get
Answer (1 mark): 157 stickers
Fraction and decimal word problems in Year 5
In Year 5, fractions word problems and decimals can include questions involving ordering, addition and subtraction of fractions. They can also involve converting between fractions and decimals.
Fraction and decimal question 1
Isobel collected 24 conkers.
She gave frac{1}{8} of the conkers to her brother.
How many conkers did she have left?
Answer (1 mark): 21 conkers left
frac{1}{8} of 24 = 3
24 – 4 = 21
Fraction and decimal question 2
Ahmed counted out 32 sweets. He gave frac{1}{4} of the sweets to his brother and frac{3}{8} of the sweets to his friend.
How many sweets did he have left?
Answer (2 marks): 12 sweets
frac{1}{4} of 32 = 8
frac{3}{8} of 32 = 12
He gave away 20 sweets, so had 12 left for himself.
Fraction and decimal question 3
Two friends shared some pizzas, 1 ate 1 frac{1}{2} pizzas, whilst the other ate frac{5}{8} of a pizza. How much did they eat altogether?
Answer (1 mark): 2 frac{1}{8} of pizza
1 frac{1}{2} = frac{12}{8}
frac {12}{8} + frac{5}{8} = frac{17}{8} = 2 frac{1}{8} pizzas
Mixed four operation word problems
Problems with mixed operations, or ‘multi-step’ word problems, require two or more operations to solve them. A range of concepts can be covered within mixed problems, including the four operations, fractions, decimals and measures. These are worth more marks than some of the more straightforward, one-step problems.
Mixed operation question 1
At the cake sale, Sam buys 6 cookies and a cupcake . He pays £2.85 altogether.
Naya buys 2 cookies and pays 90p altogether.
How much does the cupcake cost?
Answer (2 marks): 60p for one cupcake
Mixed operation question 2
Large biscuit tin – 48 biscuits (picture of boxes here)
Small biscuit tin – 30 biscuits
Ben bought 2 large tins of biscuits and 3 small tins.
How many biscuits did he buy altogether?
Answer (2 marks): 186 biscuits
2 x 48 = 96
3 x 30 = 90
96 + 90 = 186 biscuits
Mixed operation question 3
The owner of a bookshop bought a box of 15 books for £150.
He sold the books individually for £12 each.
How much profit did he make?
Answer (2 marks)
Word problem resources
Third Space Learning offers a wide array of maths and word problems resources for other year groups such as word problems for year 6, word problems for year 3 and word problems for year 4. Our word problem collection covers all four operations and other specific maths topics such as ratio word problems and percentage word problems.
Do you have pupils who need extra support in maths?
Every week Third Space Learning’s maths specialist tutors support thousands of pupils across hundreds of schools with weekly online 1-to-1 lessons and maths interventions designed to plug gaps and boost progress.
Since 2013 we’ve helped over 145,000 primary and secondary school pupils become more confident, able mathematicians. Learn more or request a personalised quote for your school to speak to us about your school’s needs and how we can help.
Word problems are a special kind of math challenge. Not only do they require computation skills, but they also test students’ reading comprehension and problem-solving abilities. This can be the perfect storm of frustration for many kids. But it doesn’t have to be so hard! Today, I’m sharing easy strategies for helping students master the word problem!
Start With Reading Comprehension
The first step in conquering word problems goes right back to good ol’ reading comprehension. Kids have a bad habit of reading word problems like they are just passing through words to get to the numbers. Blah, blah, blah. 127! Blah, blah, blah, 52! They search for numbers and then rush to DO something with those numbers.
If our students are going to find success with word problems, they need to use their good reading strategies! Teach your students to:
Reread: Slow down and reread as many times as needed
Visualize: Imagine what is taking place in the problem. What is the order of events?
Ask questions: What do I know? What do I need to find out?
Make predictions: What would be a reasonable result? Do I expect the answer to be more or less than the numbers I’m given?
Teach a Problem-Solving Routine
Help students form effective thinking habits by teaching and practicing a problem-solving process. I’m not talking about a rote set of steps like CUBES (Circle the numbers, Underline the question, Box key words, etc.). I’m talking about a THINKING routine that helps the child learn the critical thinking skills involved in solving real problems.
I’ve found a simple PLAN, SOLVE, CHECK format works best. With young children, the biggest issue I find is that they simply skip (or struggle with) one of these steps, which causes a breakdown in their ability to solve the problem. In fact, I form my guided math groups for word problems around those three categories (planning strategies, solving strategies, checking strategies.) **You can grab a FREE Word Problem Thinking Mat in the Preview File of my Tiered Word Problems Pack.
Compare Problem Structures
One of my favorite word problem teaching tools is something I call “Side-by-Sides.” This is simply where we solve two different problems and then compare the structure of the problem and the operation used.
When students model, solve, and discuss the different problem types, they grow a deeper understanding of how word problems work.
**You can grab FREE samples of side-by-side problems in the Preview File of my Side-by-Side Word Problems Pack or grab a FREE Side-by-Side Mat in the Preview File of my Tiered Word Problems Pack for 2nd Grade or 3rd Grade.
Start With the Easy Version
Differentiation isn’t just for reading! Many students get overwhelmed when they see large numbers in word problems, so why not meet them where they are? I use what I call “Tiered Word Problems” to help students understand word problem structures and grow confidence before tackling the on-level version.
In my Word Problem Solving Bundle, I provide all kinds of leveled word problems – Tiered Problem Pages, Task Cards, Exit Tickets, and more, that all increase in challenge from first through fifth grade. This allows me to scaffold instruction by student need, and throughout the school year.
**You can grab free goodies and sample pages from the Bundle in the Word Problem Solving Bundle Sampler Pack.
Use Problem-Solving Rounds
Problem-Solving Rounds changed math instruction in my classroom forever! This powerful routine encourages students to move through the problem-solving process I mentioned at the beginning of this post, to explain their thinking, and to reflect on other possible strategies and solutions.
Since using Problem-Solving Rounds, my students are more confident with word problems, have a deeper understanding of problem-types, and are better able to tackle new and challenging tasks.
The basic structure of the rounds follows the Problem-Solving Process I’ve shared. In Round 1, students read and discuss the problem with a focus on UNDERSTANDING. In Round 2, they turn to a partner and explain how they PLAN to solve the problem. They SOLVE the problem independently in Round 3, and in Round 4 they work with a partner and as a whole class to CHECK and reflect on their work. For a detailed guide to this process, check out my blog post all about Problem-Solving Rounds.
I’d love to hear from you! Comment below with your favorite word problem tips for teachers!
We recommend reading any word problem at least three times. By the conclusion of the third reading, you should be magically whisked away to your home in Kansas.
Read it once…
…to get a general idea of what’s going on. Is the problem about money? Height and width? Distances? Money? Yeah, we already said money, but it’s important. Can you draw a picture in the margin that helps you visualize the problem?
Twice…
…to translate from English into math. Read the problem carefully this time, figuring out which pieces of information are important and which ones aren’t. Then ignore the unnecessary bits. This may sound a bit harsh, but they’re big boys. They’ll get over it.
Three times…
…a lady. Wait, that’s not it.
…to make sure you answered the right question. To be sure, read the question one last time before drawing a box around your final answer. Even if this method of double-checking saves you only once out of every 100 times, it’ll help you in the long run. One out of 100 is better than zero out of 100, but now we’re moving into some advanced mathematics.
Sample Problem
The local department store was having a sale. Holla! Gabe bought a pair of shoes for $21, although they would’ve been cheaper if he’d bought penny loafers. He also bought some shirts that were on sale for 25% off their normal retail price of $18 each. Gabe, always the bargain hunter, spent $75 total. How many shirts did he buy? Also, does he really think any of them will go with those shoes?
Read the problem:
Once…
…for a general idea of what’s going on: Gabe went shopping and spent money. Sounds like an old familiar story. Time for an intervention, friends of Gabe. Preferably before he maxes out his Diner’s Club card.
Now we need to figure out how many shirts he bought, so we read the problem:
Twice…
…to translate from English into math. Remember, we can translate a bit at a time, sort of like how Gabe pays for some of his major purchases when he has them on layaway. Sheesh, Gabe. Get a hold of yourself.
(total amount Gabe spent) = (amount he spent on shoes) + (amount he spent on shirts)
We know he spent $75 total, of which $21 was spent on shoes. This gives us the equation:
$75 = 21 + (amount he spent on shirts)
The problem has gotten smaller. It’s depressing when that happens with cake, but great when it happens to a word problem. Now all we need to do is come up with a symbolic expression for how much Gabe spent on shirts, or the cost per shirt times the number of shirts:
(amount he spent on shirts) = (cost per shirt)(number of shirts)
Since the shirts are 25% off their normal price of $18, they cost $18 – 0.25(18) = $13.50 each. What a deal, and real polyester, too!
We need to introduce a variable for the number of shirts; s will do the trick nicely.
(amount he spent on shirts) = 13.5s
When we plug this into the earlier equation, we find that:
75 = 21 + 13.5s
Finally, we’ve reduced this thing to a super-simple-looking equation! Good riddance, vestiges of language! Begone, nouns and verbs!
Things look much nicer now, right? No worrying about shirts or shoes or prices or Gabe’s uncontrollable shopping addiction. For the moment, we can forget about the word problem and solve the equation. The answer is s = 4, by the way. In case you were interested.
Three times…
…to make sure we’re answering the right question. We want to know how many shirts Gabe bought. Is that the answer we arrived at? We found that s = 4, and s was the number of shirts Gabe bought, so we’re all done. Four new shirts for Gabe, and three of them feature a Hawaiian pattern. Gabe, if you insist on buying far more clothes than you need, can’t you at least have a decent fashion sense?
When translating from English into math, some information can be ignored. We don’t care that «the local department store was having a sale.» Gabe might, but we certainly don’t. We care about statements that tell us numbers, and statements that tell us what the question is. Any extraneous information has been placed there simply as a decoy. We’re not going to fall for that. Wait a second…two for one? We’ll grab our jacket and meet you there.
Some people find it helpful to underline the important pieces of information in a word problem. You’re like an actor highlighting in a script the lines that are important for him to remember. Unlike an actor, however, you can always look back at the original problem if you draw a blank. Also, you don’t need to wear any stage makeup.
In the problem we just did, the important bits might look something like this:
The local department store was having a sale. Gabe bought a pair of shoes for $21 and some shirts that were on sale for 25% off their normal retail price of $18 each. If Gabe spent $75 total, how many shirts did he buy?
As you practice, you’ll become better at figuring out which parts of the word problem you can ignore and which parts are important.
Word Problems
Introduction
Word problems (WPs) are part of the school curriculum and are taught at all levels of education. In WPs, relevant information is presented in the form of a short narrative rather than in mathematical notation (Verschaffel et al., 2000). Sometimes WPs specifically encode a quantitative relation between objects (Boonen et al., 2013). Many children from kindergarten through adulthood have severe difficulties in solving WPs (Nesher and Teubal, 1975; Riley et al., 1983; Lewis and Mayer, 1987; Hegarty et al., 1992; Verschaffel et al., 1992). Both linguistic and numerical complexity contributes to the difficulty in solving WPs. However, researchers have so far often focused on the one or the other aspect, depending on which field they come from. Even within the respective fields, linguistics, and numerical cognition, some aspects have been studied extensively, while others have been (strangely) neglected. For instance, we will see that semantics and discourse structures have been frequently studied in the context of WP complexity, but systematic syntactic manipulations are scarce. As regards numerical cognition, number properties like parity and magnitude as well as the type of mathematical reasoning have often been studied, but the type and the form of operations (e.g., carry-over effects) have not been investigated thoroughly in WPs, although they play an important role in current numerical cognition research (Moeller et al., 2011; Nuerk et al., 2011, 2015).
In this review, as researchers from the field of linguistics and the field of numerical cognition we have collaborated to provide a systematic overview of linguistic and numerical aspects relevant to solving WPs as well as their interaction. To capture a broad range of relevant facets in the review, we extended our view of the relevant literature with systematic keyword searches in several databases (Web of Science, Ebsco, Google Scholar, ScienceDirect) including the following terms: WPs, story problems in combination with situational model, performance, consistency hypothesis, language processing, relational terminology, semantic influence, rewording, semantic cues, number size and type, working memory, text comprehension, computational errors, operations, position of unknown. In Table 1, we present selected linguistic, mathematical and general factors investigated in previous studies.
TABLE 1. Selected linguistic, mathematical, and general factors investigated in previous studies.
Individual Differences and Social Factors
Individual differences and social factors must be also considered in WP research (Fite, 2002). For example, in the PISA studies—often measured with WPs—, mathematics literacy is a commonly used notion (Stacey, 2012). “It is defined as an individual’s capacity to identify and understand the role that mathematics plays in the world, to make well-founded judgments and to use and engage with mathematics in ways that meet the needs of that” (OECD, 2010). Unsuccessful WP solvers can experience negative social health and life outcome (Schley and Fujita, 2014). Even beyond social consequences, numerous studies focused on individual differences and group differences, such as students with and without learning disabilities (Kingsdorf and Krawec, 2014), and children with and without developmental disabilities (Neef et al., 2003). Hegarty et al. (1995) distinguished domain-specific strategies that successful and unsuccessful problem solvers develop with practice and how these strategies account for individual differences in performance. Different students – e.g., individuals with calculation difficulty, or WP difficulty (Powell and Fuchs, 2014) – may struggle with different types of WPs. Besides domain-general capabilities like IQ, the role of domain specific knowledge and processes were investigated to get a complete account of problem solving, basic cognitive abilities; visual, reading skills, mathematical skills, and metacognitive abilities involved in the solution process. For example Boonen et al. (2014) and Oostermeijer et al. (2014) explored the role of spatial ability and reading comprehension in WP solving, since good WP solvers do not select numbers and relational keywords but create a visual representation (Boonen et al., 2013).
Social factors like schooling, teachers and peers also deserve consideration because the way of responding [e.g., De Corte et al. (1988)], the scoring criteria, the presence of illustrations next to the text [e.g., Berends and van Lieshout (2009)], or solution models used by the teachers influence WP performance considerably. School WPs also support stereotypical thinking: WPs do not resemble problems in real-world situations (Yee and Lee, 1997). In addition, there is a strong tendency among both students and teachers to exclude real-world knowledge from their WP solution (Verschaffel et al., 1997), which is consistent with the observation that the problem solving process is also influenced by social cognitive and epistemic behavior settings (Reusser, 1988). Linguistic and pedagogical factors also affect children’s understanding of arithmetic WPs (Lean et al., 1990). Students’ beliefs about what doing and knowing mathematics means are rather different from the ideals (Jimenez and Verschaffel, 2014) and shaped by “socio-mathematical norms.” Resulting differences in motivation seem to influence the strategies used to solve WPs (Gasco and Villarroel, 2014). In sum, both individual differences and social factors contribute to WP performance and deserve consideration.
Subcategories of Word Problems and Solution Strategies
Several different types of WPs—e.g., in the underlying mathematical structure or solvability— are often presented intermixed in one study without acknowledging the problem type. This is problematic. Different types of WPs are presented for various student groups, in different schools or different age groups. For example, Swanson et al. (2013) investigated the role of strategy instruction and cognitive abilities on WP solving accuracy. The mathematical WPs they used were: addition, subtraction, and multiplication without any further description of the problem type. However, the available literature has already shown that different categories of WPs may lead to different solution strategies and different error types. For instance, different semantic problem types result in different errors (Vicente et al., 2007) and have a different difficulty level (LeBlanc and Weber-Russell, 1996). Obviously, different scientific studies reporting results for different student or age groups cannot be easily compared to one another when they use different WP types; it cannot be determined whether differences should be attributed to group or study manipulation or differences in the used stimulus material. In the following, we outline the major distinctions discussed in the literature. Besides the difficulty level, WPs have been categorized with regard to various other attributes. Based on standard algebra text books, Mayer (1981) categorized WPs according to their frequency. Riley et al. (1983) created four groups based on the semantic structure of additive arithmetic WPs (change, compare, combine, equalize) and 18 further subcategories. For instance, the change problem –where there is a start, a change, and a result state –can be subdivided into three subcategories depending on which state is the unknown.
The mathematical content of WPs can also serve as a basis for categorization. Algebra WPs typically require translation into a mathematical formula, whereas arithmetic WPs are solvable with simple arithmetic or even mental calculation. In contrast to arithmetic WPs, algebraic reasoning WPs share the same numerals and signs (Powell and Fuchs, 2014) and the manipulation of those numbers and signals differs based on the question or expected outcome (Kieran, 1990). However, the distinction is not that straightforward, as in some cases both methods can be applied. For instance, in a study by Van Dooren et al. (2002), future secondary school teachers preferred the use of algebra even when an arithmetical solution seemed more evident, and some future primary school teachers rather applied arithmetical methods. Computer-aided environments have been introduced for algebraic WPs (Reusser, 1993) to support learning on “getting the formalism” and the “equation” (Nathan et al., 1992) and to allow students to generate, manipulate, and understand abstract formal expressions for WPs. However, solution approaches are not easily dissociable between arithmetic and algebraic problems. If a WP is intended to be solved with an equation, in some cases a simple arithmetic approach is enough (Gasco et al., 2014). Under some circumstances, it is even easier to solve WPs via alternative arithmetic strategies than by deriving algebraic equations. US children perform better on a story problem if it is in a money context and the numbers involve multiples of 25 (Koedinger and Nathan, 2004). While the distinction between algebra and arithmetic WPs is important for investigation and evaluation, in this review we concentrate mainly on arithmetic WPs.
Standardized phrases and the idea that every problem is solvable are other important attributes of many, but not all WPs. Textbooks generally suggest implicitly that every WP is solvable and that every numerical information is relevant (Pape, 2003). They usually provide standardized phrases and keywords that are highly correlated with correct solutions (Hinsley et al., 1977; ?). There are so-called non-standard WPs (Jimenez and Verschaffel, 2014) which can be non-solvable WPs or if they are solvable some have multiple solutions and may contain irrelevant data. In the recent literature, non-standard WPs are getting more and more attention (Yeap et al., 2005; Csikos et al., 2011). Children give a high level of incorrect answers to non-standard WPS because these seem to contradict their mathematics-related beliefs learned in the classroom. Reusser (1988) presented 97 first and second graders with the following sentence: “There are 26 sheep and 10 goats on a ship. How old is the captain?” and 76 students “solved” the problem using the numbers in the task. The rationale behind such studies is that always-solvable textbook problems with standardized phrases and including only relevant numerical information are hardly ecologically valid. Real-life WPs are not standardized, contain irrelevant information, and a solution may not always exist.
The above subcategories, which essentially characterize specific sets of WP properties, have a direct impact on human performance in WP. For space limitations, we cannot discuss the impact of all subcategories in detail, but we illustrate their impact on performance and strategies with two examples: (i) different subcategories can result in different errors, and involve different representations and processes. For example, a familiar misconception is that multiplication (Vergnaud, 2009) always makes the result larger (which is not true for n < 1), that division makes the results smaller, and that division always involves division of the larger number by the smaller, (ii) addition problems are strongly influenced (De Corte and Verschaffel, 1987) by the semantic structure (change, compare, combine). Carpenter et al. (1981) reported that the dominant factor in determining the children’s solution strategy was this semantic structure. For instance, Change problems [cf. the classification of Riley et al. (1983)] require the child to find the difference between the two numbers given in the problem; their nature influences the strategies children adopt. Riley et al. (1983) illustrates this with the following examples: Change 2: “Joe had eight marbles. Then he gave five marbles to Tom. How many marbles does Joe have now?” Change 3: “Joe had three marbles. Then Tom gave him some more marbles. Now Joe has eight marbles. How many marbles did Tom give him?” Almost all the children used a subtraction strategy (e.g., counting up) to solve Change 2. For Change 3 almost all the children used an addition strategy (e.g., counting down). In sum, the subcategories introduced in this section influence both performance and the choice of solution strategies.
Indeed, solution strategies have systematically been in the focus of WP research and addressed the following questions: how do children and adults solve WPs? Why do they make different errors and at which level of the solution process they do so? Which kind of semantic representation do they create of the WP? Which skills are necessary for the solution process? The first theories on WP solution processes (Kintsch and Greeno, 1985) have drawn on the text comprehension theories of Mayer (1982) and Van Dijk and Kintsch (1983). When solving problems, the solver first integrates the textual information into an appropriate situation model or a mental representation of the situation being described in the problem, which then forms the basis for a solution strategy. This approach was further applied by (Thevenot and Oakhill, 2005; Jimenez and Verschaffel, 2014; Kingsdorf and Krawec, 2014). An important foundation of those approaches is that solving WPs is not a simple translation of problem sentences into equations (Paige and Simon, 1966). Often both WPs and the corresponding numerical problems are done without language translation (Schley and Fujita, 2014). Several researchers have focused on abstraction as a reductive process involved in the translation process in the WPs. Nathan et al. (1992) argue that WPs solving is an exercise in text processing required for understanding the problem (Cummins et al., 1988), which is highly dependent upon language comprehension skills. Successfully solving WPs has been argued to require at least three distinct processes (Nesher and Teubal, 1975): (i) understanding and constructing the relation between text and arithmetic task, (ii) linguistic understanding of the WP itself, and (iii) solving the arithmetic tasks. Typically only the latter process is assumed to be shared with common arithmetic tasks. Many students can successfully solve common arithmetic tasks and they show good text comprehension skills. Yet they fail to solve WPs correctly. This suggests that other factors like solution strategies and building up a mental model of the task also play a major role for the WP performance.
Linguistic Complexity and Linguistic Studies
In linguistics, the notion of complexity is discussed under a range of perspectives, with particularly fruitful definitions grounded in research on language evolution (Nichols, 1990) and language acquisition (Bulté and Housen, 2012). Following the latter, it is useful to delineate linguistic complexity from propositional complexity (the amount of meaning to be expressed) and discourse-interactional complexity (the interaction of participants in discourse). This makes it possible to zoom in on linguistic complexity as the degree to which a text at hand is elaborated and varied (Ellis, 2003, p. 340). Linguistic complexity can be analyzed with respect to all aspects of the linguistic system: from the words and their lexical and morphological aspects, via the way these words can be combined in syntax to form sentences, to the text structure, and overall discourse. Languages differ with respect to where in the linguistic system complexification is supported. For example, English makes use of word order to encode grammatical functions, whereas agglutinative languages such as Hungarian or Turkish make use of a rich morphological inventory for this and other uses. The implication of linguistic encoding differences is twofold: first, the difficulty of WPs is language-specific, thus linguistic manipulation leading to increased WP complexity in one language may not have an effect in another, more complex language. Second, the performance of language learners on WPs presented in a foreign language may be affected by the differences between the learner’s mother tongue and the language of the problem presentation. In the following two sections, we briefly summarize the main findings on aspects of linguistic complexity that affect performance.
Structural Factors
Studies on the relation between linguistic structure and student performance on WPs have considered complexity at the micro-level of word and sentence forms as well as at the macro-level of the discourse structure of the WP passage. Early approaches addressed structural complexity in terms of basic quantitative properties of the WP text, such as the number of letters, words, sentences, mean word, and sentence length, or the proportion of complex (long) words (Searle et al., 1974; Nesher, 1976; Lepik, 1990). More linguistically motivated variables have been investigated in the context of comprehension difficulties in WPs for language learners, for the most part learners of English. At the vocabulary level, comprehension difficulties which result in problem solving difficulties for English language learners may stem from the presence of unfamiliar (low-frequency) words, polysemous words, idiomatic or culturally specific lexical references. At the sentence structure level, factors that have been shown to play a role include noun phrase length, the number of prepositional phrases and participial modifiers, the presence of passive voice and complex clause structure such as relative, subordinate, complement, adverbial, or conditional clauses (Spanos et al., 1988; Abedi et al., 1997; Abedi and Lord, 2001; Shaftel et al., 2006; Thevenot et al., 2007; Martiniello, 2008).
At the discourse structure level, specifically in terms of discourse ordering, the correspondence between the order in which numerical data is presented in the WP and the order in which it can be used to solve it has been shown to be a major predictive variable. Order-consistent problems result in better performance (Searle et al., 1974). Better performance has also been observed for simpler question wording or placing the question before the text results (Cummins et al., 1988).
Semantic Factors
A single factor that is straightforwardly related to WP difficulty and that has been widely investigated is the presence or absence of explicit verbal cues whose semantics hint at the expected operation and thus directly lead toward the solution. Verbal cues include words and phrases of different categories: conjunctions (“and” for addition), adverbs (“left,” “more than,” “less than” for subtraction), or determiners (“each” for multiplication). Eye tracking studies have shown that subjects tend to focus on linguistic verbal cues and perform translation directly to the mathematical operation (e.g., Hegarty et al., 1992; van der Schoot et al., 2009).
Because verbal cues so often lead to default mathematical interpretation (Nesher, 1976), even small differences in phrasing in cue words can cause significant changes in performance (LeBlanc and Weber-Russell, 1996). This is especially relevant for young children (Lean et al., 1990), who in the course of development connect words such as “join,” “add,” “get,” “find,” or “take away” with concepts such as putting together, separating, giving away, or losing. A problem can thus be reworded by adding verbal clues which make the semantic relations more salient so that the underlying mathematical relation is more explicit. For example, the WP “There are five marbles. Two of them belong to Mary. How many belong to John?” can be reworded as “There are five marbles. Two of them belong to Mary. The rest belong to John. How many belong to John?” [from Cummins (1991)]. This kind of conceptual rewording has been shown to be useful to improve children’s performance on WPs (Vicente et al., 2007). Thus changes in wording can influence representation (De Corte et al., 1985).
Semantic or object relations between the objects described in the problem also relate to difficulty. Division problems usually involve functionally related objects (e.g., tulips–vases) and rarely categorically related objects (e.g., tulips–daisies; ?). By contrast, addition for the most part involves categorically related objects. The correlation between object relations and mathematical operations has been argued to reflect a structural correspondence between semantic and mathematical relations (Bassok et al., 1998). For this reason, the semantic structure properties of a WP have been emphasized as a more important factor contributing to difficulty than the syntactic structure (Yeap and Kaur, 2001; ?). Interestingly, an effect related to information load has been observed; the presence of content irrelevant to the core solution, i.e., the presence of numerical or linguistic distractors, results in higher error rates (Muth, 1992). De Corte and Verschaffel (1987) found that the semantic structure of WPs influences children’s choice of mathematical solution strategy. In terms of the broader task context, the required or expected way of responding to the WP has a big influence, especially for the domain of multiplication and division with rational numbers as argued in De Corte et al. (1988); for example, whether students are expected to answer the problem numerically or if they only have to indicate the required operation, or whether they respond in an open way or with multiple choice.
Numerical Complexity and Numerical Studies
Arithmetic WPs have to be usually transformed mentally into an arithmetic problem and usually require an arithmetic solution (?). This means transforming word and numbers into the appropriate operation (Neef et al., 2003). Since the arithmetic problem has to be solved in the end, numerical representations and arithmetic processes will also play an important role in the solution process. In numerical cognition, different models and representations have been proposed (e.g., Dehaene and Cohen, 1995; Nuerk et al., 2011). However, the problem here is that the literature on WP often seems (with some exceptions) to be largely in a parallel research universe to the literature on numerical cognition and arithmetic processes, so that standard models of numerical cognition are hard to apply on the existing literature. What is more, WP research on numerical factors is also affected by the scoring criteria; in some studies on WP solving, computational errors are neglected, because in many studies researchers consider a solution as correct as long as the solver has chosen the correct mathematical model (Verschaffel and De Corte, 1990). This is not the case in behavioral numerical cognition research, where the correct result is usually essential and RTs, accuracies, error types, and solution types are analyzed based on the arithmetic problem and result.
Numerical complexity can influence WP performance via at least three routes (see Figure 1):
FIGURE 1. This figure describes a possible theoretical process model of world problem solving based on this article and dissociating numerical and linguistic factors: Three general aspects are distinguished for predicting individual WP performance. Stimulus Attributes (WP difficulty), individual attributes (capabilities), and environmental factors (e.g., teaching). WP difficulty comprises linguistic factors (such as linguistic complexity of the WP text, Section 2 of this article), numerical factors (such as numerical difficulty of the numerical problem, Section 3), and their interaction (such as the relation between text and arithmetic problem, Section 4). Individual Capabilities can refer to linguistic and numerical capabilities and domain- general abilities such as individual working memory capacity. Stimulus attributes and individual attributes influence individual WP performance both directly and over two mediator variables. One mediator variable refers to domain-general attributes, such as cognitive load. Complex linguistic and numerical stimulus attributes can increase cognitive load and the impact of increased complexity may be overadditive, especially when the joint linguistic and numerical complexity exceeds the cognitive load of an individual. On the other hand, those domain- general attributes are influenced by individual capability. Cognitive load for an individual with high linguistic or numerical abilities may be lower for the same problem than for an individual with low linguistic or numerical abilities. The second mediator variable refers to specific solution strategies. If specific solution strategies can be applied to a particular WP problem, because the problem type allows this and because the individual knows the strategy, solution strategies can facilitate WP solving. Finally, environmental factors (e.g.: teaching, scoring system… etc.) influence individual capabilities, solution strategies, and also directly individual WP Performance.
-
Direct route: WPs with more complex arithmetic structure are more difficult independent of linguistic complexity.
-
Cognitive load: more complex arithmetic problems involve a higher cognitive load. For instance, carry problems are supposed to require more working memory resources. If the linguistic properties are also complex and the built-up of a mental model also requires more working memory resources, high arithmetic, and linguistic complexities could lead to over additive difficulties which could neither be explained by main effects of linguistic or numerical difficulty.
-
Solution strategies: multi-digit numbers are harder to process than single-digit numbers (Nuerk et al., 2011; for a review) and arithmetic complexity usually increases with numbers of digits. Thevenot and Oakhill (2005) compared the influence of processing three-digit numbers and two-digit numbers on WP solution strategies. They showed that processing numerically more complex three-digit numbers facilitated alternative strategies by the participants. The authors suggested that higher work load and working memory led to this facilitation. For our review and the model in the revised manuscript, the important point is that they resort to less effortful strategies. Similar results were observed by Brissiaud and Sander (2010), who manipulated the size and order of the numbers and operation thus resulting in two different solution strategies: (i) situation-strategy WPs that are easy to solve with informal strategies, e.g., double-counting, derived number fact, or trial-error strategy, (ii) mental arithmetic-strategy WPs are “easy to solve with mental calculation, but only when the relevant arithmetic knowledge is used.” Number magnitude and order determined which strategy was used most likely. In this review, we suggest that resorting to alternative easier strategies is not restricted to number magnitude, but could be used with any numerical variable that allows simpler solutions. For instance, if a number bisection task were used in a text problem, we would also suggest that participants resort to easier strategies (e.g., checking the parities of the outer number), when the bisection problem gets more complex (e.g., larger interval, decade crossing etc.).
Nevertheless, some distinctions of numerical processes can be made in our review of the WP literature and are therefore proposed as an initial step in this review. Note that in our view this is not the end of the integration of numerical cognitive research and WP research, but rather just a beginning. For an overview of the investigation of specific numerical processes in current WP research, we suggest categorizing them into five categories:
- the property of numbers (parity, single digit/multi digit, problem size, ties, type of number, role of the number, number magnitude),
- required operation (type, number)
- mathematical solution strategies (larger number, place, automatic fact retrieval, position of the unknown),
- relevance of the information.
- other numerical processes and representations
Number Properties
While some studies have shown an effect of numerical complexity, from a numerical cognition view it is surprising that actually the arithmetic complexity has rarely been systematically considered as an isolated factor in WPs, although it is frequently examined in other arithmetic problems or simply the description of numbers is missing, e.g., De Corte et al. (1990). For instance, parity attributes are rarely considered in WPs, although in children it influences task performance and strategy choice in arithmetic tasks. For instance, in the number bisection task (Is the middle number Y the exact mean of X and Z in X_Y_Z?), parity influences performance. Trials with unequal parities of X and Z are easier to solve than trials with equal parities (Nuerk et al., 2002). We suggested that this is due to a change in strategy. In trials with unequal parity (e.g., 21_25_28), it is impossible that the middle number is the mean, because the mean of numbers with unequal parity is not an integer number (and only integers were used in the experiment). Therefore, participants may change their strategy after they discovered unequal parities and may not compute further to find out whether the middle number is really the mean. A later fMRI study (Wood et al., 2008) corroborated this assumption. In the easier unequal parity (“impossible”) condition, we observed more activation in the right ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, which is activated in cognitive set changes or when participants generate alternative solutions for a task. Thus, parity can influence performance and solution strategies in arithmetic. This seems not only the case in the bisection task, which is to our knowledge rarely used in WP research, but also in standard operations like addition and subtraction. A review by Hines (2013) suggests that parity influences the difficulty of addition and subtraction, but not multiplication, and tasks containing odd numbers are more difficult than with even ones. Such parity effects have received little attention in WP research so far. Furthermore, it seems that most WPs, especially for children, contain single-digit numbers; e.g., each answer was in the range of 1–9, e.g., in Lean et al. (1990), or Powell and Fuchs (2014), only few use multi-digit numbers (Haghverdi et al., 2012). In Nesher (1976) the range of numbers is smaller than 100, contained division two-digit numbers into one-digit number.
Explanations why the studies have chosen specific numbers, e.g., mentioning problem size, are rare. De Corte et al. (1990) and Orrantia et al. (2010) controlled for the number of sentences; the size of the numbers given in the problems. In the study of van der Schoot et al. (2009) the final answers were between 14 and 40, included no fraction, no negative number, no numerical value twice, and none of the possible answers resulted in another. However, different types of numbers were presented in WPs in some more studies: (i) fraction (Raduan, 2010), (ii) whole number, (iii) decimal number (Haghverdi et al., 2012); and their effect has been rarely investigated. Koedinger and Nathan (2004) found an effect for decimal numbers: “however we also observed a smaller situation facilitation effect whereby story performance was better than word equation performance under certain conditions: namely dealing with decimal numbers.”
The mixed use of single- and multi-digit numbers is problematic because in the last 15 years, numerous numerical cognition studies have shown that single-digit number processing cannot easily be generalized to multi-digit number processing, e.g., Nuerk et al. (2001); for reviews see Nuerk and Willmes (2005) and Nuerk et al. (2015). Nuerk et al. (2015) have identified 17 numerical effects linked to different numerical representation, which are specific for multi-digit number processing and which cannot be explained by single-digit number representations. Also even the same effects are different for single- and multi-digit numbers. For instance, Ashkenazi et al. (2009) have shown that the distance effect for two-digit numbers differentiates between dyscalculic and typically developing children. The sometimes seemingly arbitrary mix of single-digit and multi-digit number use in WP research is therefore not reasonable in our view given the state of numerical cognition research and the major differences between processing those different number types. The role of a number within an operation also influences WP complexity (De Corte et al., 1988). For example, in the case of addition the role means: addend, minuend or by multiplication: multiplicand, multiplier. One important finding from recent research on multiplication WPs is that children’s performances are strongly affected by the nature of the multiplier whether, e.g., it is an integer, decimal larger than 1 or a decimal smaller than 1. On the other hand, the size of the multiplicand has little or no effect on problem difficulty. De Corte et al. (1988) stated that “two multiplication problems with the same mathematical, semantic, and surface structure but different in terms of the nature of the given numbers can elicit very distinct levels of problems difficulty.” Indeed, this corresponds to recent findings that relatedness and consistency heavily influence the ease with which a multiplication problem can be solved cf. for relatedness (Domahs et al., 2006, 2007) and for consistency Verguts and Fias (2005).
Despite the major role of number properties in numerical cognition, number property has not been investigated extensively in the WPs (Fuchs et al., 2009). Nevertheless, numbers seem to play a major role. For instance, De Corte and Verschaffel (1986) observed that in their eye tracking study there was a relatively strong focus on the numbers in the problem. Twenty-five percent of the total solution time was spent in the two small number areas. However, major number properties of numerical cognitions research such as number magnitude are rarely systematically considered in WP research. In our view, more dialog between fields, – numerical cognition and WP research – seems necessary.
Required Operation
Carrying out operations are necessary steps in solving arithmetic WPs. Operations have been used extensively in WPs. Most errors seem to originate from people’s failure to understand the language of WPs, i.e., the linguistic embedding of the calculation problem (Schumacher and Fuchs, 2012), and arithmetic computation errors themselves (Raduan, 2010; Kingsdorf and Krawec, 2014). Some errors may result from correct calculation performed on incorrect problem representation (Lewis and Mayer, 1987) and different operations may lead to different solution strategies. The most usual operation used in WP experiments are addition and subtraction (Carpenter et al., 1984; De Corte et al., 1988; Schumacher and Fuchs, 2012). Even the classification of Riley et al. (1983) was made for elementary addition and subtraction. Research in the 1980s and 1990s concentrated on how children learn to do one step addition and subtraction problems involving small whole numbers; see the review from Vicente et al. (2007). Later, the focus was more on the multiplication WPs or mixed WPs – e.g., Swanson (2004). Greer (1992) presented a framework categorization of multiplication and division WPs on the basis of the types of quantities involved (positive integers, fraction, and decimals) as models of situation. The semantic problem structure also influences the solution strategies for addition and subtraction.
Choosing the correct operation strongly depends on the type of the given numbers in the problem (De Corte et al., 1990). As already shortly outlined in above subsection on problem types, there is a huge body of research on what makes addition, subtraction, or multiplication problems difficult. Carry operations (e.g., 28 + 47; the decade value 1 from the unit sum 15 has to be carried over to the decade sum) have long been known to make multi-digit addition more difficult in children and adults; see Nuerk et al. (2015) for a review. However, solution strategies differ between children and adults – eye movement data suggest that in a choice reaction task elementary school children always compute and search for the correct results, while adults seem to also decide based on the rejection of the incorrect result. What is more, even within the carry operations at least three different cognitive processes can be identified for adults: unit sum calculation, carry detection, and carry execution (Moeller et al., 2011). Inability to execute one of these processes may lead to worse performance in carry problems in particular. Carry addition problems seem to require larger working memory resources (Ashcraft, 1995; Furst and Hitch, 2000). If cognitive load/working memory demand is high, because both the linguistic and the numerical complexity of the WP are large, this may lead to over additive problems in the domain-general processing stages involved in WP solving — see Figure 1, for an elaboration. For multiplication, we know that relatedness, ties, whether a problem stems from the 0, 1, 2, 5, or 10 row (Josta et al., 2009), or consistency influence the difficulty of a multiplication problem (Domahs et al., 2006, 2007). Although such factors have been extensively studied in numerical cognition research, they are – to the best of our knowledge – rarely considered in WP research. Since we know that these factors make the arithmetic computation, which is part of the WP solution, this lack of consideration is again problematic in our view.
Mathematical Solution Strategies
Mathematical solution strategy variations have been studied extensively, and can be a function of linguistic factors like wording, semantic categories and propositions. However, how individuals come up with mathematical solution strategies can be also be influenced by numerical factors like number magnitude (Thevenot and Oakhill, 2005). Such variables, which are independent of other factors, make WPs harder and/or influence numerical representations, have rarely been studied. The position/place of the unknown variable has an effect on representation (Garcia et al., 2006). Even studies about working memory also investigated the position of the unknown variable (Swanson, 2004). The strategy of counting on from larger is easier if the bigger number is represented first (Wilkins et al., 2001). Even for adults: 4 + 2 = 6, and 2 + 4 = 6, which are mathematically equivalent, may psychologically imply different meanings (Kaput, 1979). The sequence of the numbers, e.g., whether a problem starts with the smaller or with the larger number (Verschaffel and De Corte, 1990), the position of the numbers and particular words (Schumacher and Fuchs, 2012) influence children’s solution of elementary addition and subtraction problems. For example, in change problems children typically look for a specific number to begin with, depending on task features, like the first mentioned number (Lean et al., 1990; Wilkins et al., 2001), the type of problem (start or change set), and the size of the numbers (Verschaffel and De Corte, 1990).
Arithmetic fact retrieval is a well researched ubiquitous strategy in numerical cognition but less so in the domain of WPs. Orrantia et al. (2010) found that arithmetic fact retrieval is not limited to simple addition, but also possible in other tasks, such as single-digit arithmetic WPs. Fuchs et al. (2009) investigated so called “Number combination.” This means simple arithmetic problems that can be solved via counting or decomposition strategies or committed to long term memory for automatic retrieval. Here, arithmetic fact retrieval had to be differentiated from other strategies on three levels: operational, items difficulty, and individual differences. These numerical factors influence solution strategies in arithmetic and WPs as well. Decomposition and counting require more working memory and therefore leave less resources for the built-up and maintenance of a text situations model. However, both individual and stimulus differences should also be considered. For instance, Grabner et al. (2009) showed in an fMRI study that not only problem but also individual strategy choice contributed to fact retrieval processes when solving multiplications.
Information Relevance and Step-Wise Problem Processing
One relatively extensively studied factor in WPs is the relevance of the information. Individuals have to extract the relevant information from the text in order to carry out the correct solution. Secondary information distracts people from recognizing the underlying mathematical relations (Schley and Fujita, 2014). This extra information may also be presented in the form of an extra number or an extra operational step – one-step (i.e., one calculation step has to be performed) and two-step problems (i.e., two calculation steps have to be performed). Problem complexity increases with the addition of steps (Terao et al., 2004), as well as the addition of irrelevant information to the problem (Kingsdorf and Krawec, 2014) Presence of extraneous information and the need for an extra step reduced the accuracy of the students’ solutions, because students believe that all of the numbers in a WP should be used. All other factors being kept constant, two-step problems are much more error-prone than one-step problems (Muth, 1992). However, it cannot be concluded that the reason for two-step problems being more difficult is arithmetic complexity, because in two-step problems, the WP has also become more difficult linguistically as it usually contains more phrases and semantic distractors.
Other Numerical Processes and Representation
Several other numerical processes and representations have not been investigated in WPs. For instance, as shortly outlined above, one major factor in simple calculation problems, which can be studied in isolation, is the presence or absence of a carry operation. Children and adults take longer and commit more errors when computing the solution to a sum for which adding the units leads to a change in the number of 10s (e.g., 14 + 9 = 23; Furst and Hitch, 2000; Deschuyteneer et al., 2005) than when it does not (e.g., 11 + 12 = 23). This effect is known as the carry effect; in carry problems, a one needs to be carried from the unit slot to the decade slot. The carry effect is influenced by various processes, but even by language structure (Goebel et al., 2014). Language influences on the difficulty of the numerical computations within a WP have to our knowledge not been studied. Other central topics of numerical cognition such as, e.g., number and symbol sense contribute to WP solving are also open questions (MacGregor and Price, 1999). We have chosen some selected variables/factors, which have been investigated in the WP research.
Connecting Linguistic and Mathematical Factors
There are so many linguistic influences on numerical cognition and arithmetic that this justifies a special issue like this. For instance, number word structure seems to play an essential role. Children growing up with regular number word structure usually perform better in variety of numerical tasks from basic verbal counting up to arithmetic, e.g., Miller et al. (1995) or Dowker et al. (2008). In addition, the consistency of the order of the number word system and the Arabic number influences transcoding (Zuber et al., 2009; Pixner et al., 2011a; Imbo et al., 2014) number comparison (Nuerk et al., 2005; Pixner et al., 2011b; Klein et al., 2013; Moeller et al., 2014) calculation (Goebel et al., 2014); see also (Brysbaert et al., 1998; Colomé et al., 2010). In addition, reading direction influences numerical processes like the SNARC effect (Shaki et al., 2009; Fischer and Shaki, 2014); see Goebel et al. (2011) for reviews. Finally, grammatical and syntactic properties of elementary number words influence early number acquisition (Sarnecka, 2013) and spatial-numerical representations (Roettger and Domahs, 2015). The linguistic influence on numerical cognition is hardly debatable any more. In fact, Lachmair et al. (2014) argue for a connection of language and words, O´Neill (2013) states that the link between language and mathematics might originate from the same roots, and “required abilities are not that split up as we think,” and MacGregor and Price (1999) also argue that between language and mathematics in WPs there is deep connection: “that the cognitive ability that drives symbol processing is the connection between language and maths.” Nevertheless, systematic variation of both linguistic and numerical factors in WPs is scarce – though Bassok et al. (1998) already found that semantic relations between objects in the text of mathematical WPs were highly positively correlated with arithmetic operations that took these objects as arguments. Neural correlates of visualization and verbalization during arithmetic WP study also suggest that mental arithmetic in WPs is influenced by language processing (Zarnhofer et al., 2013).
Word problems require some connection between linguistic and mathematical understanding by the very nature of the task, because at least children do not have a repertoire of “highly automatized schemata” for representing the different problem types (Garcia et al., 2006). Therefore, it is not surprising that children make more errors when solving WPs compared to number problems (Geary, 1996; Koedinger and Nathan, 2004). Children are able to solve several types of addition and subtraction problems before they start formal schooling (De Corte and Verschaffel, 1987; Lean et al., 1990), and understand numerical concepts before seeing WPs in their curricula (Garcia et al., 2006). Therefore, most studies implicitly assume that problem solvers always have the necessary basic arithmetic skills, even in the case of children. This may lead to the misconception that numbers may play a lesser role than they actually do and factors other than computational skills are a major source of difficulty with WPs (Nesher, 1976; Reusser, 1993). In this aspect, it is also important to note that difficulties in solving WPs have been reported that could be neither attributed to the lack of general reading comprehension skills nor to the lack of general mathematical skills (Hegarty et al., 1995). Nevertheless, linguistics and numerical factors are usually not independently manipulated in WPs and not even dissociated by other means (e.g., regressions). What is more, their interaction is rarely studied [for an exception, see Verschaffel and De Corte (1990)].
Lexical Consistency Effect
One of the few frequently studied factors examining the relation between text and arithmetic problems is lexical inconsistency. Some WPs contain linguistic markers as “less” or “more.” In the direct translation strategy (Hegarty et al., 1995) students simply associate “less” with subtraction and “more” with addition. They search for linguistic markers and keywords. In the problem model strategy, they construct a mental model of the problem and plan their solution on the basis of this model. Successful learners are more likely to employ the problem model strategy; they focus more on variables names and relational terms and successful problem solvers re-read the text less frequently (Pape, 2003) in the eye-tracking studies. Unsuccessful learners, on the other hand, seem to rely on the direct translation strategy; they focus on numerals and on relational terms, and linguistics markedness in the (Hegarty et al., 1992) eyetracking study. This leads to wrong solutions in lexically inconsistent texts, where “more” is associated with subtraction and “less” with addition. To give an example for lexical inconsistency, consider the following WP adapted from Boonen et al. (2013) “At the grocery store, a bottle of olive oil costs 7 €. That is 2 € more than at the supermarket. How much will [a bottle of olive oil] cost in the supermarket?” The anticipated difficulty in comprehension and finding the correct solution is due to the fact that the adverb “more” evokes the concept of addition, but the correct solution is not 7 + 2 but 7 — 2, given the way the text is organized. Verschaffel et al. (1992) found such a reaction time consistency effect for children but not for adults. Nesher (1976) and Lean et al. (1990) obtained similar results in experiments with groups of non-disadvantaged children and students, showing that linguistic semantic consistency with respect to the required mathematical operation is an important determinant of task difficulty. Inconsistent language results in a high error rate and longer response time (Hegarty et al., 1992), even in Verschaffel (1994) retelling one-step compared WPs showed a strong evidence for the consistency hypothesis. Students made ∼13% more reversal errors on inconsistent than on consistent language problems and the difficulty of comprehending inconsistent-language problems were increased when the correct arithmetic operation was an increase. However, the literature is inconsistent if the consistency effect is present in both students and children. Children find it easier to convert the relation term “more than” into subtraction operation than the relational term “less than” into an addition operation (Lewis and Mayer, 1987; Verschaffel et al., 1992; Pape, 2003; van der Schoot et al., 2009).
When neither reading comprehension nor arithmetic skills alone can explain failure to solve WPs, a possible explanation is that linguistic complexity and numerical complexity rely on the same resources (e.g., working memory). The premise is that there is not an absolute atomic concept of difficulty for WPs. Rather; there are multiple linguistic and numerical factors which contribute to a problem’s complexity. It is a combination of these factors that might make a problem additively more or less difficult because they exert demands on more general resources like working memory. Generally, problem solving performance is related to the ability of reducing the accessibility of no target and irrelevant information in the memory (Passolunghi and Siegel, 2001). Working memory contributes to early arithmetic performance, and studies also show that this extends to WP solving (Lee et al., 2004) due to semantic memory representation “less than” which is more complex than “more than.” Changes in the structure of the text has more demand on the working. It has been suggested that WPs in general are related to working memory (Swanson et al., 1993). This will probably also be influenced by instruction specifying how participants have to solve a WP, and the method of evaluation, and scoring system. In Van Dijk and Kintsch’s (1983) model of reading comprehension, working memory is used to keep a number of text propositions active simultaneously. In particular, working memory has been related to each single component mentioned above, such as text-problem relation, the linguistic complexity, and the arithmetic complexity.
Future Direction, Open Questions
Word problem difficulty is influenced by the complexity of linguistic factors, numerical factors, and their interrelation. To better understand the difficulty of WPs, it would be desirable to manipulate such variables and their interaction following the principle of isolated variation. To support a systematic investigation, the variables to be manipulated also need to be discussed against the backdrop of the relevant conceptual and empirical issues in the underlying fields, linguistics, and numerical cognition. This has too rarely been the case in the past. For instance, in the earlier studies on algebra WPs, the linguistic cues are of mixed categories (adverbs, verbs, nouns, etc.) and the effect of the complexity of syntactic structures is not taken into account. Similarly, numerical complexity like basic number properties (e.g., magnitude, place-value processing for multi-digit numbers) or the complexity of underlying arithmetic computations (e.g., carry effects for addition, relatedness, or consistency effects for multiplication) are often neglected. WP research would be well advised to take into account the foundational categories, properties and findings of both numerical cognition and linguistics when it examines which WPs are difficult for which groups and why. Not only the main effects of numerical and linguistic complexity should be studied, but also their interaction. To make the relevant aspects explicit, Figure 1 sketches an overall process model of WP solving.
The joint investigation of linguistic and numerical processes also needs to take into account joint moderator variables such as working memory in order to explore the possible interactions between them. Since working memory affects all components of complexity of a WP, the difficulties triggered may not be simply additive, but also interactive. The resolution of linguistic and numerical difficulties may rely on the same processing stages and resources (Sternberg, 1969). To investigate this, more collaboration between linguists and numerical cognition researchers would be desirable.
Finally, we suggest a differential-psychological approach to WP research. Different students may have a problem with different types of WPs. Linguistically rather weak students may have problems with linguistically complex WPs, and arithmetically rather weak students with arithmetically complex problems. Undifferentiated presentation of WPs in experiments will not provide sufficient information about which skills and processes an individual child should practice. Only with such differentiation on an item level (as regards linguistic and numerical complexity and their interrelation) and on an individual level (as regards linguistic and numerical skills and general cognitive abilities) will it be possible to understand why a particular child has its individual difficulties with particular WP types. Such an understanding, however, is essential to promote tailored learning of one of the most difficult arithmetic problem types that students encounter in school.
Conflict of Interest Statement
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
This research was funded by the LEAD Graduate School [GSC1028], a project of the Excellence Initiative of the German federal and state governments. MW is a Junior Research Group Leader of the LEAD Graduate School and her work is supported by the Institutional Strategy of the University of Tübingen (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft, ZUK 63). GD is a doctoral student of the LEAD Graduate School. We acknowledge support by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft and the Open Access Publishing Fund of the University of Tübingen.
References
Abedi, J., and Lord, C. (2001). The language factor in mathematics tests. Appl. Meas. Educ. 14, 219–234. doi: 10.1207/S15324818AME1403_2
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Abedi, J., Lord, C., and Plummer, J. (1997). Final Report of Language Background as a Variable in NAEP Mathematics Performance. Tech. Rep. No. 429. Los Angeles, CA: University of California, Center for the Study of Evaluation/National Center for Research on Evaluation, Standards, and Student Testing.
Google Scholar
Ashcraft, M. H. (1995). Cognitive psychology and simple arithmetic: a review and summery of new directions. Math. Cogn. 1, 3–34.
Google Scholar
Berends, I. E., and van Lieshout, E. C. D. M. (2009). The effect of illustrations in arithmetic problem-solving: effects of increased cognitive load. Learn. Instr. 19, 345–353. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2008.06.012
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Boonen, A. J. H., Van Der Schoot, M., Van Wesel, F., De Vries, M. H., and Jolles, J. (2013). What underlies successful word problem solving? A path analysis in sixth grade students. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 38, 271–279. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2013.05.001
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Boonen, A. J. H., Van Wesel, F., Jolles, J., and Van Der Schoot, M. (2014). The role of visual representation type, spatial ability, and reading comprehension in word problem solving: an item-level analysis in elementary school children. Int. J. Educ. Res. 68, 15–26. doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2014.08.0010883-0355
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Brysbaert, M., Fias, W., and Noël, M. P. (1998). The Whorfian hypothesis and numerical cognition: is “twenty-four” processed in the same way as “four-and-twenty”? Cognition 66, 51–77. doi: 10.1016/S0010-0277(98)00006-7
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Bulté, B., and Housen, A. (2012). “Defining and operationalising L2 complexity,” in Dimensions of L2 Performance and Proficiency – Investigating Complexity, Accuracy and Fluency in SLA, eds A. Housen, F. Kuiken, and I. Vedder. (Amsterdam: John Benjamins), 21–46. doi: 10.1075/lllt.32.02bul
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Carpenter, T. P., Hieben, J., and Moser, J. (1981). The effect of problem structure on first-grader’s initial solution processes for simple addition and subtraction problems. J. Res. Math. Educ. 2, 27–39. doi: 10.2307/748656
CrossRef Full Text
Carpenter, T. P., Moser, T. P., and James, M. (1984). The acquisition of addition and subtraction concepts in grades one through three. J. Res. Math. Educ. 15, 172–202. doi: 10.2307/748348
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Csikos, C., Kelemen, R., and Verschaffel, L. (2011). Fifth-grade students’ approaches to and beliefs of mathematics word problem solving: a large sample Hungarian study. ZDM 43, 561–571. doi: 10.1007/s11858-011-0308-7
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Cummins, D. D. (1991). Children’s interpretations of arithmetic word problems. Cogn. Instr. 8, 261–289. doi: 10.1207/s1532690xci0803_2
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Cummins, D. D., Kintsch, W., Reusswer, K., and Weimer, R. (1988). The role of understanding in solving word problems. Cogn. Psychol. 20, 405–438. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(88)90011-4
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
De Corte, E., and Verschaffel, L. (1986). Eye-movement data as access to solution processes of elementary addition and subtraction problems. Paper Presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco.
Google Scholar
De Corte, E., and Verschaffel, L. (1987). The effect of semantic structure on first graders’ strategies for solving addition and subtraction word problems. J. Res. Math. Educ. 18, 363–381. doi: 10.2307/749085
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
De Corte, E., Verschaffel, L., and De Win, L. (1985). Influence of rewording verbal problems on children’s problem representations and solutions. J. Educ. Psychol. 77, 460–470. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.77.4.460
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
De Corte, E., Verschaffel, L., and Pauwels, A. (1990). Influence of the semantic structure of word problems on second graders eye movements. J. Educ. Psychol. 82, 359–365. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.82.2.359
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
De Corte, E., Verschaffel, L., and Van Coillie, V. (1988). Influence of number size, problem structure, and response mode on children’s solutions of multiplication word problems. J. Math. Behav. 7, 197–216.
Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., and Cohen, L. (1995). Towards an anatomical and functional model of number processing. Math. Cogn. 1, 83–120.
Google Scholar
Dellarosa, D. (1986). A computer simulation of children’s arithmetic word-problem solving. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 18, 147–154. doi: 10.3758/BF03201014
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Deschuyteneer, M., De Rammelaere, S., and Fias, W. (2005). The addition of two-digit numbers: exploring carry versus no-carry problems. Psychol. Sci. 47, 74–83.
Google Scholar
Domahs, F., Delazer, M., and Nuerk, H.-C. (2006). What makes multiplication facts difficult: problem size or neighbourhood consistency? Exp. Psychol. 53, 275–282. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.53.4.275
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Domahs, F., Janssen, U., Schlesewsky, M., Ratinckx, E., Verguts, T., Willmes, K., et al. (2007). Neighborhood consistency in mental arithmetic: behavioral and ERP evidence. Behav. Brain Funct. 3, 66. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-3-66
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Dowker, A., Bala, S., and Lloyd, D. (2008). Linguistic influences on mathematical development: how important is the transparency of the counting system. Philos. Psychol. 21, 523–538. doi: 10.1080/09515080802285511
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Ellis, R. (2003). Task-based Language Learning and Teaching. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Google Scholar
Fite, G. (2002). Reading and math: what is the connection? A short review of the literature. Kansas Science Teacher 14, 7–11.
Google Scholar
Fuchs, L. S., Powell, S. R., Seethaler, P. M., Cirino, P. T., Fletcher, J. M., Fuchs, D., et al. (2009). Remediating number combination and word problem deficits among students with mathematics difficulties: a randomized control trial. J. Educ. Psychol. 101, 561–576. doi: 10.1037/a0014701
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Garcia, A., Jimenez, L., and Hess, S. (2006). Solving arithmetic word problems: an analysis of classification as a function of difficulty in children with and without arithmetic LD. J. Learn. Disabil. 39, 270–281. doi: 10.1177/00222194060390030601
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Gasco, J., and Villarroel, J. D. (2014). The motivation of secondary school students in mathematical word problem solving. Electron. J. Res. Educ. Psychol. 12, 83–106. doi: 10.14204/ejrep.32.13076
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Geary, D. C. (1996). Children’s Mathematical Development: Research and Practical Applications. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Google Scholar
Goebel, S., Moeller, K., Pixner, S., Kaufmann, L., and Nuerk, H.-C. (2014). Language affects symbolic arithmetic in children: the case of number word inversion. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 119, 17–25. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2013.10.001
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Goebel, S. M., Shaki, S., and Fischer, M. H. (2011). The cultural number line: a review of cultural and linguistic influences on the development of number processing. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 42, 543–565. doi: 10.1177/0022022111406251
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Grabner, R. H., Ansari, D., Koschutnig, K., Reishofer, G., Ebner, F., and Neuper, C. (2009). To retrieve or to calculate? Left angular gyrus mediates the retrieval of arithmetic facts during problem solving. Neuropsychologia 47, 604–608. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.013
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Greer, B. (1992). “Multiplication and division as models of situations,” in HandBook of Research on Mathematics Teaching and Learning, ed. D. A. Grouws (New York, NY: Macmillan Publishing Company), 276–295.
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | Google Scholar
Haghverdi, M., Semnani, A. S., and Seifi, M. (2012). The relationship between different kinds of students’ errors and the knowledge required to solve mathematics word problems. Bolema Bol. Educ. Matem. 26, 649–666. doi: 10.1590/S0103-636X2012000200012
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hegarty, M., Mayer, R. E., and Green, C. E. (1992). Comprehension of arithmetic word problems: evidence from students’ eye fixations. J. Educ. Psychol. 84, 76–84. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.84.1.76
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hegarty, M., Mayer, R. E., and Monk, C. A. (1995). Comprehension of arithmetic word problems: a comparison of successful and unsuccessful problem solvers. J. Educ. Psychol. 87, 18–32. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.87.1.18
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Hinsley, D. A., Hayes, J. R., and Simon, H. A. (1977). “From words to equations: meaning and representation in algebra word problems,” in Cognition Processes in Comprehension, ed. P. S. Carpenter (Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum), 89–100.
Google Scholar
Imbo, I., Bulcke, C. V., De Brauwer, J., and Fias, W. (2014). Sixty-four or four-and-sixty? The influence of language and working memory on children’s number transcoding. Front. Psychol. 5:313. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00313
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Jimenez, L., and Verschaffel, L. (2014). Development of children’s solutions of non-standard arithmetic word problem solving non-standard arithmetic word problems. Rev. Psicodidactica 19, 93–123. doi: 10.1387/RevPsicodidact.7865
CrossRef Full Text
Josta, K., Khadera, P., Burkea, M., Bienb, S., and Röslera, F. (2009). Dissociating the solution processes of small, large, and zero multiplications by means of fMRI. Neuroimage 46, 308–318. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.044
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Kaput, J. J. (1979). “Mathematics and learning: roots of epistemological status,” in Cognitive Process Instruction, eds J. Clement and J. Lochhead (Philadephia, PA: Franklin Institute Press), 289–303.
Google Scholar
Kieran, C. (1990). “Cognitive processes involved in learning school algebra,” in Mathematics and Cognition, eds P. Nesher and J. Kilpatrick (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 96–112.
Google Scholar
Kintsch, W., and Greeno, J. G. (1985). Understanding and solving word arithmetic problems. Psychol. Rev. 92, 109–129. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.92.1.109
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Klein, E., Moeller, K., Kettl, D., Zauner, H., Wood, G., Willmes, K., et al. (2013). Object-based neglect in number processing. Behav. Brain Funct. 9, 5. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-9-5
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Koedinger, K. R., and Nathan, M. J. (2004). The real story behind story problems: effects of representations on quantitative reasoning. J. Learn. Sci. 13, 129–164. doi: 10.1207/s15327809jls1302_1
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lachmair, M., Dudschig, C., De La Vega, L., and Kaup, B. (2014). Relating numeric cognition and language processing: do numbers and words share a common representational platform? Acta Psychol. 148, 107–114. doi: 10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.12.004
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lean, G. A., Clemens, M. A., and Del Campo, G. (1990). Linguistic and pedagogical factors affecting children’s understanding of arithmetic word problems: a comparative study. Educ. Stud. Math. 21, 165–191. doi: 10.1007/BF00304900
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
LeBlanc, M. D., and Weber-Russell, S. (1996). Text integration and mathematical connections: a computer model of arithmetic word problem solving. Cogn. Sci. 20, 357–407. doi: 10.1207/s15516709cog2003_2
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lepik, M. (1990). Algebraic word problems: role of linguistic and structural variables. Educ. Stud. Math. 21, 83–90. doi: 10.1007/BF00311017
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Lewis, A. B., and Mayer, R. E. (1987). Students’ miscomprehension of relational statements in arithmetic word problems. J. Educ. Psychol. 79, 363–371. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.79.4.363
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
MacGregor, M., and Price, E. (1999). An exploration of aspects of language proficiency and algebra learnin. J. Res. Math. Educ. 30, 449–467. doi: 10.2307/749709
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Martiniello, M. (2008). Language and the performance of english-language learners in math word problems. Harv. Educ. Rev. 78, 333–368.
Google Scholar
Mayer, R. E. (1981). Frequency norms and structural analysis of algebra story problems into families, categories, and templates. Instr. Sci. 10, 135–175. doi: 10.1007/BF00132515
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Miller, K., Smith, C. M., Zhu, J., and Zhang, H. (1995). Preschool origins of cross-national differences in mathematical competence: the role of number-naming systems. Psychol. Sci. 6, 6–60. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.1995.tb00305.x
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Muth, K. D. (1992). Extraneous information and extra steps in arithmetic word problems. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 17, 278–285. doi: 10.1016/0361-476X(92)90066-90068
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Nathan, M. J., Kintsch, W., and Young, E. (1992). A theory of algebra-word-problem comprehension and its implications for the design of learning environments. Cogn. Instr. 9, 329–389. doi: 10.1207/s1532690xci0904_2
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Nesher, P. (1976). Three determinants of difficulty in verbal arithmetic problems. Educ. Stud. Math. 7, 369–388. doi: 10.1007/BF00452220
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Nesher, P., and Teubal, E. (1975). Verbal cues as an interfering factor in verbal problem solving. Educ. Stud. Math. 6, 41–51. doi: 10.1007/BF00590023
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Nuerk, H.-C., Moeller, K., Klein, E., Willmes, K., and Fischer, M. H. (2011). Extending the mental number line – a review of multi-digit number processing. J. Psychol. 219, 3–22.
Google Scholar
Nuerk, H.-C., Moeller, K., and Willmes, K. (2015). “Multi-digit number processing: overview, conceptual clarifications, and language influences,” in The Oxford Handbook of Numerical Cognition, eds R. C. Kadosh and A. Dowker (Oxford: Oxford University Press). doi: 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199642342.013.021
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Nuerk, H.-C., and Willmes, K. (2005). On the magnitude representation of two-digit numbers. Psychol. Sci. 42, 52–72.
OECD. (2010). PISA 2009 Results. What Students Know and Can Do. Student Performance in Reading, Mathematics and Science, Vol. 1. Paris: OECD Publishing.
Google Scholar
O´Neill, D. K. (2013). Preschool children’s narratives and performance on the peabody individualized achievement test – revised: evidence of a relation between early narrative and later mathematical ability. First Lang. 24, 149–183. doi: 10.1177/0142723704043529
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Oostermeijer, M., Boonen, A. J. H., and Jolles, J. (2014). The relation between children’s constructive play activities, spatial ability, and mathematical word problem-solving performance: a mediation analysis in sixth-grade students. Front. Psychol. 5:782. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00782
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Paige, J. M., and Simon, H. A. (eds). (1966). Cognitive Processes in Solving Algebra Word Problem. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons.
Google Scholar
Pape, S. J. (2003). Compare word problems: consistency hypothesis revisited. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 28, 396–421. doi: 10.1016/S0361-476X(02)00046-2
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Passolunghi, M. C., and Siegel, L. S. (2001). Short-term memory, working memory and inhibitory control in children with difficulties in arithmetic problem solving. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 80, 44–57. doi: 10.1006/jecp.2000.2626
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Pixner, S., Kaufmann, L., Moeller, K., Hermanova, V., and Nuerk, H.-C. (2011a). Whorf reloaded: language effects on non-verbal number processing in 1st grade — a trilingual study. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 108, 371–382. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.09.002
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Pixner, S., Zuber, J., Hermanova, V., Kaufmann, L., and Nuerk, H.-C. (2011b). One language, two number-word systems and many problems: numerical cognition in the Czech language. Res. Dev. Disabil. 32, 2683–2689. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.06.004
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Powell, S. R., and Fuchs, L. S. (2014). Does early algebraic reasoning differ as a function of students’ difficulty with calculations versus word problems? Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 29, 106–116. doi: 10.1111/ldrp.12037
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Raduan, I. H. (2010). Error analysis and the corresponding cognitive activities committed by year five primary students in solving mathematical word problems. Procedia Soc. Behav. Sci. 2, 3836–3838. doi: 10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.03.600
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Reusser, K. (1988). Problem solving beyond the logic of things: contextual effects on understanding and solving word problems. Instr. Sci. 17, 309–338. doi: 10.1007/BF00056219
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Reusser, K. (1993). “Tutoring systems and pedagogical theory: representational tools for understanding, planning and refllection in problem solving,” in Computers as Cognitive Tools, ed. S. L. S. Derry (Hillsdale, NJ Lawrence Erlbaum).
Google Scholar
Riley, M. S., Greeno, J. G., and Heller, J. I. (1983). “Development of children’s problem-solving ability,” in The Development of Mathematical Thinking, ed. H. P. Ginsberg (San Diego, CA: Academic Press), 153–196.
Google Scholar
Schley, D. R., and Fujita, K. (2014). Seeing the math in the story: on how abstraction promotes performance on mathematical word problems. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 5, 953–961. doi: 10.1177/1948550614539519
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Searle, B. W., Lorton, P. Jr., and Suppes, P. (1974). Structural variables affecting CAI performance on arithmetic word problems of disadvantaged and deaf students. Educ. Stud. Math. 5, 371–384. doi: 10.1007/BF01424555
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Shaftel, J., Belton-Kocher, E., Glasnapp, D., and Poggio, J. (2006). The impact of language characteristics in mathematics test items on the performance of english language learners and students with disabilities. Educ. Assess. 11, 105–126. doi: 10.1207/s15326977ea1102_2
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Spanos, G., Rhodes, N. C., and Dale, T. C. (1988). “Linguistic features of mathematical problem solving: insights and applications,” in Linguistic and Cultural Influences on Learning Mathematics, eds R. R. Cocking and J. P. Mestre (Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum), 221–240.
Google Scholar
Stacey, K. (2012). “The international assessment of mathematical literacy: PISA 2012 framework and items,” in Proceedings of the 12th International Congress on Mathematical Education, Seoul.
Google Scholar
Sternberg, S. (1969). The discovery of processing stages: extensions of Donders’ method. Attention and performance II. Acta Psychol. 30, 276–315. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(69)90055-9
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Swanson, H. L. (2004). Working memory and phonological processing as predictors of children’s mathematical problem solving at different ages. Mem. Cognit. 32, 648–661. doi: 10.3758/BF03195856
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Swanson, H. L., Cooney, J. B., and Brock, S. (1993). The influence of working memory and classification ability on childrens word problem solution. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 55, 374–395. doi: 10.1006/jecp.1993.1021
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Swanson, H. L., Lussier, C., and Orosco, M. (2013). Effects of cognitive strategy interventions and cognitive moderators on word problem solving in children at risk for problem solving difficulties. Learn. Disabil. Res. Pract. 28, 170–183. doi: 10.1111/ldrp.12019
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Terao, A., Koedinger, K. R., Sohn, M. H., Qin, Y., Anderson, J. R., and Carter, C. S. (2004). “An fMRI study of the interplay of symbolic and visuo-spatial systems in mathematical reasoning,” in Proceedings of the 26th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Chicago, 1327–1332.
Thevenot, C., Devidal, M., Barrouillet, P., and Fayol, M. (2007). Why does placing the question before an arithmetic word problem improve performance? A situation model account. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 60, 43–56. doi: 10.1080/17470210600587927
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
van der Schoot, M., Bakker Arkema, A. H., Horsley, T. M., and Van Lieshout, E. C. D. M. (2009). The consistency effect depends on markedness in less successful but not successful problem solvers: an eye movement study in primary school children. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 34, 58–66. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2008.07.002
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Van Dijk, T., and Kintsch, W. (1983). Strategies of Discourse Comprehension. New York, NY: Academic Press.
Google Scholar
Van Dooren, W., Verschaffel, L., and Onghena, P. (2002). The impact of pre-service teachers’ content knowledge on their evaluations of students’ strategies for solving arithmetic and algebra word problems. J. Res. Math. Educ. 33, 319–351. doi: 10.2307/4149957
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Verschaffel, L. (1994). Using retelling data to study elementary school children’s representations and solutions of compare problems. J. Res. Math. Educ. 25, 141–165. doi: 10.2307/749506
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Verschaffel, L., and De Corte, E. (1990). “Do non-semantic factors also influence the solution process of addition and subtraction word problems?,” in Learning and Instruction. European Research in an International Context, eds H. Mandl, E. De Corte, N. Bennett, and N. H. H. F. Friedrich (Oxford: Pergamon), 415–429.
Google Scholar
Verschaffel, L., De Corte, E., and Borghart, I. (1997). Pre-Service teachers conceptions and beliefs about the role of real-world knowledge in mathematical modelling of school word problems. Learn. Instr. 7, 339–359. doi: 10.1016/S0959-4752(97)00008-X
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Verschaffel, L., De Corte, E., and Pauwels, A. (1992). Solving compare problems: an eye movement test of Lewis and Mayer’s consistency hypothesis. J. Educ. Psychol. 84, 85–94. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.84.1.85
CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Verschaffel, L., Greer, B., and De Corte, E. (2000). Making sense of word problems. Educ. Stud. Math. 42, 211–213.
Google Scholar
Wilkins, J. L., Baroody, A. J., and Tiilikainen, S. (2001). Kindergartners’ understanding of additive commutativity within the context of word problems. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 79, 23–36. doi: 10.1006/jecp.2000.2580
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Wood, G., Nuerk, H.-C., Moeller, K., Geppert, B., Schnitker, R., Weber, J., et al. (2008). All for one, but not one for all. How multiple number representations are recruited in one numerical task. Brain Res. 1187, 154–166. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.09.094
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Yeap, B. H., Ho, S. Y., Kaur, B., and Lee, N. H. (2005). Children making sense during word problem solving. Papers from the International Conference “Redesigning Pedagogy: Research, policy and practice,” Singapore. Available at: http://math.nie.edu.sg/T3/downloads/rprpp.pdf (accessed March 2015).
Google Scholar
Yeap, B. H., and Kaur, B. (2001). “Semantic characteristics that make arithmetic word problems difficult,” in Proceedings of the 24th Annual Conference of the Mathematics Education Research Group of Australasia Incorporated (MERGA) on “Numeracy and Beyond,” Australia.
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | Google Scholar
Yee, F. P., and Lee, K. P. (1997). School word problems and stereotyped thinking. Teach. Learn. 18, 73–82.
Google Scholar
Zarnhofer, S., Braunstein, V., Ebner, F., Koschutnig, K., Neuper, C., Ninaus, M., et al. (2013). Individual differences in solving arithmetic word problems. Behav. Brain Funct. 9, 28. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-9-28
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Zuber, J., Pixner, S., Moeller, K., and Nuerk, H.-C. (2009). On the language-specificity of basic number processing: transcoding in a language with inversion and its relation to working memory capacity. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 102, 60–77. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2008.04.003
PubMed Abstract | Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar
Word Problems: Word Problems are an important aspect of the primary curriculum because they require students to apply their understanding of various topics to “real-life” circumstances. They also assist students in becoming acquainted with mathematical terminology like more, fewer, subtract, difference, altogether, equal, share, multiply, reduced, etc.
A word problem consists of a description of a real-life scenario where a mathematical calculation is required to solve a problem. Students need to learn how to solve word problems as it enables them to apply mathematical concepts to various real-life scenarios. This means that to understand the word problem, students must be familiar with the terminology linked with the mathematical symbols they are used to. Let us learn in detail about Word Problems in this article.
What is a Word Problem?
In real life, mathematical problems do not usually present themselves as (2+3) or (6-4.) Instead, they are a bit more complex than we think. Authors of mathematical curriculum sometimes implement word problems to help students understand how they are related to the real world.
Learn 10th CBSE Exam Concepts
Word problems often show mathematics happening more naturally in the real world. Word problems can vary from simple to complex.
Examples:
Here are a few examples to get you some ideas:
1. Keerthi had (5) apples. Her mother gave her (7) more apples. Now, how many apples does Keerthi have altogether?
2. There were (18) pencils and (9) pens. How many more pencils than pens are there?
3. Spurti has one dozen eggs. Her friends ate (4) for breakfast. Now, how many eggs are left with Spurti?
4. There are (18) apples. Swetha, Priya, and Rachana want to eat them in equal share. How many apples will each of the friends get?
As you can notice, word problems involve addition, subtraction, multiplication, division, or even multiple operations.
Word Problems on Quadratic Equation
Handling Maths Word Problems in General
Steps for Solving a Word Problem:
To work out any word problem, follow the steps given below:
1. Read the Problem: First, read through the problem once.
2. Highlight Facts: Then, read through the problem again and underline or highlight important facts such as numbers or words that indicate an operation.
3. Draw a Picture: Drawing a picture sometimes help visualise the problem more clearly. It can also help understand clearly the algebraic operations that need to be carried out.
4. Determine the Operation(s): Next, determine the operation or operations that need to be performed. Is it addition, subtraction, multiplication, division? What needs to be done? Drawing the picture should be a big help in figuring this out. However, look for clues in words such as:
(i) Addition: add, total, brought, plus, altogether, and, combine, more, in all
(ii) Subtraction: subtract, fewer, take away, than, left
(iii) Multiplication: total, times, triple, twice, in all
(iv) Division: each, per, out of, equal pieces, split, average
Practice 10th CBSE Exam Questions
5. Make a Math Sentence: Next, translate the word problem and drawings into a math or number sentence. This means write a sentence such as (18+3=)
6. Solve the Problem: Then, solve the number sentence and find the solution—for example, (3+8=11.)
7. Check Your Answer: Finally, check the result obtained to ensure the correct answer.
With the above (7) steps, solving word problems in mathematics becomes easy.
Addition Word Problems
Addition word problems arise in situations where there is an increase of something or gain as a result of combining one or more numbers. Think of addition as combining parts to form a whole.
Example: Teena has (6) chocolates, her brother gives her (4) chocolates. How many chocolates in total does Teena have?
To find the total chocolates, we add the number of chocolates Teena has and the number of chocolates her brother gave her.
Thus, Teena has (10) chocolates now.
Attempt 10th CBSE Exam Mock Tests
Subtraction Word Problems
Subtraction word problems arise in situations where there is a decrease of something or loss as a result of deducting a number from another.
Example: Arya plucked (10) cherries from the tree, and her friend asked her to give (4) cherries to her. How many cherries are left with Arya?
Here we subtract the number of cherries given to Arya’s friend from the total cherries plucked by Arya.
Thus, the number of cherries left with Arya is (6.)
Multiplication Word Problems
Multiplication word problems are helpful for students to develop their multiplication knowledge by applying it to real-life situations. In this kind of multiplication word problem, one quantity is compared with another quantity, larger or smaller. Work out the multiplication problem using the clues provided.
Example: Ajay has two bunch of tomatoes. Each bunch has (4) tomatoes. What is the total number of tomatoes does Ajay have?
Here, we multiply the number of the bunch by the number of tomatoes in each bunch
Thus, Ajay has (8) tomatoes in total.
Division Word Problems
Division word problems are more confusing for students to understand. The words generally used in division word problems are “shared among” or “given to each” or similar phrases that imply total quantity to be split evenly into groups. We use division or multiplication when the problem involves equal parts of a whole.
Example: He divides (12) apples evenly among (4) friends. How many apples did John give to each of his friends?
To find how many apples John gave to each of his friends, we divide the total number of apples by the number of friends.
Hence, each of John’s friends gets (3) apples.
Fraction Word Problems
Fraction word problems look more complex, but in reality, fraction word problems are just as easy as those involving whole numbers. Here, one extra step of simplification may be needed in some cases. Students should know the operations with fractions to solve word problems on fractions.
Example: Swaroop had half an apple, and his mother gave him another quarter of an apple. What portion of apple does Swaroop have?
The given example is the addition word problem that involves fractions.
Here, we add the portion of apple Swaroop initially had with the portion his mother gives. i.e., (frac{1}{2} + frac{1}{4} = frac{{2 + 1}}{4} = frac{3}{4}.)
Thus, Swaroop has (frac{3}{4})th of apple.
Solved Examples
Q.1. The male population of a village is (76138,) and the female population is (13776.) What is the total population of the village?
Ans: Male population (=76138)
Female population (=13776)
Total population of the village (=) male population (+) female population (=76138+13776 )
(=89914 )
Thus, the total population of the village is (89914.)
Q.2. Likith had collected (208) coins. He lost of (52) coins. How many coins are left with Likith?
Ans: Total coins Likith collected (=208)
The number of coins lost (=52)
Number of coins left (=208 – 52=156)
Thus, the number of coins left is (156.)
Q.3. The cost of one shirt is (₹206.) A retailer wants to buy (68) such shirts. How much should he pay?
Ans: Given that the cost of one shirt (=₹206)
Number of shirts a retailer wants to buy (=68)
Total amount to be paid (=₹206×68)
Therefore, the total amount to be paid is (₹14008.)
Q.4 If (4472,{rm{kg}}) of rice is packed in (52) bags, how much rice will each bag contain?
Ans: Since (52) bags contain rice of (4472,{rm{kg}})
Therefore, (1) bag contains rice ( = left( {frac{{4472}}{{52}}} right){rm{kg}})
( = 86,{rm{kg}})
Thus, each bag of rice contains ( = 86,{rm{kg}}.)
Q.5. One-half of the students in a school are boys, (frac{4}{5}) of these boys are studying in lower classes. What fraction of boys are studying in lower classes?
Ans: Fraction of boys studying in school ( = frac{1}{2})
Fraction of boys studying in lower classes ( = frac{4}{5}) of (frac{1}{2})
( = frac{4}{5} times frac{1}{2})
( = frac{2}{5})
Therefore, (frac{2}{5}) of boys studying in lower classes.
Q.6. Celena planned to interview some candidates for a position in her office. If she scheduled half an hour to meet each of them, how much time did she schedule for all (6) candidates?
Ans: Time spent to meet each candidate ( = frac{1}{2}) hour
Number of applicants (=6)
Time scheduled for all (6) candidates ( = frac{1}{2} times 6 = 3) hours
Thus, Celena scheduled (3) hours for the interview.
Summary
In this article, we studied the meaning and steps to solve word problems. Then we have discussed in detail taking an example of how to do the addition word problem, subtraction, multiplication, division and fraction word problem.
We have solved word problem examples for students to understand the concept clearly.
Learn the Concepts on Unitary Method
Frequently Asked Questions on Word Problems
We have provided some frequently asked questions on Word Problems here:
Q.1. What are word problems in maths?
Ans: A word problem is a few sentences describing a real-life scenario where a problem needs to be solved by a mathematical calculation.
Q.2. What are numberless word problems?
Ans: Numberless word problems are designed to provide scaffolding that allows students to understand the underlying structure of word problems.
Q.3. What is addition word problems?
Ans: Addition word problems appear when there is a gain or an increase of something due to combining one or more numbers. Think of addition as combining parts to form a whole.
Q.4. What are the steps in solving word problems?
Ans: Solving word problem requires the following steps:
1. Read the problem
2. Highlight facts
3. Draw a picture
4. Determine the operation/s.
(i) Addition
(ii) Subtraction
(iii) Multiplication
(iv) Division
5. Make a mathematical sentence
6. Solve the problem
7. Verify your answer
Q.5. What is an example of a word problem?
Ans: The typical examples of word problems in algebra are distance problems, age problems, percentage problems, work problems, mixtures problems and number problems.
Now you are provided with all the necessary information on Maths word problems and we hope this detailed article is helpful to you. If you have any queries regarding this article, please ping us through the comment section below and we will get back to you as soon as possible.